成人多动症的流行病学,以及在多学科团队科学中对多动症的终身原因和后果进行前瞻性研究的呼吁。

JCPP advances Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI:10.1002/jcv2.12178
Catharina A. Hartman
{"title":"成人多动症的流行病学,以及在多学科团队科学中对多动症的终身原因和后果进行前瞻性研究的呼吁。","authors":"Catharina A. Hartman","doi":"10.1002/jcv2.12178","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) used to be studied at age of diagnosis–the typical time that professional help is sought and children get an ADHD diagnosis was also the time that they typically enrolled in scientific studies. As a consequence, most of our knowledge is about referred children with ADHD in middle childhood in the age range of 6–12 years. One implication of this is that for a long time ADHD research was not bothered so much by studying causes of ADHD <i>after</i> its onset—in other words, within the total of studies aimed at understanding the causes of ADHD, prospective studies in ADHD research have been rare.</p><p>The first paper that I will highlight in this editorial is by Miller et al. (<span>2023</span>) who discuss the development of ADHD in the period from conception to age of onset. They stress that for understanding the causes of ADHD prospective research prior to its onset is needed and explain that any differences observed in children with ADHD (i.e., after its onset) compared to children without ADHD may be secondary to personal and environmental alterations that are evoked by the ADHD symptoms themselves. At the same time, they point out that an onset of ADHD is gradual and the distinction between pre-onset and post-onset not clear-cut: precursor behaviors of ADHD may already evoke personal and environmental alterations. Thus, for a causal understanding of ADHD, we need to know these as well. Prospective research from conception to full clinical onset allows us to chart such alterations and their temporal sequence up to a full onset of ADHD.</p><p>The value of establishing the temporal sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading to a clinical onset of a disease cannot be underestimated. It is one element (albeit by no means a sufficient element) in establishing the causality of a risk factor (the risk factor should occur prior to the onset of the disease), and a very important one in observational research. The accumulation of knowledge as to whether a risk factor is (likely) causal is necessary if we want to target the risk factor for interventions: that is, only causal risk factors can actually influence the outcome. Prospective research charting the sequences of personal and environmental alterations in risk during the gradual unfolding of ADHD over time provides opportunities to identify mediators that are useful as intervention targets in particular developmental periods. Also, prospective research may identify protective factors, by comparing children who have the same risk profile (e.g., at conception, at birth) but one group progressing to a full clinical onset of ADHD yet another not. That is, if we only study those who already have an onset of ADHD like we used to do we can never know if onset of ADHD (particularly the impairments experienced by the children) can be prevented, postponed or reduced in severity and thus if and how a developmental trajectory heading toward onset of ADHD can be shifted toward a more favorable outcome.</p><p>The paper by Miller et al. in this issue of JCPP Advances describes a research agenda for prospective research pre-onset to ADHD. The authors have formed the Early ADHD Consortium to improve future prospective research on the causes of ADHD. An important factor herein in my view is sample size. Clearly, we need the statistical power coming from large samples to get robust insights into the causes of ADHD: ADHD is a multifactorial condition resulting from the collective influence of multiple personal and environmental risk factors, each contributing a minor part but working in unison to heighten ADHD susceptibility. With none of the risk factors being necessary or sufficient, pathways toward onset of ADHD diverge among children and the to-be-expected small effects of any risk factor manifest as even smaller effects when averaged across all children in the study sample. This reasoning on why large samples are needed holds all the more if we are to identify the interplay (interactions) among risk factors. Furthermore, Miller et al. point out that neurodevelopmental alterations likely emerge prior to overt behavioral changes and that we need to understand causal factors at additional levels of understanding (e.g., metabolic or neural levels) next to the overt behavioral level, emphasizing once more the need for large samples. Team science in consortia to enlarge the sample turned out to be the game changer in the field of genetics, and this should also work out to improve our understanding of the broader (potentially interacting) causes of ADHD. JCPP <i>Advances</i> is therefore proud to publish the “Delineating early developmental pathways to ADHD: Setting an international research agenda” from the Early ADHD Consortium.