在模拟啼哭反应任务中,感知紧迫性和生理反应性预测母亲在婴儿情感调节中的行为

IF 0.1 0 HUMANITIES, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Tradition (Rabbinical Council of America) Pub Date : 2012-01-01 DOI:10.1037/E579192013-167
M. Verhage, S. D. Hartog, M. Oosterman, C. Schuengel
{"title":"在模拟啼哭反应任务中,感知紧迫性和生理反应性预测母亲在婴儿情感调节中的行为","authors":"M. Verhage, S. D. Hartog, M. Oosterman, C. Schuengel","doi":"10.1037/E579192013-167","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"Young infants need their caregivers to help them regulate their affect. When the caregiver does this successfully, infants gradually learn to regulate their affect themselves. However, regulating the affect of the infant is challenging for new parents; they have to learn which methods are effective from the signals the baby emits. To interpret these signals, caregivers have to be in control of their own emotions as well, which might be more difficult if an infant is hard to soothe, since research has shown that exposure to infant cries leads to strong physiological arousal (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989). The current study investigated with a simulated cry response task whether perceptions of a crying infant and physiological reactions were influenced by difficulty of regulating the infant’s affect. We were also interested in whether this had an effect on the response latency and the amount and consistency of the caregiver’s responses. This study has been executed with first-time pregnant women, because we expected them to be able to relate to this task more than any other group of non-parent adults, while their responses were not colored by previous parenting experiences. 179 women (age 18-40 years) who were pregnant with their first child participated at 22 weeks of pregnancy in a computerized cry response task involving audio taped baby cries. They listened to 2x10 baby cries during which they could choose one of four responses (feeding, distracting, changing the diaper or picking up the infant) to terminate the cry. The first series of cries simulated an easy to regulate infant (soothing success rate 80%, short cry duration); the second series represented a difficult to regulate infant (soothing success rate 20%, longer cry duration). After each series, the women filled out four questions regarding their perception of the cries. Physiological reactivity was measured using the VU-AMS (De Geus & Van Doornen, 1996), an ambulatory device that records ECG and ICG, allowing calculation of indicators of sympathetic (PEP; pre-ejection period) and parasympathetic nervous system activity (RSA: respiratory sinus arrhythmia). The cries of the difficult infant were perceived as more urgent, stressful, unpleasant and piercing (p<.01) than the cries of the easy infant. Women also gave more responses (p<.001) and changed their responses more often (p<.001) for the difficult infant. Perceived urgency was positively related to faster responses (p<.01), a larger number of responses (p=.064) and more altered responses (p<.01). Preliminary physiological data (n=76) showed that lower PEP (more sympathetic stress) was related to faster (p<.05) and more responses (p<.01). Longer, difficult to calm crying alters pregnant women’s perceptions of the meaning of the cries. Differences in perception and arousal may partly explain response tendencies to crying infants. Heightened physiological arousal may lead to excessive soothing behaviors.Nurturing Role Questionnaire) at 32 weeks of pregnancy (T1) and 3 months after birth (T2). Prenatally (at T1), these mothers stated that they would prefer to breastfeed as opposed to bottle-feed their infants. With respect to the postnatal breastfeeding questionnaire (at T2), mothers rated on 5-point Likert scales whether they 1) experienced breastfeeding as very difficult to very easy and 2) whether they experienced breastfeeding as very tiring to not tiring. The preliminary results of two hierarchical regression analyses, with PSE T1 scores entered first, indicated that mothers who experienced breastfeeding as easier or less tiring increased in PSE from T1 to T2 (respectively = .16, p < .05 and = .18, p < .01). PSE scores at both time points were not related to breastfeeding at T2. A subset