首页 > 最新文献

Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Absolute-judgment models better predict eyewitness decision-making than do relative-judgment models. 绝对判断模型比相对判断模型更能预测目击者的决策。
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105877
Andrew M Smith, Rebecca C Ying, Alexandria R Goldstein, Ryan J Fitzgerald

When presented with a lineup, the witness is tasked with identifying the culprit or indicating that the culprit is not present. The witness then qualifies the decision with a confidence judgment. But how do witnesses go about making these decisions and judgments? According to absolute-judgment models, witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the absolute strength of this MAX lineup member. Conversely, relative-judgment models propose that witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and then base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the relative strength of the MAX lineup member compared to the remaining lineup members. We took a critical test approach to test the predictions of both models. As predicted by the absolute-judgment model, but contrary to the predictions of the relative-judgment model, witnesses were more likely to correctly reject low-similarity lineups than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1), and more likely to reject biased lineups than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Likewise, witnesses rejected low-similarity lineups with greater confidence than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1) and rejected biased lineups with greater confidence than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Only a single pattern was consistent with the relative model and inconsistent with the absolute model: suspect identifications from biased lineups were made with greater confidence than suspect identifications from fair lineups (Experiment 2). The results suggest that absolute-judgment models better predict witness decision-making than do relative-judgment models and that pure relative-judgment models are unviable.

当排成一排时,证人的任务是指认罪犯或表示罪犯不在现场。然后,证人通过置信度判断对该决定进行定性。但是,证人是如何做出这些决定和判断的呢?根据绝对判断模型,证人会确定哪个列队成员与记忆的匹配度最高,并根据 MAX 列队成员的绝对强度做出辨认决定和置信度判断。与此相反,相对判断模型则认为,证人先确定哪个列队成员与记忆的匹配度最高,然后根据该 MAX 列队成员与其余列队成员的相对强度来做出辨认决定和可信度判断。我们采用临界检验法来检验这两种模型的预测结果。正如绝对判断模型所预测的,但与相对判断模型的预测相反,证人更有可能正确地拒绝低相似度阵容而不是高相似度阵容(实验 1),更有可能拒绝有偏见的阵容而不是公平的阵容(实验 2)。同样,证人拒绝低相似度列队的置信度高于高相似度列队(实验 1),拒绝有偏见列队的置信度高于公平列队(实验 2)。只有一种模式与相对模式一致,而与绝对模式不一致:从有偏见的列队中辨认出嫌疑人的可信度高于从公平列队中辨认出嫌疑人的可信度(实验 2)。结果表明,绝对判断模型比相对判断模型更能预测证人的决策,纯粹的相对判断模型是不可行的。
{"title":"Absolute-judgment models better predict eyewitness decision-making than do relative-judgment models.","authors":"Andrew M Smith, Rebecca C Ying, Alexandria R Goldstein, Ryan J Fitzgerald","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105877","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105877","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When presented with a lineup, the witness is tasked with identifying the culprit or indicating that the culprit is not present. The witness then qualifies the decision with a confidence judgment. But how do witnesses go about making these decisions and judgments? According to absolute-judgment models, witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the absolute strength of this MAX lineup member. Conversely, relative-judgment models propose that witnesses determine which lineup member provides the strongest match to memory and then base their identification decision and confidence judgment on the relative strength of the MAX lineup member compared to the remaining lineup members. We took a critical test approach to test the predictions of both models. As predicted by the absolute-judgment model, but contrary to the predictions of the relative-judgment model, witnesses were more likely to correctly reject low-similarity lineups than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1), and more likely to reject biased lineups than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Likewise, witnesses rejected low-similarity lineups with greater confidence than high-similarity lineups (Experiment 1) and rejected biased lineups with greater confidence than fair lineups (Experiment 2). Only a single pattern was consistent with the relative model and inconsistent with the absolute model: suspect identifications from biased lineups were made with greater confidence than suspect identifications from fair lineups (Experiment 2). The results suggest that absolute-judgment models better predict witness decision-making than do relative-judgment models and that pure relative-judgment models are unviable.</p>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141604370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do early meanings of negation map onto a fully-fledged negation concept in infancy? 否定的早期含义是否与婴儿期成熟的否定概念相对应?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105929

Young children acquire an amazing knowledge base, rapidly learning from, and even going beyond the observable evidence. They arrive at forming abstract concepts and generalizations and recruit logical operations. The question whether young infants can already rely on abstract logical operations, such as disjunction or negation, or whether these operations emerge gradually over development has recently become a central topic of interest. Here we target this question by focusing on infants' early understanding of negation. According to one view, negation comprehension is initially restricted to a narrow range of meanings (such as rejection or non-existence) and only much later infants develop a broader understanding that maps onto a fully-fledged negation concept. Alternatively, however, infants may rely on a fully-fledged negation concept from early on, but some forms of negation may pose more mapping and processing difficulties than others.

