On children’s variable success with scalar inferences: Insights from disjunction in the scope of a universal quantifier

IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Cognition Pub Date : 2018-09-01 DOI:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.020
Elena Pagliarini , Cory Bill , Jacopo Romoli , Lyn Tieu , Stephen Crain
{"title":"On children’s variable success with scalar inferences: Insights from disjunction in the scope of a universal quantifier","authors":"Elena Pagliarini ,&nbsp;Cory Bill ,&nbsp;Jacopo Romoli ,&nbsp;Lyn Tieu ,&nbsp;Stephen Crain","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.020","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous developmental studies have revealed variation in children’s ability to compute scalar inferences. While children have been shown to struggle with standard scalar inferences (e.g., with scalar quantifiers like “some”) (Chierchia, Crain, Guasti, Gualmini, &amp; Meroni, 2001; Guasti et al., 2005; Noveck, 2001; Papafragou &amp; Musolino, 2003), there is also a growing handful of inferences that children have been reported to derive quite readily (Barner &amp; Bachrach, 2010; Hochstein, Bale, Fox, &amp; Barner, 2016; Papafragou &amp; Musolino, 2003; Singh, Wexler, Astle-Rahim, Kamawar, &amp; Fox, 2016; Stiller, Goodman, &amp; Frank, 2015; Tieu, Romoli, Zhou, &amp; Crain, 2016; Tieu et al., 2017). One recent approach, which we refer to as the <em>Alternatives-based approach</em>, attributes the variability in children’s performance to limitations in how children engage with the alternative sentences that are required to compute the relevant inferences. Specifically, if the alternative sentences can be generated by simplifying the assertion, rather than by lexically replacing one scalar term with another, children should be better able to compute the inference. In this paper, we investigated this prediction by assessing how children and adults interpret sentences that embed disjunction under a universal quantifier, such as “Every elephant caught a big butterfly or a small butterfly”. For adults, such sentences typically give rise to the <em>distributive inference</em> that <em>some elephant caught a big butterfly and some elephant caught a small butterfly</em> (Crnič, Chemla, &amp; Fox, 2015; Fox, 2007; Gazdar, 1979). Another possible interpretation, though not one typically accessed by adults, is the <em>conjunctive inference</em> that <em>every elephant caught a big butterfly and a small butterfly</em> (Singh, Wexler, Astle-Rahim, Kamawar, &amp; Fox, 2016). Crucially, for our purposes, it has been argued that both of these inferences can be derived using alternatives that are generated by deleting parts of the asserted sentence, rather than through lexical replacement, making these sentences an ideal test case for evaluating the predictions of the Alternatives-based approach. The findings of our experimental study reveal that children are indeed able to successfully compute this class of inferences, providing support for the Alternatives-based approach as a viable explanation of children’s variable success in computing scalar inferences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"178 ","pages":"Pages 178-192"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8000,"publicationDate":"2018-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.020","citationCount":"14","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Cognition","FirstCategoryId":"102","ListUrlMain":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027718301124","RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q1","JCRName":"PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14

Abstract

Previous developmental studies have revealed variation in children’s ability to compute scalar inferences. While children have been shown to struggle with standard scalar inferences (e.g., with scalar quantifiers like “some”) (Chierchia, Crain, Guasti, Gualmini, & Meroni, 2001; Guasti et al., 2005; Noveck, 2001; Papafragou & Musolino, 2003), there is also a growing handful of inferences that children have been reported to derive quite readily (Barner & Bachrach, 2010; Hochstein, Bale, Fox, & Barner, 2016; Papafragou & Musolino, 2003; Singh, Wexler, Astle-Rahim, Kamawar, & Fox, 2016; Stiller, Goodman, & Frank, 2015; Tieu, Romoli, Zhou, & Crain, 2016; Tieu et al., 2017). One recent approach, which we refer to as the Alternatives-based approach, attributes the variability in children’s performance to limitations in how children engage with the alternative sentences that are required to compute the relevant inferences. Specifically, if the alternative sentences can be generated by simplifying the assertion, rather than by lexically replacing one scalar term with another, children should be better able to compute the inference. In this paper, we investigated this prediction by assessing how children and adults interpret sentences that embed disjunction under a universal quantifier, such as “Every elephant caught a big butterfly or a small butterfly”. For adults, such sentences typically give rise to the distributive inference that some elephant caught a big butterfly and some elephant caught a small butterfly (Crnič, Chemla, & Fox, 2015; Fox, 2007; Gazdar, 1979). Another possible interpretation, though not one typically accessed by adults, is the conjunctive inference that every elephant caught a big butterfly and a small butterfly (Singh, Wexler, Astle-Rahim, Kamawar, & Fox, 2016). Crucially, for our purposes, it has been argued that both of these inferences can be derived using alternatives that are generated by deleting parts of the asserted sentence, rather than through lexical replacement, making these sentences an ideal test case for evaluating the predictions of the Alternatives-based approach. The findings of our experimental study reveal that children are indeed able to successfully compute this class of inferences, providing support for the Alternatives-based approach as a viable explanation of children’s variable success in computing scalar inferences.