of the mothers (n = 77) stopped to breastfeed their infants before three months of age. The mothers who indicated that they stopped earlier than planned had marginally lower PSE scores at T2 as opposed to mothers who did not stop earlier than planned (p = .08). For health care practices, the current findings emphasize the importance of supporting mothers’ early experiences with breastfeeding, especially since success-based efficacy beliefs are related to positive parenting and child outcomes.","PeriodicalId":83356,"journal":{"name":"Tradition (Rabbinical Council of America)","volume":"3 1 1","pages":"94-94"},"PeriodicalIF":0.1000,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Perceived urgency and physiological reactivity predict maternal behaviors in infant affect regulation during a simulated cry response task\",\"authors\":\"M. Verhage, S. D. Hartog, M. Oosterman, C. Schuengel\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/E579192013-167\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"Young infants need their caregivers to help them regulate their affect. When the caregiver does this successfully, infants gradually learn to regulate their affect themselves. However, regulating the affect of the infant is challenging for new parents; they have to learn which methods are effective from the signals the baby emits. To interpret these signals, caregivers have to be in control of their own emotions as well, which might be more difficult if an infant is hard to soothe, since research has shown that exposure to infant cries leads to strong physiological arousal (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989). The current study investigated with a simulated cry response task whether perceptions of a crying infant and physiological reactions were influenced by difficulty of regulating the infant’s affect. We were also interested in whether this had an effect on the response latency and the amount and consistency of the caregiver’s responses. This study has been executed with first-time pregnant women, because we expected them to be able to relate to this task more than any other group of non-parent adults, while their responses were not colored by previous parenting experiences. 179 women (age 18-40 years) who were pregnant with their first child participated at 22 weeks of pregnancy in a computerized cry response task involving audio taped baby cries. They listened to 2x10 baby cries during which they could choose one of four responses (feeding, distracting, changing the diaper or picking up the infant) to terminate the cry. The first series of cries simulated an easy to regulate infant (soothing success rate 80%, short cry duration); the second series represented a difficult to regulate infant (soothing success rate 20%, longer cry duration). After each series, the women filled out four questions regarding their perception of the cries. Physiological reactivity was measured using the VU-AMS (De Geus & Van Doornen, 1996), an ambulatory device that records ECG and ICG, allowing calculation of indicators of sympathetic (PEP; pre-ejection period) and parasympathetic nervous system activity (RSA: respiratory sinus arrhythmia). The cries of the difficult infant were perceived as more urgent, stressful, unpleasant and piercing (p<.01) than the cries of the easy infant. Women also gave more responses (p<.001) and changed their responses more often (p<.001) for the difficult infant. Perceived urgency was positively related to faster responses (p<.01), a larger number of responses (p=.064) and more altered responses (p<.01). Preliminary physiological data (n=76) showed that lower PEP (more sympathetic stress) was related to faster (p<.05) and more responses (p<.01). Longer, difficult to calm crying alters pregnant women’s perceptions of the meaning of the cries. Differences in perception and arousal may partly explain response tendencies to crying infants. Heightened physiological arousal may lead to excessive soothing behaviors.Nurturing Role Questionnaire) at 32 weeks of pregnancy (T1) and 3 months after birth (T2). Prenatally (at T1), these mothers stated that they would prefer to breastfeed as opposed to bottle-feed their infants. With respect to the