Here we tested infants' understanding of two syntactically and semantically different forms of negation, existential negation and propositional denial in a language (Hungarian) that has a separate negative particle for each, and thus the two negation forms can be directly compared. We engaged 15- and 18-month-old infants in a search task where they had to find a toy in one out of two locations based on verbal utterances referring to the object at one of the locations involving existential negation (Nincsen - not.be.3SG) or propositional denial (Nem itt van - not here be.3SG). In Experiments 1–3 we found a parallel development for these two kinds of negation. 18-month-olds successfully comprehended both, while 15-month-olds were at chance for both. In Experiment 4 we excluded the possibility that 15-month-olds' chance performance is explained by task-related difficulties, as they succeeded in a similar, but nonverbal task. Thus, 15-month-olds likely still have not solved the mapping for the two negation forms. The parallel performance of the two age groups with the two negation types (failing or succeeding on both) is consistent with the hypothesis that different forms of negation rely on similar conceptual underpinnings already in early development.

幼儿获得了惊人的知识基础,迅速从可观察到的证据中学习,甚至超越这些证据。他们开始形成抽象概念和概括,并开始使用逻辑运算。幼儿是否已经可以依赖析取或否定等抽象逻辑运算,或者这些运算是否是在发展过程中逐渐出现的,这个问题最近成为了人们关注的核心话题。在此,我们针对这一问题,重点研究幼儿对否定的早期理解。有一种观点认为,婴儿最初对否定的理解仅限于狭窄的含义范围(如拒绝或不存在),只有到了很晚的时候,婴儿才会发展出更广泛的理解,从而映射出一个完整的否定概念。在这里,我们测试了婴儿对两种句法和语义不同的否定形式--存在否定和命题否定--的理解,这两种语言(匈牙利语)都有单独的否定词,因此可以直接比较这两种否定形式。我们让 15 和 18 个月大的婴儿参加一项搜索任务,让他们根据涉及存在否定(Nincsen - not.be.3SG)或命题否定(Nem itt van - not here be.3SG)的言语,在两个地点中的一个地点找到一个玩具。在实验 1-3 中,我们发现这两种否定的发展是平行的。18 个月大的幼儿成功地理解了这两种否定,而 15 个月大的幼儿对这两种否定的理解都处于偶然状态。在实验 4 中,我们排除了 15 个月幼儿的偶然表现是由于任务相关困难所致的可能性,因为他们在类似的非言语任务中也取得了成功。因此,15 个月大的幼儿很可能仍未解决两种否定形式的映射问题。两个年龄组在两种否定类型上的平行表现(均失败或均成功)与不同形式的否定在早期发展中已依赖于相似概念基础的假设是一致的。
{"title":"Do early meanings of negation map onto a fully-fledged negation concept in infancy?","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105929","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105929","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Young children acquire an amazing knowledge base, rapidly learning from, and even going beyond the observable evidence. They arrive at forming abstract concepts and generalizations and recruit logical operations. The question whether young infants can already rely on abstract logical operations, such as disjunction or negation, or whether these operations emerge gradually over development has recently become a central topic of interest. Here we target this question by focusing on infants' early understanding of negation. According to one view, negation comprehension is initially restricted to a narrow range of meanings (such as rejection or non-existence) and only much later infants develop a broader understanding that maps onto a fully-fledged negation concept. Alternatively, however, infants may rely on a fully-fledged negation concept from early on, but some forms of negation may pose more mapping and processing difficulties than others.</p><p>Here we tested infants' understanding of two syntactically and semantically different forms of negation, existential negation and propositional denial in a language (Hungarian) that has a separate negative particle for each, and thus the two negation forms can be directly compared. We engaged 15- and 18-month-old infants in a search task where they had to find a toy in one out of two locations based on verbal utterances referring to the object at one of the locations involving existential negation (<em>Nincsen</em> - not.be.3SG) or propositional denial (<em>Nem itt van</em> - not here be.3SG). In Experiments 1–3 we found a parallel development for these two kinds of negation. 18-month-olds successfully comprehended both, while 15-month-olds were at chance for both. In Experiment 4 we excluded the possibility that 15-month-olds' chance performance is explained by task-related difficulties, as they succeeded in a similar, but nonverbal task. Thus, 15-month-olds likely still have not solved the mapping for the two negation forms. The parallel performance of the two age groups with the two negation types (failing or succeeding on both) is consistent with the hypothesis that different forms of negation rely on similar conceptual underpinnings already in early development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724002154/pdfft?md5=471b3c98ca100e618b168c352327ac39&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724002154-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142230548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The primacy of taxonomic semantic organization over thematic semantic organization during picture naming 图片命名过程中分类语义组织优先于主题语义组织
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105951