</p><p>The second paper that I will highlight by Li et al. is also published in JCPP Advances (<span>2023</span>) and focuses on a different phase in the life course of individuals with ADHD. Li et al. are a group of authors who come from another recently formed ADHD consortium “TIMESPAN,” which zooms in on cardiometabolic diseases which in most cases have an onset (long) after the onset of ADHD. The meta-analysis by Li et al. is on cardiovascular diseases (CVD) and shows that adults with ADHD are nearly twice as likely to develop CVD than adults without ADHD. An important asset of the paper is that the authors embedded this finding in the broader available knowledge on the link between neuropsychiatric disorders and CVD, showing that the two-fold risk in relation to CVD is similar to estimates for schizophrenia and substance use disorder but larger than estimates for mood, anxiety and stress related disorders. This group from TIMESPAN has just published a similar meta-analysis on ADHD and type 2 diabetes (Garcia-Argibay et al., <span>2023</span>), and a strong empirical study on ADHD and CVD (Li et al., <span>2022</span>), and together with the evidence from the longer-standing research tradition on comorbidity of ADHD with obesity (Cortese et al., <span>2016</span>; Nigg et al., <span>2016</span>), we can conclude that the link between ADHD and cardiometabolic diseases has now been well-established. Together, these papers including the one by Li et al. in JCPP Advances discuss steps that need to be taken for a mechanistic understanding of the causes of an onset of comorbid cardiometabolic diseases in adults with ADHD as well as the consequences (particularly the consequences are studied in the TIMESPAN consortium). Like causes of ADHD need to be established pre-onset to ADHD, prospective research on the sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading up to a comorbid condition need to be established pre-onset of a comorbid condition. This, with the same complication as in ADHD that onset of a comorbid condition is gradual and does not only include overt alterations in personal risk factors but also, for example, metabolic or neural alterations. One important reason to form the TIMESPAN consortium was, like it likely was for the Early ADHD Consortium, the need for a large sample size to do prospective research on multifactorial conditions (in this case both ADHD and the different cardiometabolic diseases are multifactorial).</p><p>As argued, the ideas put forward by Miller et al. are not only relevant pre-onset to ADHD but throughout the lifespan. To illustrate this further, I want to highlight two aspects specifically. First, as pointed out already, Miller et al. emphasize that environmental risk factors of ADHD in early development may change over time. As an example of altering risk factors over development, maternal immune activation, during pregnancy, may contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (He et al., <span>2022</span>), while parental difficulties to regulate their emotions, subsequently during toddlerhood, may at that point in time contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (Claussen et al., <span>2022</span>). Note that these findings, apart from that they need further study, have never been studied prospectively in the same children to establish potential accumulation of risk. We may extend the idea that risk factors change across the lifespan from the causes of ADHD to the course of ADHD. For example, with regard to the development of comorbid obesity, impulsive reward driven eating of high caloric, low nutrient, foods leading to overweight may be particularly prevalent in adolescents with ADHD, while lack of physical activity associated with difficulties with motivation and organization in ADHD may contribute to weight gain particularly in adults (note that this is a fictitious example, changes in risk factors contributing to weight gain in individuals with ADHD relative to individuals without ADHD in different developmental periods have not yet been studied). These examples on changing risk factors across the lifespan illustrate how much prospective work, both pre-onset to or post-onset of ADHD, is still to be done.</p><p>A second example of how ideas put forward by Miller et al. on causes of ADHD can be extended across the lifespan relates to the specificity or non-specificity of personal and environmental risk factors of ADHD (non-specificity meaning that they also play role in the onset of other conditions and not just in the onset of ADHD). Miller et al. explain that how an exclusive focus on onset of ADHD and the presumption of specificity of risk for this outcome may obscure transdiagnostic patterns and lead to inaccurate causal models. That non-specificity is ubiquitous and also relevant in the course of ADHD is undisputed. I already cited the example reported by Li et al. on the association of ADHD with CVD amidst similar associations of schizophrenia, substance use disorder, mood, anxiety and stress related disorders with CVD. Similarly, the most recent Genomewide Association Study on ADHD showed high sharing of genetic (concordant and discordant) genetic variants of ADHD with other disorders showing even more than before their strong intertwinement of the genetic architecture (Demontis et al., <span>2023</span>). Specifically with regard to the onset of comorbid conditions during the course of ADHD, we may first of all draw from known personal and environmental risks of these conditions. The pertinent question with regard to specificity in this context is if these known risk factors have a similar effect whether ADHD is present or not (i.e., an interaction effect). For example, the effect may be stronger in individuals with ADHD compared to individuals without ADHD as in the previous example on how weight gain may be specifically linked to ADHD characteristics in different developmental periods (impulsivity and reward