首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Evidence that altercentric biases in a continuous false belief task depend on highlighting the agent's belief. 证据表明,在连续错误信念任务中,替代中心偏差依赖于突出代理的信念。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106055
Marie Luise Speiger, Katrin Rothmaler, Ulf Liszkowski, Hannes Rakoczy, Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann

As social beings, we excel at understanding what other people think or believe. We even seem to be influenced by the belief of others in situations where it is irrelevant to our current tasks. Such altercentric interference has been proposed to reflect implicit belief processing. However, in which situations altercentric interference occurs and to what extent it is automatic or dependent on the relevance of the belief in context are open questions. To investigate this, we developed a novel task testing whether participants show an altercentric bias when searching for an object in a continuous search space (a 'sandbox'). Critically, another agent is present that holds either a true or a false belief about the object location, depending on condition. We predicted that participants' search for the object would deviate from its actual location in direction of where the agent believed the object to be. Further, we tested how this altercentric bias would interact with an explicit belief reasoning version of the task, where participants are asked where the agent would look for the object. In two large, preregistered studies (N = 113 and N = 157), we found evidence for an altercentric bias in participants' object search. Importantly, this bias was only present in participants who conducted the explicit before the implicit task and started the experiment with the false belief condition. These findings indicate that altercentric biases depend on the relevance of the other's belief in the context of the task, suggesting that spontaneous belief processing is not automatic but context dependent.

作为社会生物,我们擅长理解别人的想法或信仰。在与我们当前任务无关的情况下,我们甚至似乎会受到他人信仰的影响。这种异中心干扰被提出来反映内隐信念加工。然而,在哪些情况下会发生交替中心干扰,以及它在多大程度上是自动的或依赖于背景中信念的相关性,这些都是悬而未决的问题。为了研究这一点,我们开发了一个新的任务,测试参与者在连续搜索空间(“沙盒”)中搜索物体时是否表现出替代中心偏见。关键的是,另一个代理存在,根据条件对物体位置持有正确或错误的信念。我们预测,参与者对物体的搜索将偏离其实际位置,偏离代理认为物体所在的方向。此外,我们测试了这种另类中心偏见如何与任务的明确信念推理版本相互作用,在该版本中,参与者被问及代理将在哪里寻找对象。在两项大型预注册研究(N = 113和N = 157)中,我们发现了参与者在对象搜索中存在异中心偏倚的证据。重要的是,这种偏见只出现在那些先执行外显任务后执行内隐任务,并以错误信念条件开始实验的参与者身上。这些发现表明,替代中心偏见依赖于任务情境中他人信念的相关性,这表明自发信念加工不是自动的,而是依赖于情境的。
{"title":"Evidence that altercentric biases in a continuous false belief task depend on highlighting the agent's belief.","authors":"Marie Luise Speiger, Katrin Rothmaler, Ulf Liszkowski, Hannes Rakoczy, Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106055","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106055","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As social beings, we excel at understanding what other people think or believe. We even seem to be influenced by the belief of others in situations where it is irrelevant to our current tasks. Such altercentric interference has been proposed to reflect implicit belief processing. However, in which situations altercentric interference occurs and to what extent it is automatic or dependent on the relevance of the belief in context are open questions. To investigate this, we developed a novel task testing whether participants show an altercentric bias when searching for an object in a continuous search space (a 'sandbox'). Critically, another agent is present that holds either a true or a false belief about the object location, depending on condition. We predicted that participants' search for the object would deviate from its actual location in direction of where the agent believed the object to be. Further, we tested how this altercentric bias would interact with an explicit belief reasoning version of the task, where participants are asked where the agent would look for the object. In two large, preregistered studies (N = 113 and N = 157), we found evidence for an altercentric bias in participants' object search. Importantly, this bias was only present in participants who conducted the explicit before the implicit task and started the experiment with the false belief condition. These findings indicate that altercentric biases depend on the relevance of the other's belief in the context of the task, suggesting that spontaneous belief processing is not automatic but context dependent.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106055"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142928520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why do Americans foresee a grim future for their country? The influences of country well-being, national identity, and news coverage. 为什么美国人预见到他们国家的严峻未来?国家福祉、国家认同和新闻报道的影响。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106052
Nazike Mert, Qi Wang

The present research examines the factors that contribute to a negative bias in how Americans imagine the future of their country. Specifically, we tested the effects of perceived country well-being, national identity (Study 1), and news coverage (Study 2) on Americans' collective future thinking. Study 1 was situated in a cross-cultural context, in which US and Chinese participants listed within 1 min as many exciting or worrying events as they could that might happen in their country's future and reported perceived country well-being and national identity. In Study 2, US participants read positive, negative, or neutral news events happening in their country and then imagined what might happen in their country's near and distant futures. Americans imagined more negative relative to positive events and rated positive events less positively and negative events more negatively than did Chinese, with the cultural differences explained by the lower perceived country well-being among Americans. US participants exposed to negative news showed greater negative bias in their collective future thoughts than those exposed to neutral or positive news, and the effect was explained by the lower perceived country well-being in the negative news condition. These findings underscore the complexity of collective future perceptions and the significance of psychological and societal factors in shaping how people foresee their country's future.