Different organizational structures have been argued to underlie semantic knowledge about concepts; taxonomic organization, based on shared features, and thematic organization based on co-occurrence in common scenes and scenarios. The goal of the current study is to examine which of the two organizational systems are more engaged in the semantic context of a picture naming task. To address this question, we examined the representational structure underlying the semantic space in different picture naming tasks by applying representational similarity analysis (RSA) to electroencephalography (EEG) datasets. In a series of experiments, EEG signals were collected while participants named pictures under different semantic contexts. Study 1 reanalyzes existing data from semantic contexts directing attention to taxonomic organization and semantic contexts that are not biased towards either taxonomic or thematic organization. In Study 2 we keep the stimuli the same and vary semantic contexts to draw attention to either taxonomic or thematic organization. The RSA approach allows us to examine the pairwise similarity in scalp-recorded amplitude patterns at each time point following the onset of the picture and relate it to theoretical taxonomic and thematic measures derived from computational models of semantics. Across all tasks, the similarity structure of scalp-recorded neural activity correlated better with taxonomic than thematic measures, in time windows associated with semantic processing. Most strikingly, we found that the scalp-recorded patterns of neural activity between taxonomically related items were more similar to each other than the scalp-recorded patterns of neural activity for thematically related or unrelated items, even in tasks that makes thematic information more salient. These results suggest that the principle semantic organization of these concepts during picture naming is taxonomic, at least in the context of picture naming.

不同的组织结构被认为是概念语义知识的基础;分类组织基于共同特征,而主题组织则基于共同场景和情景中的共同出现。本研究的目的是考察在图片命名任务的语义语境中,这两种组织系统中哪一种更能发挥作用。为了解决这个问题,我们将表征相似性分析(RSA)应用于脑电图(EEG)数据集,从而研究了不同图片命名任务中语义空间的表征结构。在一系列实验中,我们收集了参与者在不同语义语境下命名图片时的脑电信号。研究 1 对现有的数据进行了重新分析,这些数据来自关注分类组织的语义上下文和不偏重分类或主题组织的语义上下文。在研究 2 中,我们保持刺激物不变,并改变语义上下文,以引起对分类学或主题组织的注意。通过 RSA 方法,我们可以检查图片开始后每个时间点头皮记录的振幅模式的成对相似性,并将其与语义计算模型中得出的理论分类和主题测量结果联系起来。在所有任务中,在与语义处理相关的时间窗口中,头皮记录神经活动的相似性结构与分类学测量的相关性要好于与主题测量的相关性。最引人注目的是,我们发现分类学相关项目之间的头皮记录神经活动模式比主题相关或不相关项目的头皮记录神经活动模式更加相似,即使在主题信息更加突出的任务中也是如此。这些结果表明,在图片命名过程中,这些概念的主要语义组织是分类学的,至少在图片命名的语境中是如此。
{"title":"The primacy of taxonomic semantic organization over thematic semantic organization during picture naming","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105951","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105951","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Different organizational structures have been argued to underlie semantic knowledge about concepts; taxonomic organization, based on shared features, and thematic organization based on co-occurrence in common scenes and scenarios. The goal of the current study is to examine which of the two organizational systems are more engaged in the semantic context of a picture naming task. To address this question, we examined the representational structure underlying the semantic space in different picture naming tasks by applying representational similarity analysis (RSA) to electroencephalography (EEG) datasets. In a series of experiments, EEG signals were collected while participants named pictures under different semantic contexts. Study 1 reanalyzes existing data from semantic contexts directing attention to taxonomic organization and semantic contexts that are not biased towards either taxonomic or thematic organization. In Study 2 we keep the stimuli the same and vary semantic contexts to draw attention to either taxonomic or thematic organization. The RSA approach allows us to examine the pairwise similarity in scalp-recorded amplitude patterns at each time point following the onset of the picture and relate it to theoretical taxonomic and thematic measures derived from computational models of semantics. Across all tasks, the similarity structure of scalp-recorded neural activity correlated better with taxonomic than thematic measures, in time windows associated with semantic processing. Most strikingly, we found that the scalp-recorded patterns of neural activity between taxonomically related items were more similar to each other than the scalp-recorded patterns of neural activity for thematically related or unrelated items, even in tasks that makes thematic information more salient. These results suggest that the principle semantic organization of these concepts during picture naming is taxonomic, at least in the context of picture naming.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142230547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speech-to-song transformation in perception and production 感知和制作中的语音到歌曲转换
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105933

The speech-to-song transformation is an illusion in which certain spoken phrases are perceived as more song-like after being repeated several times. The present study addresses whether this perceptual transformation leads to a corresponding change in how accurately participants imitate pitch/time patterns in speech. We used illusion-inducing (illusion stimuli) and non-inducing (control stimuli) spoken phrases as stimuli. In each trial, one stimulus was presented eight times in succession. Participants were asked to reproduce the phrase and rate how music-like the phrase sounded after the first and final (eighth) repetitions. The ratings of illusion stimuli reflected more song-like perception after the final repetition than the first repetition, but the ratings of control stimuli did not change over repetitions. The results from imitative production mirrored the perceptual effects: pitch matching of illusion stimuli improved from the first to the final repetition, but pitch matching of control stimuli did not improve. These findings suggest a consistent pattern of speech-to-song transformation in both perception and production, suggesting that distinctions between music and language may be more malleable than originally thought both in perception and production.