drivenness; motivational and organizational difficulties). In addition, prospective research may potentially show that timing may be different: known risk factors and precursors of onset of comorbidity may exert their influence earlier in individuals with than without ADHD. For example, there is some evidence that the onset of a condition is earlier in persons with than without ADHD (e.g., substance abuse: Dunne et al., <span>2014</span>; CVD, Li et al., <span>2022</span>). In addition, there may be risk factors that are specific to individuals with ADHD that lead up to the comorbid condition and which we currently do not know. Close monitoring in prospective research, as advocated by Miller et al., but now applied to the unfolding of the course of ADHD, specifically onset of comorbid conditions, will help establish this knowledge. This second example on the specificity of personal and environmental risk factors shows that broad knowledge beyond just ADHD is required. For example, in the example of Timespan, both specialists on ADHD and on cardiometabolic diseases take part. Again, if extended to other levels of understanding (metabolomic, neural, etc.), in addition to the overt personal risk factors, this holds even more. Thus, not only large sample sizes but also broad knowledge to do the best possible research can be accomplished by multidisciplinary team science.</p><p>In this editorial, I argued that making the distinctions between the causes of ADHD and the course of ADHD and between the causes of onset of comorbidity and the course thereafter are essential. At the same time, the gradual onset of ADHD and any comorbid condition makes clear that there is no hard cut between causes and consequences. Yet through close monitoring over time, we will be able to establish the cascades of personal and environmental alterations leading to the onset of ADHD and to the onset of comorbid conditions. In my ideal world, we would do prospective mechanistic research on ADHD (or any other condition) from pregnancy to death, but this is obviously not going to happen. Yet, papers from multidisciplinary research teams working together on different parts of the lifespan, like Miller et al. in the Early ADHD Consortium, and Li et al. in the Timespan consortium, show that we are making good progress in understanding the ADHD lifespan.</p><p><b>Catharina A. Hartman</b>: Conceptualization; writing–original draft; writing–review and editing.</p><p>Catharina A. Hartman <i>is part of the TIMESPAN consortium, but not a co-author on the cited papers.</i> She is also Deputy Editor-in-Chief for JCPP <i>Advances</i>.</p>","PeriodicalId":73542,"journal":{"name":"JCPP advances","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://acamh.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcv2.12178","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Epidemiology of ADHD coming of age and a plea for prospective research on causes and consequences of ADHD throughout the lifespan in multidisciplinary team science\",\"authors\":\"Catharina A. Hartman\",\"doi\":\"10.1002/jcv2.12178\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p>Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) used to be studied at age of diagnosis–the typical time that professional help is sought and children get an ADHD diagnosis was also the time that they typically enrolled in scientific studies. As a consequence, most of our knowledge is about referred children with ADHD in middle childhood in the age range of 6–12 years. One implication of this is that for a long time ADHD research was not bothered so much by studying causes of ADHD <i>after</i> its onset—in other words, within the total of studies aimed at understanding the causes of ADHD, prospective studies in ADHD research have been rare.</p><p>The first paper that I will highlight in this editorial is by Miller et al. (<span>2023</span>) who discuss the development of ADHD in the period from conception to age of onset. They stress that for understanding the causes of ADHD prospective research prior to its onset is needed and explain that any differences observed in children with ADHD (i.e., after its onset) compared to children without ADHD may be secondary to personal and environmental alterations that are evoked by the ADHD symptoms themselves. At the same time, they point out that an onset of ADHD is gradual and the distinction between pre-onset and post-onset not clear-cut: precursor behaviors of ADHD may already evoke personal and environmental alterations. Thus, for a causal understanding of ADHD, we need to know these as well. Prospective research from conception to full clinical onset allows us to chart such alterations and their temporal sequence up to a full onset of ADHD.</p><p>The value of establishing the temporal sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading to a clinical onset of a disease cannot be underestimated. It is one element (albeit by no means a sufficient element) in establishing the causality of a risk factor (the risk factor should occur prior to the onset of the disease), and a very important one in observational research. The accumulation of knowledge as to whether a risk factor is (likely) causal is necessary if we want to target the risk factor for interventions: that is, only causal risk factors can actually influence the outcome. Prospective research charting the sequences of personal and environmental alterations in risk during the gradual unfolding of ADHD over time provides opportunities to identify mediators that are useful as intervention targets in particular developmental periods. Also, prospective research may identify protective factors, by comparing children who have the same risk profile (e.g., at conception, at birth) but one group progressing to a full clinical onset of ADHD yet another not. That is, if we only study those who already have an onset of ADHD like we used to do we can never know if onset of ADHD (particularly the impairments experienced by the children) can be prevented, postponed or reduced in severity and thus if and how a developmental trajectory heading toward onset of ADHD can be shifted toward a more favorable outcome.