本研究考察了导致美国人对国家未来抱有负面偏见的因素。具体来说,我们测试了感知国家福祉、国家认同(研究1)和新闻报道(研究2)对美国人集体未来思维的影响。研究1是在跨文化背景下进行的,美国和中国的参与者在1分钟内尽可能多地列出他们国家未来可能发生的令人兴奋或担忧的事件,并报告他们对国家福祉和民族认同的看法。在研究2中,美国参与者阅读了发生在他们国家的积极、消极或中性的新闻事件,然后想象他们国家近期和遥远的未来可能发生的事情。与中国人相比,美国人对积极事件的想象更消极,对积极事件的评价更不积极,对消极事件的评价更消极,文化差异可以用美国人对国家幸福感的感知较低来解释。面对负面新闻的美国参与者比那些面对中性或正面新闻的参与者在集体未来想法中表现出更大的负面偏见,这种影响可以用负面新闻条件下较低的国家幸福感来解释。这些发现强调了集体对未来看法的复杂性,以及心理和社会因素在塑造人们如何预测自己国家未来方面的重要性。
{"title":"Why do Americans foresee a grim future for their country? The influences of country well-being, national identity, and news coverage.","authors":"Nazike Mert, Qi Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106052","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106052","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present research examines the factors that contribute to a negative bias in how Americans imagine the future of their country. Specifically, we tested the effects of perceived country well-being, national identity (Study 1), and news coverage (Study 2) on Americans' collective future thinking. Study 1 was situated in a cross-cultural context, in which US and Chinese participants listed within 1 min as many exciting or worrying events as they could that might happen in their country's future and reported perceived country well-being and national identity. In Study 2, US participants read positive, negative, or neutral news events happening in their country and then imagined what might happen in their country's near and distant futures. Americans imagined more negative relative to positive events and rated positive events less positively and negative events more negatively than did Chinese, with the cultural differences explained by the lower perceived country well-being among Americans. US participants exposed to negative news showed greater negative bias in their collective future thoughts than those exposed to neutral or positive news, and the effect was explained by the lower perceived country well-being in the negative news condition. These findings underscore the complexity of collective future perceptions and the significance of psychological and societal factors in shaping how people foresee their country's future.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106052"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142898904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral-dilemma judgments by individuals and groups: Are many heads really more utilitarian than one? 个人和群体的道德困境判断:多个头像真的比一个头像更功利吗?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106053
Marta Rokosz, Michał Białek, Michał M Stefańczyk, Bertram Gawronski

Moral dilemmas often involve a conflict between action-options that maximize outcomes for the greater good (utilitarianism) and inaction-options that conform to moral norms (deontology). Previous research suggests that, compared to individuals, groups show stronger support for outcome-maximizing actions that violate moral norms. The current study used a computational modeling approach to investigate whether this difference is driven by (1) stronger sensitivity to consequences, (2) weaker sensitivity to moral norms, or (3) weaker action aversion in moral-dilemma judgments made by groups. The results suggest that groups show a stronger sensitivity to consequences than individuals. Groups and individuals did not differ in terms of their sensitivity to moral norms and their general action aversion. The findings challenge the idea that groups are less action averse and less concerned about violating moral norms than individuals and instead suggest that group decisions are more strongly guided by outcomes for the greater good.