语音到歌曲的转换是一种错觉,即某些口语短语在重复多次后会被认为更像歌曲。本研究探讨了这种知觉转换是否会导致受试者模仿语音中音高/时间模式的准确性发生相应变化。我们使用幻觉诱导(幻觉刺激)和非诱导(对照刺激)口语短语作为刺激。在每个试验中,一个刺激连续出现八次。参与者被要求重现该短语,并在第一次和最后一次(第八次)重现后对该短语听起来的音乐感进行评分。幻觉刺激的评分反映了最后一次重复后比第一次重复时更像歌曲的感觉,但对照刺激的评分在重复时没有变化。模仿制作的结果反映了知觉效应:幻觉刺激的音高匹配从第一次重复到最后一次重复都有所改善,但对照刺激的音高匹配没有改善。这些研究结果表明,在感知和制作过程中,语音到歌曲的转化模式是一致的,这表明在感知和制作过程中,音乐和语言之间的区别可能比最初想象的更具可塑性。
{"title":"Speech-to-song transformation in perception and production","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105933","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105933","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The speech-to-song transformation is an illusion in which certain spoken phrases are perceived as more song-like after being repeated several times. The present study addresses whether this perceptual transformation leads to a corresponding change in how accurately participants imitate pitch/time patterns in speech. We used illusion-inducing (illusion stimuli) and non-inducing (control stimuli) spoken phrases as stimuli. In each trial, one stimulus was presented eight times in succession. Participants were asked to reproduce the phrase and rate how music-like the phrase sounded after the first and final (eighth) repetitions. The ratings of illusion stimuli reflected more song-like perception after the final repetition than the first repetition, but the ratings of control stimuli did not change over repetitions. The results from imitative production mirrored the perceptual effects: pitch matching of illusion stimuli improved from the first to the final repetition, but pitch matching of control stimuli did not improve. These findings suggest a consistent pattern of speech-to-song transformation in both perception and production, suggesting that distinctions between music and language may be more malleable than originally thought both in perception and production.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142172476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does reductive information increase satisfaction with scientific explanations? Three preregistered tests of the reductive allure effect 还原信息会提高科学解释的满意度吗?还原诱惑效应的三个预先注册测试
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105941

Understanding information processing biases is critical for improving scientific literacy. Research suggests that people rate scientific explanations with reductive jargon (e.g., irrelevant chemistry jargon in the explanation of a biological phenomenon) as better than those without – a phenomenon known as the reductive allure (RA) effect. Here, however, in three preregistered online experiments, we were unable to replicate this reductive allure effect using similar (and in some cases identical) materials and procedures to the original demonstration of the phenomena. Our results suggest that text-based RA effects may not be as strong as previously thought and are possibly changing over time.

了解信息处理偏差对于提高科学素养至关重要。研究表明,人们对使用还原性专业术语(例如,在解释生物现象时使用无关的化学专业术语)的科学解释的评价要好于没有还原性专业术语的解释--这种现象被称为还原性诱惑(RA)效应。然而,在这里,在三个预先登记的在线实验中,我们使用了与原始现象演示相似(在某些情况下完全相同)的材料和程序,却无法复制这种还原诱惑效应。我们的结果表明,基于文本的还原诱惑效应可能并不像以前认为的那样强烈,而且可能会随着时间的推移而发生变化。
{"title":"Does reductive information increase satisfaction with scientific explanations? Three preregistered tests of the reductive allure effect","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105941","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105941","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Understanding information processing biases is critical for improving scientific literacy. Research suggests that people rate scientific explanations with reductive jargon (e.g., irrelevant chemistry jargon in the explanation of a biological phenomenon) as better than those without – a phenomenon known as the reductive allure (RA) effect. Here, however, in three preregistered online experiments, we were unable to replicate this reductive allure effect using similar (and in some cases identical) materials and procedures to the original demonstration of the phenomena. Our results suggest that text-based RA effects may not be as strong as previously thought and are possibly changing over time.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142164821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating children's valuation of authentic and inauthentic objects: Visible object properties vs. invisible ownership history 调查儿童对真假物品的评价:看得见的物品属性与看不见的所有权历史
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105935