</p><p>The paper by Miller et al. in this issue of JCPP Advances describes a research agenda for prospective research pre-onset to ADHD. The authors have formed the Early ADHD Consortium to improve future prospective research on the causes of ADHD. An important factor herein in my view is sample size. Clearly, we need the statistical power coming from large samples to get robust insights into the causes of ADHD: ADHD is a multifactorial condition resulting from the collective influence of multiple personal and environmental risk factors, each contributing a minor part but working in unison to heighten ADHD susceptibility. With none of the risk factors being necessary or sufficient, pathways toward onset of ADHD diverge among children and the to-be-expected small effects of any risk factor manifest as even smaller effects when averaged across all children in the study sample. This reasoning on why large samples are needed holds all the more if we are to identify the interplay (interactions) among risk factors. Furthermore, Miller et al. point out that neurodevelopmental alterations likely emerge prior to overt behavioral changes and that we need to understand causal factors at additional levels of understanding (e.g., metabolic or neural levels) next to the overt behavioral level, emphasizing once more the need for large samples. Team science in consortia to enlarge the sample turned out to be the game changer in the field of genetics, and this should also work out to improve our understanding of the broader (potentially interacting) causes of ADHD. JCPP <i>Advances</i> is therefore proud to publish the “Delineating early developmental pathways to ADHD: Setting an international research agenda” from the Early ADHD Consortium.</p><p>The second paper that I will highlight by Li et al. is also published in JCPP Advances (<span>2023</span>) and focuses on a different phase in the life course of individuals with ADHD. Li et al. are a group of authors who come from another recently formed ADHD consortium “TIMESPAN,” which zooms in on cardiometabolic diseases which in most cases have an onset (long) after the onset of ADHD. The meta-analysis by Li et al. is on cardiovascular diseases (CVD) and shows that adults with ADHD are nearly twice as likely to develop CVD than adults without ADHD. An important asset of the paper is that the authors embedded this finding in the broader available knowledge on the link between neuropsychiatric disorders and CVD, showing that the two-fold risk in relation to CVD is similar to estimates for schizophrenia and substance use disorder but larger than estimates for mood, anxiety and stress related disorders. This group from TIMESPAN has just published a similar meta-analysis on ADHD and type 2 diabetes (Garcia-Argibay et al., <span>2023</span>), and a strong empirical study on ADHD and CVD (Li et al., <span>2022</span>), and together with the evidence from the longer-standing research tradition on comorbidity of ADHD with obesity (Cortese et al., <span>2016</span>; Nigg et al., <span>2016</span>), we can conclude that the link between ADHD and cardiometabolic diseases has now been well-established. Together, these papers including the one by Li et al. in JCPP Advances discuss steps that need to be taken for a mechanistic understanding of the causes of an onset of comorbid cardiometabolic diseases in adults with ADHD as well as the consequences (particularly the consequences are studied in the TIMESPAN consortium). Like causes of ADHD need to be established pre-onset to ADHD, prospective research on the sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading up to a comorbid condition need to be established pre-onset of a comorbid condition. This, with the same complication as in ADHD that onset of a comorbid condition is gradual and does not only include overt alterations in personal risk factors but also, for example, metabolic or neural alterations. One important reason to form the TIMESPAN consortium was, like it likely was for the Early ADHD Consortium, the need for a large sample size to do prospective research on multifactorial conditions (in this case both ADHD and the different cardiometabolic diseases are multifactorial).