道德困境通常涉及两种选择之间的冲突,一种是为了更大的利益而最大化结果的行动选择(功利主义),另一种是符合道德规范的不行动选择(义务论)。先前的研究表明,与个人相比,群体对违反道德规范的结果最大化行为表现出更强的支持。目前的研究使用了计算模型的方法来研究这种差异是由(1)对后果的更强敏感性,(2)对道德规范的更弱敏感性,还是(3)群体在道德困境判断中更弱的行为厌恶所驱动的。结果表明,群体对结果的敏感度要高于个体。群体和个人在对道德规范的敏感性和对一般行为的厌恶程度方面没有差异。与个人相比,群体不那么厌恶行动,也不那么担心违反道德规范,这一发现对这种观点提出了挑战,相反,研究结果表明,群体决策更强烈地受到更大利益的结果的引导。
{"title":"Moral-dilemma judgments by individuals and groups: Are many heads really more utilitarian than one?","authors":"Marta Rokosz, Michał Białek, Michał M Stefańczyk, Bertram Gawronski","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106053","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106053","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moral dilemmas often involve a conflict between action-options that maximize outcomes for the greater good (utilitarianism) and inaction-options that conform to moral norms (deontology). Previous research suggests that, compared to individuals, groups show stronger support for outcome-maximizing actions that violate moral norms. The current study used a computational modeling approach to investigate whether this difference is driven by (1) stronger sensitivity to consequences, (2) weaker sensitivity to moral norms, or (3) weaker action aversion in moral-dilemma judgments made by groups. The results suggest that groups show a stronger sensitivity to consequences than individuals. Groups and individuals did not differ in terms of their sensitivity to moral norms and their general action aversion. The findings challenge the idea that groups are less action averse and less concerned about violating moral norms than individuals and instead suggest that group decisions are more strongly guided by outcomes for the greater good.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106053"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142899520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Part-based processing, but not holistic processing, predicts individual differences in face recognition abilities. 基于部分的处理,而不是整体的处理,可以预测人脸识别能力的个体差异。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106057
Pierre-Louis Audette, Laurianne Côté, Caroline Blais, Justin Duncan, Francis Gingras, Daniel Fiset

This study aimed to assess the roles of part-based and holistic processing for face processing ability (FPA). A psychophysical paradigm in which the efficiency at recognizing isolated or combined facial parts was used (N = 64), and holistic processing was defined as the perceptual integration from multiple parts. FPA and object processing ability were measured using a battery of tasks. A multiple linear regression including three predictors, namely perceptual integration, part-based efficiency, and object processing, explained 40 % of the variance in FPA. Most importantly, our results reveal a strong predictive relationship between part-based efficiency and FPA, a small predictive relationship between object processing ability and FPA, and no predictive relationship between perceptual integration and FPA. This result was obtained despite considerable variance in perceptual integration skills-with some participants exhibiting a highly efficient integration. These results indicate that part-based processing plays a pivotal role in FPA, whereas holistic processing does not.

本研究旨在探讨部分加工和整体加工对人脸加工能力的影响。一种心理物理范式,其中使用了识别孤立或组合面部部位的效率(N = 64),整体加工被定义为来自多个部分的感知整合。FPA和物体处理能力是通过一系列任务来测量的。多元线性回归包括三个预测因子,即感知整合、基于零件的效率和物体处理,解释了40%的FPA方差。最重要的是,我们的研究结果表明,零件效率与FPA之间存在较强的预测关系,物体处理能力与FPA之间存在较小的预测关系,而感知整合与FPA之间没有预测关系。这一结果是在感知整合技能有很大差异的情况下得出的,一些参与者表现出了高效的整合。这些结果表明,部分加工在FPA中起关键作用,而整体加工在FPA中不起关键作用。
{"title":"Part-based processing, but not holistic processing, predicts individual differences in face recognition abilities.","authors":"Pierre-Louis Audette, Laurianne Côté, Caroline Blais, Justin Duncan, Francis Gingras, Daniel Fiset","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106057","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106057","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to assess the roles of part-based and holistic processing for face processing ability (FPA). A psychophysical paradigm in which the efficiency at recognizing isolated or combined facial parts was used (N = 64), and holistic processing was defined as the perceptual integration from multiple parts. FPA and object processing ability were measured using a battery of tasks. A multiple linear regression including three predictors, namely perceptual integration, part-based efficiency, and object processing, explained 40 % of the variance in FPA. Most importantly, our results reveal a strong predictive relationship between part-based efficiency and FPA, a small predictive relationship between object processing ability and FPA, and no predictive relationship between perceptual integration and FPA. This result was obtained despite considerable variance in perceptual integration skills-with some participants exhibiting a highly efficient integration. These results indicate that part-based processing plays a pivotal role in FPA, whereas holistic processing does not.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106057"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142923737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are there unconscious visual images in aphantasia? Development of an implicit priming paradigm. 在幻像症中是否存在无意识的视觉形象?内隐启动范式的发展。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106059
Rudy Purkart, Maël Delem, Virginie Ranson, Charlotte Andrey, Rémy Versace, Eddy Cavalli, Gaën Plancher