In human culture, an object's value is influenced by tangible properties (e.g. visual desirability and constituent materials) and intangible ownership history (e.g. authentic objects owned by celebrities are often worth more than similar inauthentic objects). Children are sensitive to both of these factors as independent determinants of value, but research has yet to elucidate how they interact. Here, we investigate whether children aged 5–11 years consider object properties or authentic ownership history to be the greater determinant of value and examine how their object valuations are influenced by owners' characteristics. In Study 1, visually desirable and undesirable items belonging to ‘famously good’ owners received higher valuations than similar items belonging to non-famous owners, whereas desirable items belonging to ‘famously bad’ owners received significantly lower values. In Study 2, children considered items made from cheap materials belonging to famously good owners, but not famously bad owners, to be as valuable as similar items made from expensive materials belonging to non-famous owners. In Study 3, physical contact with a famously bad owner had a detrimental impact on value, but worn and unworn objects belonging to famously good owners did not significantly differ in value. Across studies, we documented evidence that children's sensitivity to authentic ownership history and physical contact as determinants of value increases with age. Together, these findings demonstrate that children's valuation of ownership history relative to object properties depends on the owner's ‘essence’, and their sensitivity to owner contact as a mediator of value indicates awareness of ‘magical contagion’.

在人类文化中,一件物品的价值受到有形属性(如视觉吸引力和组成材料)和无形所有权历史(如名人拥有的真品往往比类似的非真品更值钱)的影响。作为价值的独立决定因素,儿童对这两个因素都很敏感,但研究尚未阐明它们是如何相互作用的。在此,我们将研究 5-11 岁的儿童是认为物品的属性还是真实的所有权历史更能决定物品的价值,并探讨物品所有者的特征如何影响儿童对物品的评价。在研究 1 中,属于 "著名的好 "物主的视觉上可取和不可取物品的价值高于属于非著名物主的类似物品,而属于 "著名的坏 "物主的可取物品的价值则明显较低。在研究 2 中,儿童认为用廉价材料制作的、属于 "有名的好主人 "而非 "有名的坏主人 "的物品,与用昂贵材料制作的、属于 "无名的好主人 "的类似物品一样有价值。在研究 3 中,与名声不好的主人的身体接触会对物品价值产生不利影响,但属于名声好的主人的穿过和没穿过的物品在价值上没有显著差异。在所有的研究中,我们都有证据表明,随着年龄的增长,儿童对真实物主历史和身体接触作为价值决定因素的敏感度也会提高。这些发现共同表明,儿童对所有权历史和物品属性的评价取决于物品所有者的 "本质",而他们对物品所有者接触作为价值中介的敏感性表明他们意识到了 "魔力传染"。
{"title":"Investigating children's valuation of authentic and inauthentic objects: Visible object properties vs. invisible ownership history","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105935","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105935","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In human culture, an object's value is influenced by tangible properties (e.g. visual desirability and constituent materials) <em>and</em> intangible ownership history (e.g. authentic objects owned by celebrities are often worth more than similar inauthentic objects). Children are sensitive to both of these factors as independent determinants of value, but research has yet to elucidate how they interact. Here, we investigate whether children aged 5–11 years consider object properties or authentic ownership history to be the greater determinant of value and examine how their object valuations are influenced by owners' characteristics. In Study 1, visually desirable and undesirable items belonging to ‘famously good’ owners received higher valuations than similar items belonging to non-famous owners, whereas desirable items belonging to ‘famously bad’ owners received significantly lower values. In Study 2, children considered items made from cheap materials belonging to famously good owners, but not famously bad owners, to be as valuable as similar items made from expensive materials belonging to non-famous owners. In Study 3, physical contact with a famously bad owner had a detrimental impact on value, but worn and unworn objects belonging to famously good owners did not significantly differ in value. Across studies, we documented evidence that children's sensitivity to authentic ownership history and physical contact as determinants of value increases with age. Together, these findings demonstrate that children's valuation of ownership history relative to object properties depends on the owner's ‘essence’, and their sensitivity to owner contact as a mediator of value indicates awareness of ‘magical contagion’.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S001002772400221X/pdfft?md5=9944150959e06cdbc574ec0a4f778f4f&pid=1-s2.0-S001002772400221X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142164820","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognition of perceptual resolution across and around the visual field 跨视野和绕视野感知分辨率的元认知
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105938