</p><p>As argued, the ideas put forward by Miller et al. are not only relevant pre-onset to ADHD but throughout the lifespan. To illustrate this further, I want to highlight two aspects specifically. First, as pointed out already, Miller et al. emphasize that environmental risk factors of ADHD in early development may change over time. As an example of altering risk factors over development, maternal immune activation, during pregnancy, may contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (He et al., <span>2022</span>), while parental difficulties to regulate their emotions, subsequently during toddlerhood, may at that point in time contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (Claussen et al., <span>2022</span>). Note that these findings, apart from that they need further study, have never been studied prospectively in the same children to establish potential accumulation of risk. We may extend the idea that risk factors change across the lifespan from the causes of ADHD to the course of ADHD. For example, with regard to the development of comorbid obesity, impulsive reward driven eating of high caloric, low nutrient, foods leading to overweight may be particularly prevalent in adolescents with ADHD, while lack of physical activity associated with difficulties with motivation and organization in ADHD may contribute to weight gain particularly in adults (note that this is a fictitious example, changes in risk factors contributing to weight gain in individuals with ADHD relative to individuals without ADHD in different developmental periods have not yet been studied). These examples on changing risk factors across the lifespan illustrate how much prospective work, both pre-onset to or post-onset of ADHD, is still to be done.</p><p>A second example of how ideas put forward by Miller et al. on causes of ADHD can be extended across the lifespan relates to the specificity or non-specificity of personal and environmental risk factors of ADHD (non-specificity meaning that they also play role in the onset of other conditions and not just in the onset of ADHD). Miller et al. explain that how an exclusive focus on onset of ADHD and the presumption of specificity of risk for this outcome may obscure transdiagnostic patterns and lead to inaccurate causal models. That non-specificity is ubiquitous and also relevant in the course of ADHD is undisputed. I already cited the example reported by Li et al. on the association of ADHD with CVD amidst similar associations of schizophrenia, substance use disorder, mood, anxiety and stress related disorders with CVD. Similarly, the most recent Genomewide Association Study on ADHD showed high sharing of genetic (concordant and discordant) genetic variants of ADHD with other disorders showing even more than before their strong intertwinement of the genetic architecture (Demontis et al., <span>2023</span>). Specifically with regard to the onset of comorbid conditions during the course of ADHD, we may first of all draw from known personal and environmental risks of these conditions. The pertinent question with regard to specificity in this context is if these known risk factors have a similar effect whether ADHD is present or not (i.e., an interaction effect). For example, the effect may be stronger in individuals with ADHD compared to individuals without ADHD as in the previous example on how weight gain may be specifically linked to ADHD characteristics in different developmental periods (impulsivity and reward drivenness; motivational and organizational difficulties). In addition, prospective research may potentially show that timing may be different: known risk factors and precursors of onset of comorbidity may exert their influence earlier in individuals with than without ADHD. For example, there is some evidence that the onset of a condition is earlier in persons with than without ADHD (e.g., substance abuse: Dunne et al., <span>2014</span>; CVD, Li et al., <span>2022</span>). In addition, there may be risk factors that are specific to individuals with ADHD that lead up to the comorbid condition and which we currently do not know. Close monitoring in prospective research, as advocated by Miller et al., but now applied to the unfolding of the course of ADHD, specifically onset of comorbid conditions, will help establish this knowledge. This second example on the specificity of personal and environmental risk factors shows that broad knowledge beyond just ADHD is required. For example, in the example of Timespan, both specialists on ADHD and on cardiometabolic diseases take part. Again, if extended to other levels of understanding (metabolomic, neural, etc.), in addition to the overt personal risk factors, this holds even more. Thus, not only large sample sizes but also broad knowledge to do the best possible research can be accomplished by multidisciplinary team science.</p><p>In this editorial, I argued that making the distinctions between the causes of ADHD and the course of ADHD and between the causes of onset of comorbidity and the course thereafter are essential. At the same time, the gradual onset of ADHD and any comorbid condition makes clear that there is no hard cut between causes and consequences. Yet through close monitoring over time, we will be able to establish the cascades of personal and environmental alterations leading to the onset of ADHD and to the onset of comorbid conditions. In my ideal world, we would do prospective mechanistic research on ADHD (or any other condition) from pregnancy to death, but this is obviously not going to happen. Yet, papers from multidisciplinary research teams working together on different parts of the lifespan, like Miller et al. in the Early ADHD Consortium, and Li et al. in the Timespan consortium, show that we are making good progress in understanding the ADHD lifespan.</p><p><b>Catharina A. Hartman</b>: Conceptualization; writing–original draft; writing–review and editing.</p><p>Catharina A. Hartman <i>is part of the TIMESPAN consortium, but not a co-author on the cited papers.</i> She is also Deputy Editor-in-Chief for JCPP <i>Advances</i>.