For some people the experience of visual imagery is lacking, a condition recently referred to as aphantasia. So far, most of the studies on aphantasia rely on subjective reports, leaving the question of whether mental images can exist without reaching consciousness unresolved. In the present study, the formation of mental images was estimated in individuals with aphantasia without explicitly asking them to generate mental images. 151 Participants performed an implicit priming task where a probe is assumed to automatically reactivate a mental image. An explicit priming task, where participants were explicitly required to form a mental image after a probe, served as a control task. While control participants showed a priming effect in both the implicit and explicit tasks, aphantasics did not show any priming effects. These results suggest that aphantasia relies on a genuine inability to generate mental images rather than on a deficit in accessing these images consciously. Our priming paradigm might be a promising tool for characterizing mental images without relying on participant introspection.

对于一些人来说,缺乏视觉意象的体验,这种情况最近被称为幻觉。到目前为止,大多数关于幻觉的研究都依赖于主观报告,留下了一个问题,即心理图像是否可以在没有意识的情况下存在。在本研究中,在没有明确要求他们产生心理图像的情况下,估计了幻像症患者心理图像的形成。参与者执行了一个内隐启动任务,假设探针会自动重新激活一个心理图像。一项显性启动任务作为控制任务,参与者被明确要求在探测后形成一个心理图像。而对照组的参与者在内隐和外显任务中都表现出启动效应,而幻觉组则没有任何启动效应。这些结果表明,幻像症依赖于真正无法产生心理图像,而不是有意识地访问这些图像的缺陷。我们的启动范式可能是一个很有前途的工具,可以在不依赖于参与者内省的情况下描述心理图像。
{"title":"Are there unconscious visual images in aphantasia? Development of an implicit priming paradigm.","authors":"Rudy Purkart, Maël Delem, Virginie Ranson, Charlotte Andrey, Rémy Versace, Eddy Cavalli, Gaën Plancher","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106059","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106059","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>For some people the experience of visual imagery is lacking, a condition recently referred to as aphantasia. So far, most of the studies on aphantasia rely on subjective reports, leaving the question of whether mental images can exist without reaching consciousness unresolved. In the present study, the formation of mental images was estimated in individuals with aphantasia without explicitly asking them to generate mental images. 151 Participants performed an implicit priming task where a probe is assumed to automatically reactivate a mental image. An explicit priming task, where participants were explicitly required to form a mental image after a probe, served as a control task. While control participants showed a priming effect in both the implicit and explicit tasks, aphantasics did not show any priming effects. These results suggest that aphantasia relies on a genuine inability to generate mental images rather than on a deficit in accessing these images consciously. Our priming paradigm might be a promising tool for characterizing mental images without relying on participant introspection.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106059"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142956731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transfer of congruency effects between Stroop and multiplication tasks: Evidence that retrieval of multiplication facts requires inhibitory control. Stroop和乘法任务之间一致性效应的转移:乘法事实的检索需要抑制控制的证据。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106054
Joanne Eaves, Camilla Gilmore, Shachar Hochman, Lucy Cragg

Inhibitory control is classically considered a domain-general process, yet recent findings suggest it may operate in context-specific ways. This has important implications for theories in other cognitive domains, such as mathematics, in which inhibitory control is proposed to play a key role. Inhibitory control has been implicated in resolving interference between competing number facts when retrieving them from memory, yet clear evidence for this is lacking. Here we report two pre-registered experiments with adults that investigated transfer of inhibitory control between interleaved Stroop and multiplication fact retrieval trials. Experiment 1 (n = 450) measured the congruency sequence effect, where transfer of inhibitory control between trials leads to a reduced congruency effect following an incongruent trial. Experiment 2 (n = 370) measured transfer of the list-wide proportion congruency effect, where the congruency effect is reduced when incongruent trials are more frequent. We found evidence of transfer of the congruency sequence effect between Stroop and multiplication. This did not differ depending on whether the Stroop task used number or animal stimuli. There was no transfer of the list-wide proportion congruency effect. These results suggest that reactive, transient domain-general inhibitory control processes are involved in retrieving multiplication facts from memory. Our findings have implications for theories of cognitive control and mathematical cognition, but caution should be taken in interpreting implications for educational interventions.

