对社会群体的两分法思考:了解一个群体可以激活对另一个群体的相反信念

IF 3 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Cognitive Psychology Pub Date : 2021-09-01 DOI:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101408
Hannah J. Kramer , Deborah Goldfarb , Sarah M. Tashjian , Kristin Hansen Lagattuta
{"title":"对社会群体的两分法思考:了解一个群体可以激活对另一个群体的相反信念","authors":"Hannah J. Kramer ,&nbsp;Deborah Goldfarb ,&nbsp;Sarah M. Tashjian ,&nbsp;Kristin Hansen Lagattuta","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101408","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Across three studies (<em>N</em> = 607), we examined people’s use of a <em>dichotomizing heuristic</em>—the inference that characteristics belonging to one group do not apply to another group—when making judgments about novel social groups. Participants learned information about one group (e.g., “Zuttles like apples”), and then made inferences about another group (e.g., “Do Twiggums like apples or hate apples?”). Study 1 acted as a proof of concept: Eight-year-olds and adults (but not 5-year-olds) assumed that the two groups would have opposite characteristics. Learning about the group as a generic whole versus as specific individuals boosted the use of the heuristic. Study 2 and Study 3 (sample sizes, methods, and analyses pre-registered), examined whether the presence or absence of several factors affected the activation and scope of the dichotomizing heuristic in adults. Whereas learning about or treating the groups as separate was necessary for activating dichotomous thinking, intergroup conflict and featuring only two (versus many) groups was not required. Moreover, the heuristic occurred when participants made both binary and scaled decisions. Once triggered, adults applied this cognitive shortcut widely—not only to benign (e.g., liking apples) and novel characteristics (e.g., liking modies), but also to evaluative traits signaling the morals or virtues of a social group (e.g., meanness or intelligence). Adults did not, however, extend the heuristic to the edges of improbability: They failed to dichotomize when doing so would attribute highly unusual preferences (e.g., disliking having fun). Taken together, these studies indicate the presence of a dichotomizing heuristic with broad implications for how people make social group inferences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"129 ","pages":"Article 101408"},"PeriodicalIF":3.0000,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101408","citationCount":"8","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Dichotomous thinking about social groups: Learning about one group can activate opposite beliefs about another group\",\"authors\":\"Hannah J. Kramer ,&nbsp;Deborah Goldfarb ,&nbsp;Sarah M. Tashjian ,&nbsp;Kristin Hansen Lagattuta\",\"doi\":\"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101408\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<div><p>Across three studies (<em>N</em> = 607), we examined people’s use of a <em>dichotomizing heuristic</em>—the inference that characteristics belonging to one group do not apply to another group—when making judgments about novel social groups. Participants learned information about one group (e.g., “Zuttles like apples”), and then made inferences about another group (e.g., “Do Twiggums like apples or hate apples?”). Study 1 acted as a proof of concept: Eight-year-olds and adults (but not 5-year-olds) assumed that the two groups would have opposite characteristics. Learning about the group as a generic whole versus as specific individuals boosted the use of the heuristic. Study 2 and Study 3 (sample sizes, methods, and analyses pre-registered), examined whether the presence or absence of several factors affected the activation and scope of the dichotomizing heuristic in adults. Whereas learning about or treating the groups as separate was necessary for activating dichotomous thinking, intergroup conflict and featuring only two (versus many) groups was not required. Moreover, the heuristic occurred when participants made both binary and scaled decisions. Once triggered, adults applied this cognitive shortcut widely—not only to benign (e.g., liking apples) and novel characteristics (e.g., liking modies), but also to evaluative traits signaling the morals or virtues of a social group (e.g., meanness or intelligence). Adults did not, however, extend the heuristic to the edges of improbability: They failed to dichotomize when doing so would attribute highly unusual preferences (e.g., disliking having fun). Taken together, these studies indicate the presence of a dichotomizing heuristic with broad implications for how people make social group inferences.</p></div>\",\"PeriodicalId\":50669,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Cognitive Psychology\",\"volume\":\"129 \",\"pages\":\"Article 101408\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":3.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2021-09-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2021.101408\",\"citationCount\":\"8\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Cognitive Psychology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"102\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028521000323\",\"RegionNum\":2,\"RegionCategory\":\"心理学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"Q1\",\"JCRName\":\"PSYCHOLOGY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognitive Psychology","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010028521000323","RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8