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":73542,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"JCPP advances\",\"volume\":null,\"pages\":null},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2023-06-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://acamh.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcv2.12178\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"JCPP advances\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jcv2.12178\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"JCPP advances","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jcv2.12178","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)过去是在诊断的年龄进行研究的——孩子们寻求专业帮助和得到ADHD诊断的典型时间,也是他们通常参加科学研究的时间。因此,我们的大部分知识都是关于在6-12岁的儿童中期患有多动症的儿童。这其中的一个含义是,在很长一段时间里,ADHD研究并没有那么多地关注ADHD发病后的病因——换句话说,在所有旨在了解ADHD病因的研究中,对ADHD研究的前瞻性研究很少。我将在这篇社论中强调的第一篇论文是Miller等人(2023)的论文,他们讨论了ADHD从受孕到发病的发展过程。他们强调,为了了解ADHD发病前的原因,需要进行前瞻性研究,并解释在ADHD儿童中观察到的任何差异(即发病后)与非ADHD儿童相比,可能是继发于ADHD症状本身引起的个人和环境改变。同时,他们指出多动症的发病是逐渐的,发病前和发病后的区别并不明确:多动症的前兆行为可能已经引起了个人和环境的改变。因此,为了理解ADHD的因果关系,我们也需要知道这些。从受孕到完全临床发作的前瞻性研究使我们能够绘制出这些变化及其时间序列,直到ADHD完全发作。建立导致疾病临床发作的个人和环境变化的时间序列的价值不容低估。它是确定风险因素因果关系的一个因素(尽管绝不是一个充分的因素)(风险因素应该在疾病发作之前发生),也是观察性研究中非常重要的一个因素。如果我们想要针对风险因素进行干预,那么关于风险因素是否(可能)具有因果关系的知识积累是必要的:也就是说,只有因果风险因素才能真正影响结果。前瞻性研究绘制了随着时间的推移,ADHD逐渐发展过程中个人和环境变化的风险序列,为确定在特定发育时期作为干预目标有用的中介提供了机会。此外,前瞻性研究可以通过比较具有相同风险概况(例如,在受孕时,在出生时)的儿童来确定保护因素,但一组发展为完全临床发病的ADHD,而另一组则没有。也就是说,如果我们只研究那些已经有多动症的人,就像我们过去做的那样,我们永远不知道多动症的发病(特别是儿童经历的损伤)是否可以预防,推迟或减轻严重程度,因此,是否以及如何将发展轨迹转向多动症的发病可以转向更有利的结果。Miller等人在本期《JCPP进展》上发表的论文描述了对ADHD发病前的前瞻性研究的研究议程。作者已经成立了早期多动症联盟,以改善对多动症病因的未来前瞻性研究。在我看来,这里的一个重要因素是样本量。显然,我们需要来自大样本的统计力量来深入了解ADHD的原因:ADHD是一种多因素的疾病,是由多种个人和环境风险因素的共同影响造成的,每个因素只占很小的一部分,但却共同提高了ADHD的易感性。由于没有一个风险因素是必要的或充分的,儿童之间的ADHD发病途径是不同的,当对研究样本中的所有儿童进行平均时,任何风险因素预期的小影响都表现出更小的影响。如果我们要确定风险因素之间的相互作用(相互作用),那么为什么需要大样本的推理就更加有效。此外,Miller等人指出,神经发育改变可能先于显性行为改变出现,我们需要在显性行为水平之外的其他理解水平(例如,代谢或神经水平)上理解因果因素,再次强调需要大样本。扩大样本的联合科学团队被证明是遗传学领域的游戏规则改变者,这也应该有助于提高我们对多动症更广泛(潜在的相互作用)原因的理解。因此,JCPP进展很自豪地发表了早期ADHD联盟的“描绘ADHD的早期发展途径:制定国际研究议程”。我将重点介绍的Li等人的第二篇论文也发表在JCPP Advances(2023)上,重点关注ADHD患者生命历程中的不同阶段。Li等人。 是一组作者,他们来自最近成立的另一个多动症联盟“TIMESPAN”,该联盟专注于心脏代谢疾病,这些疾病在大多数情况下都是在多动症发病后(很长时间)发病的。Li等人的荟萃分析是关于心血管疾病(CVD)的,显示患有ADHD的成年人患CVD的可能性几乎是没有ADHD的成年人的两倍。这篇论文的一个重要优点是,作者将这一发现嵌入到关于神经精神疾病和心血管疾病之间联系的更广泛的现有知识中,表明心血管疾病的双重风险与精神分裂症和物质使用障碍的估计相似,但比情绪、焦虑和压力相关疾病的估计要大。TIMESPAN的这个研究小组刚刚发表了一项类似的关于多动症和2型糖尿病的荟萃分析(Garcia-Argibay等人,2023),一项关于多动症和心血管疾病的强有力的实证研究(Li等人,2022),以及关于多动症与肥胖共病的长期研究传统的证据(Cortese等人,2016;Nigg et al., 2016),我们可以得出结论,ADHD和心脏代谢疾病之间的联系现在已经得到了证实。这些论文,包括Li等人在JCPP Advances上发表的论文,共同讨论了需要采取的步骤,以便从机制上理解ADHD成人共病心脏代谢疾病发病的原因及其后果(特别是TIMESPAN联盟研究的后果)。就像ADHD的病因需要在ADHD发病前确定一样,对导致共病的个人和环境改变顺序的前瞻性研究需要在共病发病前确定。这与多动症的并发症是一样的,共病的发作是逐渐的,不仅包括个人危险因素的明显改变,还包括代谢或神经的改变。组建TIMESPAN联盟的一个重要原因是,就像早期ADHD联盟一样,需要一个大样本来对多因素条件进行前瞻性研究(在这种情况下,ADHD和不同的心脏代谢疾病都是多因素的)。正如所争论的那样,Miller等人提出的观点不仅与ADHD发病前有关,而且与整个生命周期有关。为了进一步说明这一点,我想特别强调两个方面。首先,正如已经指出的,Miller等人强调ADHD早期发展的环境风险因素可能随着时间的推移而改变。作为在发育过程中改变风险因素的一个例子,怀孕期间母亲的免疫激活可能会导致ADHD晚发的风险(He et al., 2022),而父母在随后的幼儿期难以调节自己的情绪,可能会导致ADHD晚发的风险(Claussen et al., 2022)。需要注意的是,这些发现除了需要进一步研究外,从未在同一儿童中进行前瞻性研究,以确定潜在的风险积累。我们可以将风险因素在整个生命周期中的变化从ADHD的起因延伸到ADHD的过程。例如,关于并发肥胖的发展,冲动奖励驱动的高热量,低营养的食物导致超重可能在患有多动症的青少年中特别普遍,而缺乏与多动症的动机和组织困难相关的身体活动可能会导致体重增加,特别是在成年人中(注意,这是一个虚构的例子,在不同的发育时期,ADHD患者与非ADHD患者体重增加的危险因素的变化尚未得到研究)。这些关于在整个生命周期中改变风险因素的例子表明,在ADHD发病前或发病后,仍有大量的前瞻性工作要做。Miller等人提出的关于ADHD病因的观点可以延伸到整个生命周期的第二个例子与ADHD的个人和环境风险因素的特异性或非特异性有关(非特异性意味着它们也在其他疾病的发病中发挥作用,而不仅仅是在ADHD的发病中)。Miller等人解释了对ADHD发病的独家关注和对这种结果的特异性风险的假设如何模糊了跨诊断模式并导致不准确的因果模型。这种非特异性是普遍存在的,在ADHD过程中也是相关的,这是无可争议的。我已经引用了Li等人关于ADHD与CVD的关联的例子,以及精神分裂症、物质使用障碍、情绪、焦虑和压力相关疾病与CVD的类似关联。 同样,最近关于多动症的全基因组关联研究表明,多动症与其他疾病的遗传变异(一致和不一致)具有高度的共享性,甚至比以前更强烈地交织在一起(Demontis et al., 2023)。特别是关于ADHD过程中出现的合并症,我们首先可以从已知的这些疾病的个人和环境风险中得出结论。在这种情况下,关于特异性的相关问题是,这些已知的风险因素是否与多动症存在与否有相似的影响(即,相互作用效应)。例如,与没有多动症的人相比,患有多动症的人的影响可能更强,正如前面的例子所示,体重增加可能与不同发育时期的多动症特征(冲动性和奖励驱动;动机和组织困难)。此外,前瞻性研究可能潜在地表明,时间可能是不同的:已知的风险因素和共病发病的前兆可能在患有ADHD的个体中比没有ADHD的个体更早地发挥其影响。例如,有证据表明,患有多动症的人比没有多动症的人更早发病(例如,药物滥用:Dunne et al., 2014;CVD, Li等人,2022)。此外,可能有一些特定于多动症患者的风险因素导致了合并症,而我们目前还不知道这些因素。Miller等人提倡在前瞻性研究中密切监测,但现在应用于ADHD病程的展开,特别是合并症的发作,将有助于建立这一知识。关于个人和环境风险因素的特殊性的第二个例子表明,除了多动症之外,还需要广泛的知识。例如,在时间跨度的例子中,多动症和心脏代谢疾病的专家都参与了研究。同样,如果扩展到其他层面的理解(代谢组学,神经学等),除了明显的个人风险因素,这一点甚至更有意义。因此,通过多学科团队科学,不仅可以获得大量的样本量,而且还可以获得广泛的知识来进行尽可能好的研究。在这篇社论中,我认为区分ADHD的起因和过程,区分共病的起因和其后的过程是必要的。与此同时,多动症的逐渐发病和任何合并症都清楚地表明,在原因和后果之间没有严格的区分。然而,通过长期的密切监测,我们将能够确定导致ADHD发病和合并症发病的个人和环境变化的级联反应。在我的理想世界里,我们会对多动症(或任何其他情况)从怀孕到死亡进行前瞻性的机制研究,但这显然是不可能发生的。然而,来自多学科研究团队的论文共同研究了生命的不同部分,如Miller等人在早期ADHD联盟中,Li等人在时间跨度联盟中,表明我们在理解ADHD寿命方面取得了良好的进展。卡萨琳娜·哈特曼:概念化;原创作品草案;写作-审查和编辑。Catharina a . Hartman是TIMESPAN联盟的成员,但不是被引用论文的合著者。她也是JCPP Advances的副主编。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Epidemiology of ADHD coming of age and a plea for prospective research on causes and consequences of ADHD throughout the lifespan in multidisciplinary team science

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) used to be studied at age of diagnosis–the typical time that professional help is sought and children get an ADHD diagnosis was also the time that they typically enrolled in scientific studies. As a consequence, most of our knowledge is about referred children with ADHD in middle childhood in the age range of 6–12 years. One implication of this is that for a long time ADHD research was not bothered so much by studying causes of ADHD after its onset—in other words, within the total of studies aimed at understanding the causes of ADHD, prospective studies in ADHD research have been rare.