抑制性控制通常被认为是一个领域一般过程,但最近的研究结果表明,它可能以特定情境的方式运作。这对其他认知领域的理论有重要的启示,如数学,其中抑制控制被认为起着关键作用。抑制控制涉及到在从记忆中检索数字事实时解决相互竞争的数字事实之间的干扰,但缺乏明确的证据。在这里,我们报告了两个预先注册的成人实验,研究了交叉Stroop和乘法事实检索试验之间的抑制控制转移。实验1 (n = 450)测量了一致性序列效应,其中试验之间的抑制控制转移导致不一致试验后一致性效应降低。实验2 (n = 370)测量了全列表比例一致性效应的转移,其中不一致试验越频繁,一致性效应越低。我们发现了同余序列效应在Stroop和乘法之间转移的证据。这并不取决于Stroop任务是使用数字刺激还是动物刺激。全表比例一致性效应不存在转移。这些结果表明,反应性,瞬态域-一般抑制控制过程涉及从记忆中检索乘法事实。我们的研究结果对认知控制和数学认知理论具有启示意义,但在解释教育干预的意义时应谨慎。
{"title":"Transfer of congruency effects between Stroop and multiplication tasks: Evidence that retrieval of multiplication facts requires inhibitory control.","authors":"Joanne Eaves, Camilla Gilmore, Shachar Hochman, Lucy Cragg","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106054","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106054","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Inhibitory control is classically considered a domain-general process, yet recent findings suggest it may operate in context-specific ways. This has important implications for theories in other cognitive domains, such as mathematics, in which inhibitory control is proposed to play a key role. Inhibitory control has been implicated in resolving interference between competing number facts when retrieving them from memory, yet clear evidence for this is lacking. Here we report two pre-registered experiments with adults that investigated transfer of inhibitory control between interleaved Stroop and multiplication fact retrieval trials. Experiment 1 (n = 450) measured the congruency sequence effect, where transfer of inhibitory control between trials leads to a reduced congruency effect following an incongruent trial. Experiment 2 (n = 370) measured transfer of the list-wide proportion congruency effect, where the congruency effect is reduced when incongruent trials are more frequent. We found evidence of transfer of the congruency sequence effect between Stroop and multiplication. This did not differ depending on whether the Stroop task used number or animal stimuli. There was no transfer of the list-wide proportion congruency effect. These results suggest that reactive, transient domain-general inhibitory control processes are involved in retrieving multiplication facts from memory. Our findings have implications for theories of cognitive control and mathematical cognition, but caution should be taken in interpreting implications for educational interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106054"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Blocking of associative learning by explicit descriptions. 显性描述对联想学习的阻碍。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015
Tom Kelly, Elliot A Ludvig

People given written descriptions often learn and decide differently from those learning from experience, even in formally identical tasks. This paper presents two experiments detailing how telling participants about the value of one stimulus impacts a keystone learning effect - blocking. The paper investigates if descriptions can be used to effectively block future trial-by-trial learning. Participants were presented with coloured shape stimuli and asked if those shapes caused reward. Experiment 1 found both standard, trial-by-trial experienced blocking and the novel effect of described blocking of future trial-by-trial learning. Experiment 2 investigated the conditions that promote described blocking by manipulating the training that occurred prior to exposure to the description. In the Pre-training Present group, participants exposed to a training set of compound and elemental stimuli produced more pronounced blocking than the Pre-training Absent group, which had no such training. These results show that explicit descriptions about causal relations can block learning from subsequent experience, providing a new extension of associative learning toward the verbal domain.

即使在形式上相同的任务中,被书面描述的人的学习和决定往往与那些从经验中学习的人不同。本文提出了两个实验,详细说明了告诉参与者一个刺激的价值如何影响一个关键的学习效应-阻碍。本文探讨了描述是否可以有效地阻止未来的逐次学习。研究人员向参与者展示了彩色形状的刺激,并询问这些形状是否会引起奖励。实验1发现了标准的、逐次经历的阻碍和描述的阻碍对未来逐次学习的新影响。实验2研究了通过操纵描述前的训练来促进描述阻塞的条件。在训练前在场组中,暴露于复合和基本刺激训练集的参与者比没有进行此类训练的训练前缺席组产生了更明显的阻滞。这些结果表明,对因果关系的明确描述可以阻碍对后续经验的学习,为联想学习向言语领域的扩展提供了新的途径。
{"title":"Blocking of associative learning by explicit descriptions.","authors":"Tom Kelly, Elliot A Ludvig","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106015","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People given written descriptions often learn and decide differently from those learning from experience, even in formally identical tasks. This paper presents two experiments detailing how telling participants about the value of one stimulus impacts a keystone learning effect - blocking. The paper investigates if descriptions can be used to effectively block future trial-by-trial learning. Participants were presented with coloured shape stimuli and asked if those shapes caused reward. Experiment 1 found both standard, trial-by-trial experienced blocking and the novel effect of described blocking of future trial-by-trial learning. Experiment 2 investigated the conditions that promote described blocking by manipulating the training that occurred prior to exposure to the description. In the Pre-training Present group, participants exposed to a training set of compound and elemental stimuli produced more pronounced blocking than the Pre-training Absent group, which had no such training. These results show that explicit descriptions about causal relations can block learning from subsequent experience, providing a new extension of associative learning toward the verbal domain.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106015"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142786606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disclosing sample bias fails to fully correct judgments of partisan extremity. 揭示样本偏见并不能完全纠正党派极端的判断。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106050
Alexandra M van der Valk, Alexander C Walker, Jonathan A Fugelsang, Derek J Koehler