postnatal breastfeeding questionnaire (at T2), mothers rated on 5-point Likert scales whether they 1) experienced breastfeeding as very difficult to very easy and 2) whether they experienced breastfeeding as very tiring to not tiring. The preliminary results of two hierarchical regression analyses, with PSE T1 scores entered first, indicated that mothers who experienced breastfeeding as easier or less tiring increased in PSE from T1 to T2 (respectively = .16, p < .05 and = .18, p < .01). PSE scores at both time points were not related to breastfeeding at T2. A subset of the mothers (n = 77) stopped to breastfeed their infants before three months of age. The mothers who indicated that they stopped earlier than planned had marginally lower PSE scores at T2 as opposed to mothers who did not stop earlier than planned (p = .08). For health care practices, the current findings emphasize the importance of supporting mothers’ early experiences with breastfeeding, especially since success-based efficacy beliefs are related to positive parenting and child outcomes.\",\"PeriodicalId\":83356,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Tradition (Rabbinical Council of America)\",\"volume\":\"3 1 1\",\"pages\":\"94-94\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.1000,\"publicationDate\":\"2012-01-01\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Tradition (Rabbinical Council of America)\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/E579192013-167\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"0\",\"JCRName\":\"HUMANITIES, MULTIDISCIPLINARY\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Tradition (Rabbinical Council of America)","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/E579192013-167","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"0","JCRName":"HUMANITIES, MULTIDISCIPLINARY","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

年幼的婴儿需要照顾者帮助他们调节自己的情绪。当照顾者成功地做到这一点时,婴儿就会逐渐学会自己调节自己的情绪。然而,调节婴儿的影响对新父母来说是一个挑战;他们必须从婴儿发出的信号中学习哪些方法是有效的。为了解释这些信号,照顾者也必须控制自己的情绪,如果婴儿很难安抚,这可能会更加困难,因为研究表明,接触婴儿的哭声会导致强烈的生理唤醒(Donovan & Leavitt, 1989)。本研究通过模拟哭闹反应任务调查了婴儿哭闹知觉和生理反应是否受到婴儿情绪调节困难的影响。我们也感兴趣的是,这是否对反应延迟和照顾者反应的数量和一致性有影响。这项研究是在第一次怀孕的妇女中进行的,因为我们希望她们比其他任何一组没有父母的成年人更能胜任这项任务,而她们的反应不受以前为人父母经历的影响。179名怀孕第一个孩子的妇女(年龄在18-40岁之间)在怀孕22周时参加了一项计算机化的哭泣反应任务,该任务涉及录制婴儿哭声。他们听了2x10个婴儿的哭声,在此期间,他们可以从四种反应(喂食、分散注意力、换尿布或抱起婴儿)中选择一种来结束哭声。第一个系列的哭声模拟了一个容易调节的婴儿(安抚成功率80%,哭声持续时间短);第二组表现为难以调节的婴儿(安抚成功率20%,哭闹持续时间较长)。在每个系列之后,这些女性填写了四个关于她们对哭泣的看法的问题。使用VU-AMS (De Geus & Van Doornen, 1996)测量生理反应性,这是一种记录心电图和ICG的移动设备,可以计算交感神经指标(PEP;和副交感神经系统活动(RSA:呼吸性窦性心律失常)。难相处婴儿的哭声被认为比易相处婴儿的哭声更紧急、更紧张、更不愉快、更刺耳(p< 0.01)。对于有困难的婴儿,妇女也给出了更多的回答(p<.001),并且更频繁地改变他们的回答(p<.001)。感知紧迫性与更快的反应(p< 0.01)、更多的反应(p= 0.064)和更多的改变反应(p< 0.01)正相关。初步生理数据(n=76)显示,PEP越低(交感神经压力越大)反应越快(p< 0.05),反应越多(p< 0.01)。长时间的、难以平息的哭泣会改变孕妇对哭泣意义的认知。知觉和觉醒的差异可能部分解释了对哭闹婴儿的反应倾向。增强的生理唤醒可能导致过度的安抚行为。在妊娠32周(T1)和出生后3个月(T2)进行养育角色问卷调查。在产前(1岁时),这些母亲表示她们更喜欢母乳喂养,而不是用奶瓶喂养婴儿。关于产后母乳喂养问卷(T2),母亲们用5分李克特量表对她们的感受进行打分:1)母乳喂养从非常困难到非常容易;2)母乳喂养从非常累到不累。首先输入PSE T1评分的两次分层回归分析的初步结果显示,在T1至T2期间,经历母乳喂养更容易或更少疲劳的母亲PSE得分增加(分别为= 0.16,p < 0.05和= 0.18,p < 0.01)。两个时间点的PSE评分与T2的母乳喂养无关。一部分母亲(n = 77)在婴儿3个月前停止母乳喂养。与没有比计划更早停止治疗的母亲相比,表明她们比计划更早停止治疗的母亲在T2时的PSE分数略低(p = .08)。对于卫生保健实践,目前的研究结果强调了支持母亲早期母乳喂养经验的重要性,特别是因为基于成功的效能信念与积极的养育和儿童结果有关。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Perceived urgency and physiological reactivity predict maternal behaviors in infant affect regulation during a simulated cry response task
Young infants need their caregivers to help them regulate their affect. When the caregiver does this successfully, infants gradually learn to regulate their affect themselves. However, regulating the affect of the infant is challenging for new parents; they have to learn which methods are effective from the signals the baby emits. To interpret these signals, caregivers have to be in control of their own emotions as well, which might be more difficult if an infant is hard to soothe, since research has shown that exposure to infant cries leads to strong physiological arousal (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989). The current study investigated with a simulated cry response task whether perceptions of a crying infant and physiological reactions were influenced by difficulty of regulating the infant’s affect. We were also interested in whether this had an effect on the response latency and the amount and consistency of the caregiver’s responses. This study has been executed with first-time pregnant women, because we expected them to be able to relate to this task more than any other group of non-parent adults, while their responses were not colored by previous parenting experiences. 