The first paper that I will highlight in this editorial is by Miller et al. (2023) who discuss the development of ADHD in the period from conception to age of onset. They stress that for understanding the causes of ADHD prospective research prior to its onset is needed and explain that any differences observed in children with ADHD (i.e., after its onset) compared to children without ADHD may be secondary to personal and environmental alterations that are evoked by the ADHD symptoms themselves. At the same time, they point out that an onset of ADHD is gradual and the distinction between pre-onset and post-onset not clear-cut: precursor behaviors of ADHD may already evoke personal and environmental alterations. Thus, for a causal understanding of ADHD, we need to know these as well. Prospective research from conception to full clinical onset allows us to chart such alterations and their temporal sequence up to a full onset of ADHD.

The value of establishing the temporal sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading to a clinical onset of a disease cannot be underestimated. It is one element (albeit by no means a sufficient element) in establishing the causality of a risk factor (the risk factor should occur prior to the onset of the disease), and a very important one in observational research. The accumulation of knowledge as to whether a risk factor is (likely) causal is necessary if we want to target the risk factor for interventions: that is, only causal risk factors can actually influence the outcome. Prospective research charting the sequences of personal and environmental alterations in risk during the gradual unfolding of ADHD over time provides opportunities to identify mediators that are useful as intervention targets in particular developmental periods. Also, prospective research may identify protective factors, by comparing children who have the same risk profile (e.g., at conception, at birth) but one group progressing to a full clinical onset of ADHD yet another not. That is, if we only study those who already have an onset of ADHD like we used to do we can never know if onset of ADHD (particularly the impairments experienced by the children) can be prevented, postponed or reduced in severity and thus if and how a developmental trajectory heading toward onset of ADHD can be shifted toward a more favorable outcome.

The paper by Miller et al. in this issue of JCPP Advances describes a research agenda for prospective research pre-onset to ADHD. The authors have formed the Early ADHD Consortium to improve future prospective research on the causes of ADHD. An important factor herein in my view is sample size. Clearly, we need the statistical power coming from large samples to get robust insights into the causes of ADHD: ADHD is a multifactorial condition resulting from the collective influence of multiple personal and environmental risk factors, each contributing a minor part but working in unison to heighten ADHD susceptibility. With none of the risk factors being necessary or sufficient, pathways toward onset of ADHD diverge among children and the to-be-expected small effects of any risk factor manifest as even smaller effects when averaged across all children in the study sample. This reasoning on why large samples are needed holds all the more if we are to identify the interplay (interactions) among risk factors. Furthermore, Miller et al. point out that neurodevelopmental alterations likely emerge prior to overt behavioral changes and that we need to understand causal factors at additional levels of understanding (e.g., metabolic or neural levels) next to the overt behavioral level, emphasizing once more the need for large samples. Team science in consortia to enlarge the sample turned out to be the game changer in the field of genetics, and this should also work out to improve our understanding of the broader (potentially interacting) causes of ADHD. JCPP Advances is therefore proud to publish the “Delineating early developmental pathways to ADHD: Setting an international research agenda” from the Early ADHD Consortium.

The second paper that I will highlight by Li et al. is also published in JCPP Advances (2023) and focuses on a different phase in the life course of individuals with ADHD. Li et al. are a group of authors who come from another recently formed ADHD consortium “TIMESPAN,” which zooms in on cardiometabolic diseases which in most cases have an onset (long) after the onset of ADHD. The meta-analysis by Li et al. is on cardiovascular diseases (CVD) and shows that adults with ADHD are nearly twice as likely to develop CVD than adults without ADHD. An important asset of the paper is that the authors embedded this finding in the broader available knowledge on the link between neuropsychiatric disorders and CVD, showing that the two-fold risk in relation to CVD is similar to estimates for schizophrenia and substance use disorder but larger than estimates for mood, anxiety and stress related disorders. This group from TIMESPAN has just published a similar meta-analysis on ADHD and type 2 diabetes (Garcia-Argibay et al., 2023), and a strong empirical study on ADHD and CVD (Li et al., 2022), and together with the evidence from the longer-standing research tradition on comorbidity of ADHD with obesity (Cortese et al., 2016; Nigg et al., 2016), we can conclude that the link between ADHD and cardiometabolic diseases has now been well-established. Together, these papers including the one by Li et al. in JCPP Advances discuss steps that need to be taken for a mechanistic understanding of the causes of an onset of comorbid cardiometabolic diseases in adults with ADHD as well as the consequences (particularly the consequences are studied in the TIMESPAN consortium). Like causes of ADHD need to be established pre-onset to ADHD, prospective research on the sequence of personal and environmental alterations leading up to a comorbid condition need to be established pre-onset of a comorbid condition. This, with the same complication as in ADHD that onset of a comorbid condition is gradual and does not only include overt alterations in personal risk factors but also, for example, metabolic or neural alterations. One important reason to form the TIMESPAN consortium was, like it likely was for the Early ADHD Consortium, the need for a large sample size to do prospective research on multifactorial conditions (in this case both ADHD and the different cardiometabolic diseases are multifactorial).

