Demonstrating habituation of a startle response to loud noise.

B. Phelps, Lyndsay Doyle-Lunders, Amanda Harsin-Waite, Nicole L. Hofman, Lacy M. Knutson
{"title":"Demonstrating habituation of a startle response to loud noise.","authors":"B. Phelps, Lyndsay Doyle-Lunders, Amanda Harsin-Waite, Nicole L. Hofman, Lacy M. Knutson","doi":"10.1037/H0100714","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"While classical conditioning is no doubt a standard part of most if not all introductory psychology courses, habituation is less prevalent as a topic. A search of Teaching of Psychology and others sources uncovered classroom demonstrations of classical conditioning processes, (Gibb, 1983; Kohn and Kalat, 1992; Sparrow & Fernald, 1989; Vernoy, 1987) while no examples of demonstrations of habituation were found. Most psychology students are familiar with the startle reflex, which is \"a universal and phylogenetically ancient reflex,\" (Howard & Ford, 1992, p. 695); it is one of the innate, defensive reflexes of humans and other vertebrates that occur in response to unconditioned eliciting stimuli such as an unexpected loud noise. A startle reflex that is elicited by an unpredicted noise is referred to as an acoustic or audiogenic startle reflex, (Wilkens, Hallett, & Wess, 1986; Brown, Rothwell, Thompson, Britton, Day, & Marsden, 1991; Gluck, Mercado, & Myers, 2008). Habituation is the process by which such reflexes are modified by experience; hence, it is a basic form of learning (Bouton, 2007; Gluck et al., 2008). A normal audiogenic startle reflex was first quantifiably studied by Jacobsen (1926), with further study by Landis and Hunt, (1936, 1937, 1939). The acoustic startle reflex consists of eye movements, especially blinking, facial grimacing, contractions of flexor muscles in the neck, trunk and elbows, with extensor muscles being activated much less often. Movements of the head and the remainder of the body are less reliably involved (Brown et al., 1991; Wilkens et al., 1986). Habituation occurred for all of these components with the exception of blinking (Wilkens et al., 1986). Howard and Ford (1992) observed considerable variation between individuals in the topography and duration of the startle reflex. As noted by Wilkens et al., (1986) the startle reflex is subject to habituation. Typically, an unconditioned response or reflex that is elicited by an unconditioned stimulus will show habituation if the stimulus is repeated (Gluck et al., 2008). That is, measures of the response will show orderly decreases in magnitude as the stimulus is repeatedly applied. More simply put, habituation is the process by which a stimulus loses its attention-grabbing properties, i.e., its \"novelty,\" and decreases in responsiveness to the stimulus are seen when the stimulus is presented repeatedly or for an extended period. Through the process of habituation, an organism ceases to engage in attending or other responses to the stimulus; the stimulus comes to be ignored (Bouton, 2007; Gluck et al., 2008). To illustrate habituation, the contingencies needed to elicit an acoustic startle reflex were arranged and repeated to record the habituation of the various responses. * METHODOLOGY Due to difficulties with an audience observing a person undergo a startle reflex, the demonstration to be described was conducted in another setting and digitally videotaped for later use in the classroom. Prior to any observations, protocol approval was obtained from the South Dakota State University Institutional Review Board. Four research assistants were involved, one of whom had volunteered to be videotaped and exposed to an eliciting stimulus; we initially needed to determine an effective unconditioned stimulus (US) with which to elicit the startle reflex, with the designated volunteer out of the room. We initially tested balloons being popped as suggested by Vernoy (1987), but found this event was not effective at eliciting a significant response. After some trial and error, we decided to strike a metal table with a standard weight claw hammer, which at an approximate distance of 1 m elicited a clearly observable startle reflex. A sound decibel meter allowed us to quantify the decibel level, approximately 80 dB. To produce rapid habituation, an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 15 s was employed. …","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"13 1","pages":"17-19"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2012-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"2","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The behavior analyst today","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100714","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2