Do people have accurate metacognition of non-uniformities in perceptual resolution across (i.e., eccentricity) and around (i.e., polar angle) the visual field? Despite its theoretical and practical importance, this question has not yet been empirically tested. This study investigated metacognition of perceptual resolution by guessing patterns during a degradation (i.e., loss of high spatial frequencies) localization task. Participants localized the degraded face among the nine faces that simultaneously appeared throughout the visual field: fovea (fixation at the center of the screen), parafovea (left, right, above, and below fixation at 4° eccentricity), and periphery (left, right, above, and below fixation at 10° eccentricity). We presumed that if participants had accurate metacognition, in the absence of a degraded face, they would exhibit compensatory guessing patterns based on counterfactual reasoning (“The degraded face must have been presented at locations with lower perceptual resolution, because if it were presented at locations with higher perceptual resolution, I would have easily detected it.”), meaning that we would expect more guess responses for locations with lower perceptual resolution. In two experiments, we observed guessing patterns that suggest that people can monitor non-uniformities in perceptual resolution across, but not around, the visual field during tasks, indicating partial in-the-moment metacognition. Additionally, we found that global explicit knowledge of perceptual resolution is not sufficient to guide in-the-moment metacognition during tasks, which suggests a dissociation between local and global metacognition.

人们对整个视野(即偏心率)和周围视野(即极角)知觉分辨率的不均匀性是否有准确的元认知?尽管这个问题在理论和实践上都很重要,但至今尚未得到实证检验。本研究通过在退化(即高空间频率损失)定位任务中猜测图案的方式,对知觉分辨率的元认知进行了研究。参与者在同时出现在整个视野中的九张面孔中对退化的面孔进行定位,这九张面孔分别是:眼窝(屏幕中心的固定位置)、眼窝旁(偏心率为 4°,固定位置的左、右、上、下)和周边(偏心率为 10°,固定位置的左、右、上、下)。我们假定,如果被试具有准确的元认知能力,那么在没有出现退化人脸的情况下,他们就会表现出基于反事实推理的补偿性猜测模式("退化人脸一定是出现在知觉分辨率较低的位置,因为如果它出现在知觉分辨率较高的位置,我就会很容易地发现它"),也就是说,我们会期望被试对知觉分辨率较低的位置做出更多的猜测反应。在两个实验中,我们观察到的猜测模式表明,人们可以在任务过程中监测整个视野而非周围视野中知觉分辨率的不均匀性,这表明了部分即时元认知。此外,我们还发现,关于知觉分辨率的全局性显性知识不足以指导任务过程中的即时元认知,这表明局部元认知和全局元认知之间存在分离。
{"title":"Metacognition of perceptual resolution across and around the visual field","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105938","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105938","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Do people have accurate metacognition of non-uniformities in perceptual resolution across (i.e., eccentricity) and around (i.e., polar angle) the visual field? Despite its theoretical and practical importance, this question has not yet been empirically tested. This study investigated metacognition of perceptual resolution by guessing patterns during a degradation (i.e., loss of high spatial frequencies) localization task. Participants localized the degraded face among the nine faces that simultaneously appeared throughout the visual field: fovea (fixation at the center of the screen), parafovea (left, right, above, and below fixation at 4° eccentricity), and periphery (left, right, above, and below fixation at 10° eccentricity). We presumed that if participants had accurate metacognition, in the absence of a degraded face, they would exhibit compensatory guessing patterns based on counterfactual reasoning (“The degraded face must have been presented at locations with lower perceptual resolution, because if it were presented at locations with higher perceptual resolution, I would have easily detected it.”), meaning that we would expect more guess responses for locations with lower perceptual resolution. In two experiments, we observed guessing patterns that suggest that people can monitor non-uniformities in perceptual resolution across, but not around, the visual field during tasks, indicating partial in-the-moment metacognition. Additionally, we found that global explicit knowledge of perceptual resolution is not sufficient to guide in-the-moment metacognition during tasks, which suggests a dissociation between local and global metacognition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142130178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Simple actions modulate context-dependent visual size perception at late processing stages 简单动作能在后期处理阶段调节与上下文相关的视觉大小感知
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105940

A simple button press towards a prime stimulus enhances subsequent visual search for objects that match the prime. The present study investigated whether this action effect is a general phenomenon across different task domains, and the underlying neural mechanisms. The action effect was measured in an unspeeded size-matching task, with the presentation of the central target and the surrounding inducers of the Ebbinghaus illusion together to one eye or separately to each eye, and when repetitive TMS was applied over right primary motor cortex (M1). The results showed that a prior key-press significantly reduced the illusion effect compared to passive viewing. Notably, the action effect persisted with dichoptic presentation of the Ebbinghaus configuration, but disappeared with the right M1 disruption. These results suggest that action guides visual perception to influence human behavior, which mainly affects the late visual processing stage and probably relies on feedback projections from the motor cortex.