How do we infer the beliefs of an entire group (e.g., Democrats) after being exposed to the beliefs of only a handful of group members? What if we know that the beliefs we encountered were selected in a biased manner? Across two experiments, we recruited 640 U.S. residents and assessed whether they could recognize and correct for such sample bias. Some participants viewed biased samples that exclusively featured the political opinions of extreme partisans, while others viewed representative samples free from selection biases. Results suggest that people do attempt to correct for known sample bias, but their efforts are often insufficient, leading them to make inaccurate inferences that align with sample bias. Specifically, participants tended to overestimate the ideological extremity of both Democrats and Republicans to a greater extent when exposed to explicitly biased samples, as opposed to representative ones. They also perceived members of the political party in question as holding more homogenous views, presumably because samples of extreme party members' views tend to have less variability than representative samples. Perhaps as a consequence, participants exposed to what they knew to be a biased sample, and who subsequently gave more biased estimates, did not express lower confidence in their estimates compared to participants who were shown representative samples. We discuss how a tendency to insufficiently adjust for transparently biased samples may contribute to partisan misperceptions that fuel political polarization.

在只接触到少数群体成员的信仰后,我们如何推断出整个群体(例如民主党人)的信仰?如果我们知道我们遇到的信念是以一种有偏见的方式选择的呢?在两个实验中,我们招募了640名美国居民,并评估他们是否能够识别和纠正这种样本偏差。一些参与者看到的是带有偏见的样本,其中只包含极端党派的政治观点,而另一些人看到的是没有选择偏见的代表性样本。结果表明,人们确实试图纠正已知的样本偏差,但他们的努力往往不够,导致他们做出与样本偏差一致的不准确推断。具体来说,当参与者接触到明显有偏见的样本时,他们倾向于在更大程度上高估民主党和共和党的意识形态极端,而不是代表性的样本。他们还认为,所讨论的政党成员持有更一致的观点,大概是因为极端政党成员的观点样本往往比代表性样本具有更小的可变性。也许正因为如此,那些暴露在他们所知道的有偏差样本中的参与者,以及随后给出更有偏差估计的参与者,对他们的估计并没有表现出比展示有代表性样本的参与者更低的信心。我们讨论了不充分调整透明偏见样本的倾向如何可能导致党派误解,从而加剧政治两极分化。
{"title":"Disclosing sample bias fails to fully correct judgments of partisan extremity.","authors":"Alexandra M van der Valk, Alexander C Walker, Jonathan A Fugelsang, Derek J Koehler","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106050","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106050","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How do we infer the beliefs of an entire group (e.g., Democrats) after being exposed to the beliefs of only a handful of group members? What if we know that the beliefs we encountered were selected in a biased manner? Across two experiments, we recruited 640 U.S. residents and assessed whether they could recognize and correct for such sample bias. Some participants viewed biased samples that exclusively featured the political opinions of extreme partisans, while others viewed representative samples free from selection biases. Results suggest that people do attempt to correct for known sample bias, but their efforts are often insufficient, leading them to make inaccurate inferences that align with sample bias. Specifically, participants tended to overestimate the ideological extremity of both Democrats and Republicans to a greater extent when exposed to explicitly biased samples, as opposed to representative ones. They also perceived members of the political party in question as holding more homogenous views, presumably because samples of extreme party members' views tend to have less variability than representative samples. Perhaps as a consequence, participants exposed to what they knew to be a biased sample, and who subsequently gave more biased estimates, did not express lower confidence in their estimates compared to participants who were shown representative samples. We discuss how a tendency to insufficiently adjust for transparently biased samples may contribute to partisan misperceptions that fuel political polarization.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106050"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142928517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Objective priming from pre-imagining inputs before binocular rivalry presentations does not predict individual differences in the subjective intensity of imagined experiences. 双目匹敌呈现前的客观前想象输入并不能预测想象体验主观强度的个体差异。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106048
Loren N Bouyer, Dietrich S Schwarzkopf, Blake W Saurels, Derek H Arnold