查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
关于儿童在标量推理上的可变成功:从全称量词范围内的析取的见解
以前的发展研究揭示了儿童计算标量推理能力的差异。虽然儿童已经被证明在标准的标量推理(例如,像“一些”这样的标量量词)(Chierchia, Crain, Guasti, Gualmini, &Meroni, 2001;Guasti et al., 2005;诺韦克,2001;Papafragou,Musolino, 2003),也有越来越多的推论,据报道,儿童很容易得出(Barner &Bachrach, 2010;霍克斯坦,贝尔,福克斯,&;改革者,2016;Papafragou,Musolino, 2003;辛格、韦克斯勒、阿斯特尔-拉希姆、卡马瓦尔等;狐狸,2016;斯蒂勒,古德曼,&;弗兰克,2015;Tieu, Romoli, Zhou, &;科伦,2016;Tieu et al., 2017)。最近的一种方法,我们称之为基于替代的方法,将儿童表现的可变性归因于儿童如何参与计算相关推断所需的替代句子的限制。具体来说,如果替代句子可以通过简化断言来生成,而不是通过在词汇上用一个标量项替换另一个标量项来生成,那么孩子们应该能够更好地计算推理。在本文中,我们通过评估儿童和成人如何解释在通用量词下嵌入析取的句子来研究这一预测,例如“每只大象捕获了一只大蝴蝶或一只小蝴蝶”。对于成年人来说,这样的句子通常会产生一种分布推理,即有些大象抓到了一只大蝴蝶,有些大象抓到了一只小蝴蝶(crninik, Chemla, &狐狸,2015;狐狸,2007;Gazdar, 1979)。另一种可能的解释是,每头大象都抓到了一只大蝴蝶和一只小蝴蝶(Singh, weexler, Astle-Rahim, Kamawar, &狐狸,2016)。至关重要的是,就我们的目的而言,有人认为这两种推论都可以通过删除断言句子的部分而不是通过词汇替换生成的替代来推导,这使得这些句子成为评估基于替代方法的预测的理想测试用例。我们的实验研究结果表明,儿童确实能够成功地计算这类推断,为基于替代的方法提供支持,作为儿童在计算标量推断方面可变成功的可行解释。
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
相关文献
A Letter from Paris.
IF 0 Medical examiner (Chicago, Ill.)Pub Date : 1875-07-01 DOI:
C C Matteson
A Letter from Paris
IF 0.3 2区 艺术学MODERN DRAMAPub Date : 1960-05-01 DOI: 10.3138/MD.3.1.4
Jacques Scherer
来源期刊
Cognition
Cognition PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL-
CiteScore
6.40
自引率
5.90%
发文量
283
期刊介绍: Cognition is an international journal that publishes theoretical and experimental papers on the study of the mind. It covers a wide variety of subjects concerning all the different aspects of cognition, ranging from biological and experimental studies to formal analysis. Contributions from the fields of psychology, neuroscience, linguistics, computer science, mathematics, ethology and philosophy are welcome in this journal provided that they have some bearing on the functioning of the mind. In addition, the journal serves as a forum for discussion of social and political aspects of cognitive science.
期刊最新文献
When should the majority rule?: Children's developing intuitions about majority rules voting Kinematics in context: Predicting other’s action intentions entails the perception of affordances How the perception of events in children is influenced by language The role of iconicity in children's production of adverbial clauses Belief polarization can be caused by disagreements over source independence: Computational modelling, experimental evidence, and applicability to real-world politics
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1