摘要

在三个研究中(N = 607),我们检查了人们在对新的社会群体做出判断时使用的二分类启发式——推断属于一个群体的特征不适用于另一个群体。参与者先了解一组的信息(例如,“Zuttles喜欢苹果”),然后对另一组进行推断(例如,“twiggum喜欢苹果还是讨厌苹果?”)。研究1作为一个概念的证明:8岁的孩子和成年人(但不是5岁的孩子)认为这两组人会有相反的特征。将群体作为一个整体来了解,而不是作为特定的个体来了解,提高了启发式的使用。研究2和研究3(样本量、方法和预先注册的分析)检验了几个因素的存在或不存在是否会影响成人二分启发式的激活和范围。然而,对于激活二分法思维来说,学习或对待不同的群体是必要的,而群体间的冲突和只有两个(而不是许多)群体是不必要的。此外,当参与者同时做出二进制和缩放决策时,启发式也会发生。一旦被触发,成年人就会广泛地应用这种认知捷径——不仅是良性的(例如,喜欢苹果)和新奇的特征(例如,喜欢moddies),而且还适用于表明一个社会群体的道德或美德的评价特征(例如,卑鄙或智力)。然而,成年人并没有将这种启发式扩展到非概率的边缘:当这样做会归因于非常不寻常的偏好(例如,不喜欢玩)时,他们无法进行二分类。综上所述,这些研究表明,二分类启发式的存在对人们如何进行社会群体推理具有广泛的影响。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Dichotomous thinking about social groups: Learning about one group can activate opposite beliefs about another group

Across three studies (N = 607), we examined people’s use of a dichotomizing heuristic—the inference that characteristics belonging to one group do not apply to another group—when making judgments about novel social groups. Participants learned information about one group (e.g., “Zuttles like apples”), and then made inferences about another group (e.g., “Do Twiggums like apples or hate apples?”). Study 1 acted as a proof of concept: Eight-year-olds and adults (but not 5-year-olds) assumed that the two groups would have opposite characteristics. Learning about the group as a generic whole versus as specific individuals boosted the use of the heuristic. Study 2 and Study 3 (sample sizes, methods, and analyses pre-registered), examined whether the presence or absence of several factors affected the activation and scope of the dichotomizing heuristic in adults. Whereas learning about or treating the groups as separate was necessary for activating dichotomous thinking, intergroup conflict and featuring only two (versus many) groups was not required. Moreover, the heuristic occurred when participants made both binary and scaled decisions. Once triggered, adults applied this cognitive shortcut widely—not only to benign (e.g., liking apples) and novel characteristics (e.g., liking modies), but also to evaluative traits signaling the morals or virtues of a social group (e.g., meanness or intelligence). Adults did not, however, extend the heuristic to the edges of improbability: They failed to dichotomize when doing so would attribute highly unusual preferences (e.g., disliking having fun). Taken together, these studies indicate the presence of a dichotomizing heuristic with broad implications for how people make social group inferences.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
Cognitive Psychology
Cognitive Psychology 医学-心理学
CiteScore
5.40
自引率
3.80%
发文量
29
审稿时长
50 days
期刊介绍: Cognitive Psychology is concerned with advances in the study of attention, memory, language processing, perception, problem solving, and thinking. Cognitive Psychology specializes in extensive articles that have a major impact on cognitive theory and provide new theoretical advances. Research Areas include: • Artificial intelligence • Developmental psychology • Linguistics • Neurophysiology • Social psychology.
期刊最新文献
Optimal allocation of time in risky choices under opportunity costs. In the United States, children are more likely than adults to condone discrimination. Updating of information in working memory: Time course and consequences. Exploring the bounded rationality in human decision anomalies through an assemblable computational framework. A position coding model that accounts for the effects of event boundaries on temporal order memory.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1