Most people can imagine images that they experience within their mind's eye. However, there are marked individual differences, with some people reporting that they are unable to visualise (aphantasics), and others who report having imagined experiences that are as realistic as seeing (hyper-phantasics). The vividness of imagery is most often measured via subjective self-report. Chang and Pearson (2018), however, have suggested that a binocular rivalry (BR) protocol can be used as an objective measure. They found that pre-imagining a moving input could enhance performance on an objective probe detection task when probes are embedded in imagery consistent inputs, as opposed to imagery inconsistent inputs. To date, nobody has assessed if this type of objective imagery priming can be used to predict the vividness of different people's visualisations. Here, we report that imagery priming of objective sensitivity to probes within static BR inputs does not correlate with the ratings people use to describe the vividness of their visualisations (a between participants effect). However, objective priming of sensitivity to probes embedded in BR inputs was greater on trials when participants reported that their pre-imagined experience had been more vivid than average (a within participants effect). Overall, our data suggest that while imagery can prime objective sensitivity to probes during BR, there is currently no strong evidence that this effect can be used as a reliable objective method to predict the subjective vividness of different people's visualisations.

大多数人都能在他们的脑海中想象他们所经历的图像。然而,有明显的个体差异,一些人报告说他们无法想象(幻觉),而另一些人报告说他们想象的经历和看到的一样真实(超级幻觉)。意象的生动程度通常是通过主观自我报告来衡量的。然而,Chang和Pearson(2018)建议双目竞争(BR)协议可以用作客观衡量标准。他们发现,当探针嵌入图像一致的输入时,预先想象一个移动的输入可以提高客观探针检测任务的性能,而不是图像不一致的输入。到目前为止,还没有人评估过这种类型的客观意象启动是否可以用来预测不同人的视觉形象的生动程度。在这里,我们报告了静态BR输入中对探针的客观敏感性的图像启动与人们用来描述其可视化的生动度的评级(参与者之间的效应)无关。然而,当参与者报告他们预先想象的经历比平均水平更生动时,对嵌入在BR输入中的探针的客观启动敏感性在试验中更大(参与者内部效应)。总的来说,我们的数据表明,虽然图像可以在BR过程中提高对探针的客观灵敏度,但目前还没有强有力的证据表明,这种效应可以作为一种可靠的客观方法来预测不同人的主观生动度。
{"title":"Objective priming from pre-imagining inputs before binocular rivalry presentations does not predict individual differences in the subjective intensity of imagined experiences.","authors":"Loren N Bouyer, Dietrich S Schwarzkopf, Blake W Saurels, Derek H Arnold","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106048","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106048","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most people can imagine images that they experience within their mind's eye. However, there are marked individual differences, with some people reporting that they are unable to visualise (aphantasics), and others who report having imagined experiences that are as realistic as seeing (hyper-phantasics). The vividness of imagery is most often measured via subjective self-report. Chang and Pearson (2018), however, have suggested that a binocular rivalry (BR) protocol can be used as an objective measure. They found that pre-imagining a moving input could enhance performance on an objective probe detection task when probes are embedded in imagery consistent inputs, as opposed to imagery inconsistent inputs. To date, nobody has assessed if this type of objective imagery priming can be used to predict the vividness of different people's visualisations. Here, we report that imagery priming of objective sensitivity to probes within static BR inputs does not correlate with the ratings people use to describe the vividness of their visualisations (a between participants effect). However, objective priming of sensitivity to probes embedded in BR inputs was greater on trials when participants reported that their pre-imagined experience had been more vivid than average (a within participants effect). Overall, our data suggest that while imagery can prime objective sensitivity to probes during BR, there is currently no strong evidence that this effect can be used as a reliable objective method to predict the subjective vividness of different people's visualisations.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106048"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142865770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognition facilitates theory of mind through optimal weighting of trait inferences. 元认知通过对特质推断的最佳加权来促进心理理论。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-12-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106042
Emily L Long, Caroline Catmur, Stephen M Fleming, Geoffrey Bird