Abstract

While classical conditioning is no doubt a standard part of most if not all introductory psychology courses, habituation is less prevalent as a topic. A search of Teaching of Psychology and others sources uncovered classroom demonstrations of classical conditioning processes, (Gibb, 1983; Kohn and Kalat, 1992; Sparrow & Fernald, 1989; Vernoy, 1987) while no examples of demonstrations of habituation were found. Most psychology students are familiar with the startle reflex, which is "a universal and phylogenetically ancient reflex," (Howard & Ford, 1992, p. 695); it is one of the innate, defensive reflexes of humans and other vertebrates that occur in response to unconditioned eliciting stimuli such as an unexpected loud noise. A startle reflex that is elicited by an unpredicted noise is referred to as an acoustic or audiogenic startle reflex, (Wilkens, Hallett, & Wess, 1986; Brown, Rothwell, Thompson, Britton, Day, & Marsden, 1991; Gluck, Mercado, & Myers, 2008). Habituation is the process by which such reflexes are modified by experience; hence, it is a basic form of learning (Bouton, 2007; Gluck et al., 2008). A normal audiogenic startle reflex was first quantifiably studied by Jacobsen (1926), with further study by Landis and Hunt, (1936, 1937, 1939). The acoustic startle reflex consists of eye movements, especially blinking, facial grimacing, contractions of flexor muscles in the neck, trunk and elbows, with extensor muscles being activated much less often. Movements of the head and the remainder of the body are less reliably involved (Brown et al., 1991; Wilkens et al., 1986). Habituation occurred for all of these components with the exception of blinking (Wilkens et al., 1986). Howard and Ford (1992) observed considerable variation between individuals in the topography and duration of the startle reflex. As noted by Wilkens et al., (1986) the startle reflex is subject to habituation. Typically, an unconditioned response or reflex that is elicited by an unconditioned stimulus will show habituation if the stimulus is repeated (Gluck et al., 2008). That is, measures of the response will show orderly decreases in magnitude as the stimulus is repeatedly applied. More simply put, habituation is the process by which a stimulus loses its attention-grabbing properties, i.e., its "novelty," and decreases in responsiveness to the stimulus are seen when the stimulus is presented repeatedly or for an extended period. Through the process of habituation, an organism ceases to engage in attending or other responses to the stimulus; the stimulus comes to be ignored (Bouton, 2007; Gluck et al., 2008). To illustrate habituation, the contingencies needed to elicit an acoustic startle reflex were arranged and repeated to record the habituation of the various responses. * METHODOLOGY Due to difficulties with an audience observing a person undergo a startle reflex, the demonstration to be described was conducted in another setting and digitally videotaped for later use in the classroom. Prior to any observations, protocol approval was obtained from the South Dakota State University Institutional Review Board. Four research assistants were involved, one of whom had volunteered to be videotaped and exposed to an eliciting stimulus; we initially needed to determine an effective unconditioned stimulus (US) with which to elicit the startle reflex, with the designated volunteer out of the room. We initially tested balloons being popped as suggested by Vernoy (1987), but found this event was not effective at eliciting a significant response. After some trial and error, we decided to strike a metal table with a standard weight claw hammer, which at an approximate distance of 1 m elicited a clearly observable startle reflex. A sound decibel meter allowed us to quantify the decibel level, approximately 80 dB. To produce rapid habituation, an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 15 s was employed. …
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
表现出对大声噪音的惊吓反应的习惯。
虽然经典条件反射毫无疑问是大多数(如果不是全部的话)心理学入门课程的标准部分,但习惯化作为一个主题却不那么普遍。对《心理学教学》和其他资料的研究揭示了经典条件反射过程的课堂演示(Gibb, 1983;Kohn and Kalat, 1992;Sparrow & Fernald, 1989;Vernoy, 1987),而没有发现习惯化的例子。大多数心理学专业的学生都熟悉惊吓反射,这是“一种普遍的、在系统发育上古老的反射”(Howard & Ford, 1992, p. 695);这是人类和其他脊椎动物的一种天生的、防御性的反射,发生在对无条件的刺激的反应中,比如意想不到的大声噪音。由不可预测的噪音引起的惊吓反射被称为声学或听源性惊吓反射(Wilkens, Hallett, & Wess, 1986;布朗,罗斯威尔,汤普森,布里顿,戴,马斯登,1991;Gluck, Mercado, & Myers, 2008)。习惯化是这种反射被经验修正的过程;因此,它是一种基本的学习形式(Bouton, 2007;Gluck et al., 2008)。Jacobsen(1926)首先对正常的听原惊吓反射进行了定量研究,Landis和Hunt(1936、1937、1939)进一步进行了研究。声惊反射包括眼球运动,尤其是眨眼、面部鬼脸、颈部、躯干和肘部屈肌的收缩,伸肌的激活频率要低得多。头部和身体其他部分的运动不太可靠地参与其中(Brown et al., 1991;Wilkens et al., 1986)。除了眨眼之外,所有这些组成部分都发生了习惯化(Wilkens et al., 1986)。Howard和Ford(1992)观察到个体在惊吓反射的地形和持续时间上有相当大的差异。正如Wilkens等人(1986)所指出的那样,惊吓反射受习惯化的影响。通常,由非条件刺激引起的非条件反应或反射,如果刺激被重复,就会表现出习惯化(Gluck et al., 2008)。也就是说,当刺激被反复施加时,对反应的测量将显示出有序的强度下降。更简单地说,习惯化是一个刺激失去其吸引注意力的特性的过程,即它的“新颖性”,当刺激重复出现或长时间出现时,对刺激的反应就会减少。通过习惯化过程,生物体停止参与对刺激的响应或其他反应;刺激逐渐被忽略(Bouton, 2007;Gluck et al., 2008)。为了说明习惯,需要引起声惊吓反射的偶然事件被安排和重复,以记录各种反应的习惯化。由于观众很难观察到一个人经历惊吓反射,所以要描述的演示在另一个环境中进行,并以数字录像带的方式进行,以便以后在课堂上使用。在任何观察之前,方案获得了南达科他州立大学机构审查委员会的批准。四名研究助理参与其中,其中一人自愿接受录像并暴露于诱发性刺激;我们最初需要确定一个有效的非条件刺激(US)来引发惊吓反射,指定的志愿者离开房间。我们最初按照Vernoy(1987)的建议测试了气球被爆开,但发现这一事件并不能有效地引起显著的反应。经过一些试验和错误,我们决定用标准重量的羊角锤敲击金属桌子,在大约1米的距离上引起了明显可观察到的惊吓反射。一个声音分贝计让我们可以量化分贝水平,大约80分贝。为了产生快速的习惯,使用了15秒的刺激间隔(ISI)。...
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
求助全文
约1分钟内获得全文 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Functional and morphological maturation of the full-sized and mini-pig corpus luteum by programmed cell death mechanism. Procedural aspects that control discounting rates when using the fill-in-the-blank and multiple-choice methods On the sequential and concurrent presentation of trials establishing prerequisites for emergent relations. Using SAFMEDS and direct instruction to teach the model of hierarchical complexity The zeitgeist of behavior analytic research in the 21st century: A keyword analysis.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1