As argued, the ideas put forward by Miller et al. are not only relevant pre-onset to ADHD but throughout the lifespan. To illustrate this further, I want to highlight two aspects specifically. First, as pointed out already, Miller et al. emphasize that environmental risk factors of ADHD in early development may change over time. As an example of altering risk factors over development, maternal immune activation, during pregnancy, may contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (He et al., 2022), while parental difficulties to regulate their emotions, subsequently during toddlerhood, may at that point in time contribute to the risk of a later onset of ADHD (Claussen et al., 2022). Note that these findings, apart from that they need further study, have never been studied prospectively in the same children to establish potential accumulation of risk. We may extend the idea that risk factors change across the lifespan from the causes of ADHD to the course of ADHD. For example, with regard to the development of comorbid obesity, impulsive reward driven eating of high caloric, low nutrient, foods leading to overweight may be particularly prevalent in adolescents with ADHD, while lack of physical activity associated with difficulties with motivation and organization in ADHD may contribute to weight gain particularly in adults (note that this is a fictitious example, changes in risk factors contributing to weight gain in individuals with ADHD relative to individuals without ADHD in different developmental periods have not yet been studied). These examples on changing risk factors across the lifespan illustrate how much prospective work, both pre-onset to or post-onset of ADHD, is still to be done.

A second example of how ideas put forward by Miller et al. on causes of ADHD can be extended across the lifespan relates to the specificity or non-specificity of personal and environmental risk factors of ADHD (non-specificity meaning that they also play role in the onset of other conditions and not just in the onset of ADHD). Miller et al. explain that how an exclusive focus on onset of ADHD and the presumption of specificity of risk for this outcome may obscure transdiagnostic patterns and lead to inaccurate causal models. That non-specificity is ubiquitous and also relevant in the course of ADHD is undisputed. I already cited the example reported by Li et al. on the association of ADHD with CVD amidst similar associations of schizophrenia, substance use disorder, mood, anxiety and stress related disorders with CVD. Similarly, the most recent Genomewide Association Study on ADHD showed high sharing of genetic (concordant and discordant) genetic variants of ADHD with other disorders showing even more than before their strong intertwinement of the genetic architecture (Demontis et al., 2023). Specifically with regard to the onset of comorbid conditions during the course of ADHD, we may first of all draw from known personal and environmental risks of these conditions. The pertinent question with regard to specificity in this context is if these known risk factors have a similar effect whether ADHD is present or not (i.e., an interaction effect). For example, the effect may be stronger in individuals with ADHD compared to individuals without ADHD as in the previous example on how weight gain may be specifically linked to ADHD characteristics in different developmental periods (impulsivity and reward drivenness; motivational and organizational difficulties). In addition, prospective research may potentially show that timing may be different: known risk factors and precursors of onset of comorbidity may exert their influence earlier in individuals with than without ADHD. For example, there is some evidence that the onset of a condition is earlier in persons with than without ADHD (e.g., substance abuse: Dunne et al., 2014; CVD, Li et al., 2022). In addition, there may be risk factors that are specific to individuals with ADHD that lead up to the comorbid condition and which we currently do not know. Close monitoring in prospective research, as advocated by Miller et al., but now applied to the unfolding of the course of ADHD, specifically onset of comorbid conditions, will help establish this knowledge. This second example on the specificity of personal and environmental risk factors shows that broad knowledge beyond just ADHD is required. For example, in the example of Timespan, both specialists on ADHD and on cardiometabolic diseases take part. Again, if extended to other levels of understanding (metabolomic, neural, etc.), in addition to the overt personal risk factors, this holds even more. Thus, not only large sample sizes but also broad knowledge to do the best possible research can be accomplished by multidisciplinary team science.

In this editorial, I argued that making the distinctions between the causes of ADHD and the course of ADHD and between the causes of onset of comorbidity and the course thereafter are essential. At the same time, the gradual onset of ADHD and any comorbid condition makes clear that there is no hard cut between causes and consequences. Yet through close monitoring over time, we will be able to establish the cascades of personal and environmental alterations leading to the onset of ADHD and to the onset of comorbid conditions. In my ideal world, we would do prospective mechanistic research on ADHD (or any other condition) from pregnancy to death, but this is obviously not going to happen. Yet, papers from multidisciplinary research teams working together on different parts of the lifespan, like Miller et al. in the Early ADHD Consortium, and Li et al. in the Timespan consortium, show that we are making good progress in understanding the ADHD lifespan.

Catharina A. Hartman: Conceptualization; writing–original draft; writing–review and editing.

Catharina A. Hartman is part of the TIMESPAN consortium, but not a co-author on the cited papers. She is also Deputy Editor-in-Chief for JCPP Advances.

求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
审稿时长
13 weeks
期刊最新文献
Issue Information Implementing open science and reproducible research practices in mental health research through registered reports Special educational needs provision and academic outcomes for children with teacher reported language difficulties at school entry Examining the association of neighborhood conditions on attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in autistic youth using the child opportunity index 2.0 The trajectory of anxiety symptoms during the transition from childhood to young adulthood is predicted by IQ and sex, but not polygenic risk scores
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1