The ability to represent and infer accurately others' mental states, known as Theory of Mind (ToM), has been theorised to be associated with metacognitive ability. Here, we considered the role of metacognition in mental state inference through the lens of a recent theoretical approach to explaining ToM, the 'Mind-space' framework. The Mind-space framework posits that trait inference, representation of the qualities of the mind giving rise to the mental state, is important in forming accurate mental state inferences. We tested a potential role for metacognition in facilitating optimal weighting of trait inferences, as well as several theoretical predictions regarding factors associated with the accuracy of trait inference and confidence in those trait inferences. Participants completed a judgement-of-confidence task in the trait inference domain alongside the Interview Task, a recently-developed task for assessing the accuracy of trait and mental state inferences. A simple relationship in which increased metacognitive sensitivity is associated with increased accuracy of mental states inferences was not found. However, when predicting trial-level performance, confidence in trait inference was shown to modulate the effect of trait inference accuracy on mental state inference accuracy. This effect was greater in magnitude with lower metacognitive sensitivity, i.e., when confidence is more likely to be misplaced. Furthermore, participants' trait inference ability was associated with the accuracy of their understanding of the average mind. In addition, the accuracy of specific trait inferences was predicted by the participant's similarity to the target, but this similarity benefit was reduced in participants whose self-perception was inaccurate. Reported confidence in a given trait inference was also predicted by participant-target similarity, such that participants showed greater overconfidence in judgements made about similar targets. This overconfidence effect was larger when self-perception was more erroneous. Results support several theoretical claims made by the Mind-space theory, and further elucidate the processes underlying accurate mental state inference.

准确描述和推断他人心理状态的能力,被称为心理理论(ToM),已被理论证明与元认知能力有关。在这里,我们通过最近的一种理论方法来解释ToM,即“心理空间”框架,来考虑元认知在心理状态推断中的作用。心理空间框架假定特质推断,即心理品质的表征,产生心理状态,对于形成准确的心理状态推断很重要。我们测试了元认知在促进特质推断的最佳加权方面的潜在作用,以及关于特质推断的准确性和对这些特质推断的信心相关因素的几个理论预测。参与者在完成特质推断领域的自信判断任务的同时,还完成了面试任务,这是一项最近开发的评估特质和心理状态推断准确性的任务。没有发现一个简单的关系,即增加的元认知敏感性与心理状态推断的准确性增加有关。然而,当预测试验水平的表现时,特质推断的信心被证明可以调节特质推断准确度对心理状态推断准确度的影响。这种影响在元认知敏感度较低的情况下更为显著,也就是说,当信心更有可能被放错位置时。此外,参与者的特质推断能力与他们对平均心智的理解的准确性有关。此外,特定特征推断的准确性由参与者与目标的相似性预测,但这种相似性利益在自我知觉不准确的参与者中降低。参与者与目标的相似性也可以预测参与者对特定特征推断的信心,因此参与者对相似目标的判断表现出更大的过度自信。当自我认知错误时,这种过度自信的影响更大。结果支持了心理空间理论提出的几个理论主张,并进一步阐明了准确心理状态推断的过程。
{"title":"Metacognition facilitates theory of mind through optimal weighting of trait inferences.","authors":"Emily L Long, Caroline Catmur, Stephen M Fleming, Geoffrey Bird","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106042","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.106042","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to represent and infer accurately others' mental states, known as Theory of Mind (ToM), has been theorised to be associated with metacognitive ability. Here, we considered the role of metacognition in mental state inference through the lens of a recent theoretical approach to explaining ToM, the 'Mind-space' framework. The Mind-space framework posits that trait inference, representation of the qualities of the mind giving rise to the mental state, is important in forming accurate mental state inferences. We tested a potential role for metacognition in facilitating optimal weighting of trait inferences, as well as several theoretical predictions regarding factors associated with the accuracy of trait inference and confidence in those trait inferences. Participants completed a judgement-of-confidence task in the trait inference domain alongside the Interview Task, a recently-developed task for assessing the accuracy of trait and mental state inferences. A simple relationship in which increased metacognitive sensitivity is associated with increased accuracy of mental states inferences was not found. However, when predicting trial-level performance, confidence in trait inference was shown to modulate the effect of trait inference accuracy on mental state inference accuracy. This effect was greater in magnitude with lower metacognitive sensitivity, i.e., when confidence is more likely to be misplaced. Furthermore, participants' trait inference ability was associated with the accuracy of their understanding of the average mind. In addition, the accuracy of specific trait inferences was predicted by the participant's similarity to the target, but this similarity benefit was reduced in participants whose self-perception was inaccurate. Reported confidence in a given trait inference was also predicted by participant-target similarity, such that participants showed greater overconfidence in judgements made about similar targets. This overconfidence effect was larger when self-perception was more erroneous. Results support several theoretical claims made by the Mind-space theory, and further elucidate the processes underlying accurate mental state inference.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":"256 ","pages":"106042"},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142873131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1