对着素色刺激按下一个简单的按钮,会增强随后对与素色匹配的物体的视觉搜索。本研究探讨了这种动作效应是否是不同任务领域的普遍现象,以及其背后的神经机制。研究人员在一项无速度的大小匹配任务中测量了行动效应,该任务将中央目标和周围的艾宾浩斯幻觉诱导物同时呈现在一只眼睛上或分别呈现在两只眼睛上,并在右侧初级运动皮层(M1)上重复应用 TMS。结果表明,与被动观看相比,事先按下按键会显著降低幻觉效果。值得注意的是,动作效应在艾宾浩斯构型的双向呈现中持续存在,但在右侧 M1 干扰下消失了。这些结果表明,动作引导视觉感知影响人类行为,主要影响视觉处理的后期阶段,可能依赖于运动皮层的反馈投射。
{"title":"Simple actions modulate context-dependent visual size perception at late processing stages","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105940","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105940","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A simple button press towards a prime stimulus enhances subsequent visual search for objects that match the prime. The present study investigated whether this action effect is a general phenomenon across different task domains, and the underlying neural mechanisms. The action effect was measured in an unspeeded size-matching task, with the presentation of the central target and the surrounding inducers of the Ebbinghaus illusion together to one eye or separately to each eye, and when repetitive TMS was applied over right primary motor cortex (M1). The results showed that a prior key-press significantly reduced the illusion effect compared to passive viewing. Notably, the action effect persisted with dichoptic presentation of the Ebbinghaus configuration, but disappeared with the right M1 disruption. These results suggest that action guides visual perception to influence human behavior, which mainly affects the late visual processing stage and probably relies on feedback projections from the motor cortex.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027724002269/pdfft?md5=89837510b0c8abc6b41596937edd0bf0&pid=1-s2.0-S0010027724002269-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142121641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What is autonoetic consciousness? Examining what underlies subjective experience in memory and future thinking 什么是自体意识?研究记忆和未来思维中主观体验的基础是什么
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105934

Autonoetic consciousness is the awareness that an event we remember is one that we ourselves experienced. It is a defining feature of our subjective experience of remembering and imagining future events. Given its subjective nature, there is ongoing debate about how to measure it. Our goal was to develop a framework to identify cognitive markers of autonoetic consciousness. Across two studies (N = 342) we asked young, healthy participants to provide written descriptions of two autobiographical memories, two plausible future events, and an experimentally encoded video. Participants then rated their subjective experience during remembering and imagining. Exploratory Factor Analysis of this data uncovered the latent variables underlying autonoetic consciousness across these different events. In contrast to work that emphasizes the distinction between Remember and Know as being key to autonoetic consciousness, Re-experiencing, and Pre-experiencing for future events, were consistently identified as core markers of autonoetic consciousness. This was alongside Mental Time Travel in all types of memory events, but not for imagining the future. In addition, our factor analysis allows us to demonstrate directly - for the first time - the features of mental imagery associated with the sense of autonoetic consciousness in autobiographical memory; vivid, visual imagery from a first-person perspective. Finally, with regression analysis, the emergent factor structure of autonoetic consciousness was able to predict the richness of autobiographical memory texts, but not of episodic recall of the encoded video. This work provides a novel way to assess autonoetic consciousness, illustrates how autonoetic consciousness manifests differently in memory and imagination and defines the mental representations intrinsic to this process.

自发意识是一种意识,即我们记忆中的事件是我们自己亲身经历过的。它是我们记忆和想象未来事件的主观体验的一个决定性特征。鉴于其主观性,人们一直在争论如何衡量它。我们的目标是建立一个框架来识别自体意识的认知标记。在两项研究中(N = 342),我们要求年轻、健康的参与者对两段自传式记忆、两个似是而非的未来事件和一段实验编码视频进行书面描述。然后,参与者对他们在记忆和想象过程中的主观体验进行评分。对这些数据的探索性因子分析揭示了这些不同事件中潜在的自体意识变量。与强调 "记住 "和 "知道 "之间的区别是自体意识关键的研究不同,"再体验 "和 "对未来事件的预体验 "一直被认为是自体意识的核心标志。在所有类型的记忆事件中,这与心理时空旅行同时存在,但在想象未来时却不存在。此外,通过因子分析,我们首次直接展示了与自传体记忆中的自体意识相关的心理意象特征;第一人称视角的生动视觉意象。最后,通过回归分析,自发意识的新兴因子结构能够预测自传体记忆文本的丰富性,但不能预测编码视频的情节性回忆。这项研究提供了一种评估自体意识的新方法,说明了自体意识在记忆和想象中的不同表现,并定义了这一过程中固有的心理表征。
{"title":"What is autonoetic consciousness? Examining what underlies subjective experience in memory and future thinking","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105934","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105934","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Autonoetic consciousness is the awareness that an event we remember is one that we ourselves experienced. It is a defining feature of our subjective experience of remembering and imagining future events. Given its subjective nature, there is ongoing debate about how to measure it. Our goal was to develop a framework to identify cognitive markers of autonoetic consciousness. Across two studies (<em>N</em> = 342) we asked young, healthy participants to provide written descriptions of two autobiographical memories, two plausible future events, and an experimentally encoded video. Participants then rated their subjective experience during remembering and imagining. Exploratory Factor Analysis of this data uncovered the latent variables underlying autonoetic consciousness across these different events. In contrast to work that emphasizes the distinction between Remember and Know as being key to autonoetic consciousness, Re-experiencing, and Pre-experiencing for future events, were consistently identified as core markers of autonoetic consciousness. This was alongside Mental Time Travel in all types of memory events, but not for imagining the future. In addition, our factor analysis allows us to demonstrate directly - for the first time - the features of mental imagery associated with the sense of autonoetic consciousness in autobiographical memory; vivid, visual imagery from a first-person perspective. Finally, with regression analysis, the emergent factor structure of autonoetic consciousness was able to predict the richness of autobiographical memory texts, but not of episodic recall of the encoded video. This work provides a novel way to assess autonoetic consciousness, illustrates how autonoetic consciousness manifests differently in memory and imagination and defines the mental representations intrinsic to this process.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142098119","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thinking outside the red box: Does the simultaneous Showup distinguish between filler siphoning and diagnostic feature detection accounts of lineup/Showup differences? 跳出红框思考:同步展示是否能区分填充物虹吸和诊断特征检测对阵容/展示差异的解释?
IF 2.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105930