179 women (age 18-40 years) who were pregnant with their first child participated at 22 weeks of pregnancy in a computerized cry response task involving audio taped baby cries. They listened to 2x10 baby cries during which they could choose one of four responses (feeding, distracting, changing the diaper or picking up the infant) to terminate the cry. The first series of cries simulated an easy to regulate infant (soothing success rate 80%, short cry duration); the second series represented a difficult to regulate infant (soothing success rate 20%, longer cry duration). After each series, the women filled out four questions regarding their perception of the cries. Physiological reactivity was measured using the VU-AMS (De Geus & Van Doornen, 1996), an ambulatory device that records ECG and ICG, allowing calculation of indicators of sympathetic (PEP; pre-ejection period) and parasympathetic nervous system activity (RSA: respiratory sinus arrhythmia). The cries of the difficult infant were perceived as more urgent, stressful, unpleasant and piercing (p<.01) than the cries of the easy infant. Women also gave more responses (p<.001) and changed their responses more often (p<.001) for the difficult infant. Perceived urgency was positively related to faster responses (p<.01), a larger number of responses (p=.064) and more altered responses (p<.01). Preliminary physiological data (n=76) showed that lower PEP (more sympathetic stress) was related to faster (p<.05) and more responses (p<.01). Longer, difficult to calm crying alters pregnant women’s perceptions of the meaning of the cries. Differences in perception and arousal may partly explain response tendencies to crying infants. Heightened physiological arousal may lead to excessive soothing behaviors.Nurturing Role Questionnaire) at 32 weeks of pregnancy (T1) and 3 months after birth (T2). Prenatally (at T1), these mothers stated that they would prefer to breastfeed as opposed to bottle-feed their infants. With respect to the postnatal breastfeeding questionnaire (at T2), mothers rated on 5-point Likert scales whether they 1) experienced breastfeeding as very difficult to very easy and 2) whether they experienced breastfeeding as very tiring to not tiring. The preliminary results of two hierarchical regression analyses, with PSE T1 scores entered first, indicated that mothers who experienced breastfeeding as easier or less tiring increased in PSE from T1 to T2 (respectively = .16, p < .05 and = .18, p < .01). PSE scores at both time points were not related to breastfeeding at T2. A subset of the mothers (n = 77) stopped to breastfeed their infants before three months of age. The mothers who indicated that they stopped earlier than planned had marginally lower PSE scores at T2 as opposed to mothers who did not stop earlier than planned (p = .08). For health care practices, the current findings emphasize the importance of supporting mothers’ early experiences with breastfeeding, especially since success-based efficacy beliefs are related to positive parenting and child outcomes.
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Baby empathy: Infant distress and peer prosocial behaviour Assessing the Emotional Quality of Parent–Child Relationships Involving Young Children with Special Needs: Applying the Constructs of Emotional Availability and Expressed Emotion The interaction of maternal personality traits and intimate partner violence as influences on maternal representations Nurturing Natures: Attachment and Children's Emotional, Sociocultural and Brain Development: Graham Music, Psychology Press, Hove, East Sussex, England, 2011, 314 pp Environmental Enrichment and Caregiver Training to Support the Development of Birth to 6-Year-Olds in Turkish Orphanages
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1