Lineups are considered a superior method of identification to showups, but why is contested. There are two main theories: diagnostic feature detection theory, which holds that surrounding the suspect with fillers causes the eyewitness to focus on the features that are most diagnostic, and differential filler siphoning theory that claims that the fillers draw incorrect choices away from the suspect. Colloff and Wixted (2020) created a novel identification task, called a simultaneous showup, designed to prevent filler siphoning, while still allowing comparison to occur between members of the array. However, even in the simultaneous showup, it is possible that covert filler siphoning occurs. In Experiment 1, we replicated the simultaneous showup condition and also asked participants if the other photos affected their decision making; we found evidence that participants self-reported both diagnostic feature detection and covert filler siphoning. In Experiment 2, we replicated Colloff and Wixted (2020, Experiment 3) main findings. Additionally, we found that participants self-reported both diagnostic feature detection and covert filler siphoning. This led us to conclude that the simultaneous showup procedure could not fully exclude covert filler siphoning from occurring.

列队指认被认为是优于示众指认的一种指认方法,但其原因却存在争议。主要有两种理论:一种是诊断特征检测理论,该理论认为,用填充物围绕嫌疑人会使目击者将注意力集中在最有诊断价值的特征上;另一种是差异填充物虹吸理论,该理论认为,填充物会将错误的选择从嫌疑人身上吸引走。Colloff 和 Wixted(2020 年)创造了一种新颖的识别任务,称为 "同时展示",旨在防止填充物虹吸,同时仍允许在阵列成员之间进行比较。然而,即使在同时展示中,也有可能发生隐蔽的填充物虹吸。在实验 1 中,我们复制了同时展示的条件,同时询问参与者其他照片是否影响了他们的决策;我们发现有证据表明,参与者自我报告了诊断性特征检测和隐蔽性填充物虹吸。在实验 2 中,我们重复了 Colloff 和 Wixted(2020 年,实验 3)的主要发现。此外,我们还发现被试同时自我报告了诊断性特征检测和隐蔽性填充物虹吸。这使我们得出结论:同时展示程序并不能完全排除隐蔽填充物虹吸的发生。
{"title":"Thinking outside the red box: Does the simultaneous Showup distinguish between filler siphoning and diagnostic feature detection accounts of lineup/Showup differences?","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105930","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cognition.2024.105930","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Lineups are considered a superior method of identification to showups, but why is contested. There are two main theories: diagnostic feature detection theory, which holds that surrounding the suspect with fillers causes the eyewitness to focus on the features that are most diagnostic, and differential filler siphoning theory that claims that the fillers draw incorrect choices away from the suspect. <span><span>Colloff and Wixted (2020)</span></span> created a novel identification task, called a simultaneous showup, designed to prevent filler siphoning, while still allowing comparison to occur between members of the array. However, even in the simultaneous showup, it is possible that covert filler siphoning occurs. In Experiment 1, we replicated the simultaneous showup condition and also asked participants if the other photos affected their decision making; we found evidence that participants self-reported both diagnostic feature detection and covert filler siphoning. In Experiment 2, we replicated <span><span>Colloff and Wixted (2020, Experiment 3)</span></span> main findings. Additionally, we found that participants self-reported both diagnostic feature detection and covert filler siphoning. This led us to conclude that the simultaneous showup procedure could not fully exclude covert filler siphoning from occurring.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48455,"journal":{"name":"Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142098122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1