Dagmar der Weduwen, Nick A R Jones, Adèle Dubosque, Stefan Schuster, Keith T Sillar, Mike Webster, Luke Rendell
{"title":"箭鱼的觅食成功率随直接竞争水平而变化,但与群体大小无关。","authors":"Dagmar der Weduwen, Nick A R Jones, Adèle Dubosque, Stefan Schuster, Keith T Sillar, Mike Webster, Luke Rendell","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arae040","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Group living can lead to kleptoparasitism, the theft of resources by competitors. Under such conditions, foragers may alter their behavior to minimize competition. However, it is unclear how such behavioral changes impact foraging performance. Archerfish (<i>Toxotes</i> spp.) are a good model for investigating the behavioral responses to kleptoparasitism, as their hunting method (shooting waterjets at insects perched above the water) leaves them vulnerable to theft. They must hit the target prey with sufficient force to dislodge it; thus, the prey may land some distance away from the shooter. Kleptoparasitism rates increase with group size in archerfish, and individuals alter their behavior around conspecifics. We investigated whether group size affected shooting success, using 7-spot archerfish <i>T. chatareus</i>. We considered a fish's shot to be successful if it knocked a fly, placed on a transparent platform above the tank, into the water. The probability of shooting success was modeled as a function of group size, aiming duration, nearest neighbor distance and position, and trial number. We found no effect of group size, aiming duration, or nearest neighbor distance or position on shooting success. Shooting success increased as trials progressed, likely due to the fish becoming more familiar with the task. We also found no change in the kleptoparasitism rate between group sizes. Instead, the likelihood of the shooter consuming the prey depended on the types of competition present at the time of shooting. We suggest that archerfish shooting behavior can be influenced by the presence of conspecifics in ways not previously considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"35 4","pages":"arae040"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5000,"publicationDate":"2024-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11134209/pdf/","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Archerfish foraging success varies with immediate competition level but not group size.\",\"authors\":\"Dagmar der Weduwen, Nick A R Jones, Adèle Dubosque, Stefan Schuster, Keith T Sillar, Mike Webster, Luke Rendell\",\"doi\":\"10.1093/beheco/arae040\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"<p><p>Group living can lead to kleptoparasitism, the theft of resources by competitors. Under such conditions, foragers may alter their behavior to minimize competition. However, it is unclear how such behavioral changes impact foraging performance. Archerfish (<i>Toxotes</i> spp.) are a good model for investigating the behavioral responses to kleptoparasitism, as their hunting method (shooting waterjets at insects perched above the water) leaves them vulnerable to theft. They must hit the target prey with sufficient force to dislodge it; thus, the prey may land some distance away from the shooter. Kleptoparasitism rates increase with group size in archerfish, and individuals alter their behavior around conspecifics. We investigated whether group size affected shooting success, using 7-spot archerfish <i>T. chatareus</i>. We considered a fish's shot to be successful if it knocked a fly, placed on a transparent platform above the tank, into the water. The probability of shooting success was modeled as a function of group size, aiming duration, nearest neighbor distance and position, and trial number. We found no effect of group size, aiming duration, or nearest neighbor distance or position on shooting success. Shooting success increased as trials progressed, likely due to the fish becoming more familiar with the task. We also found no change in the kleptoparasitism rate between group sizes. Instead, the likelihood of the shooter consuming the prey depended on the types of competition present at the time of shooting. We suggest that archerfish shooting behavior can be influenced by the presence of conspecifics in ways not previously considered.</p>\",\"PeriodicalId\":8840,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"Behavioral Ecology\",\"volume\":\"35 4\",\"pages\":\"arae040\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":2.5000,\"publicationDate\":\"2024-05-21\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11134209/pdf/\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"Behavioral Ecology\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"93\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae040\",\"RegionNum\":3,\"RegionCategory\":\"环境科学与生态学\",\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"2024/7/1 0:00:00\",\"PubModel\":\"eCollection\",\"JCR\":\"Q2\",\"JCRName\":\"BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Behavioral Ecology","FirstCategoryId":"93","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arae040","RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"2024/7/1 0:00:00","PubModel":"eCollection","JCR":"Q2","JCRName":"BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
摘要
群居生活可能导致偷窃寄生,即竞争对手偷窃资源。在这种情况下,觅食者可能会改变行为,以尽量减少竞争。然而,目前还不清楚这种行为变化如何影响觅食表现。箭鱼(Toxotes spp.)是研究偷窃寄生行为反应的一个很好的模型,因为它们的狩猎方法(向栖息在水面上的昆虫喷射水柱)使它们很容易被偷窃。它们必须以足够的力量击中目标猎物才能使其脱落;因此,猎物可能会落到离射手有一段距离的地方。箭鱼的爬行寄生率随群体大小而增加,个体在同类周围的行为也会改变。我们使用 7 点箭鱼 T. chatareus 研究了群体大小是否会影响射杀成功率。如果一条鱼将放在鱼缸上方透明平台上的苍蝇击落水中,我们就认为它的射击成功了。射击成功的概率被模拟为群体大小、瞄准持续时间、最近邻距离和位置以及试验次数的函数。我们发现,群体大小、瞄准持续时间、近邻距离或位置对射击成功率没有影响。随着试验次数的增加,射击成功率也在增加,这可能是由于鱼对任务越来越熟悉。我们还发现,不同大小的群体之间的偷食率也没有变化。相反,射手吃掉猎物的可能性取决于射箭时的竞争类型。我们认为,箭鱼的射击行为会受到同种鱼的影响,而这种影响是以前从未考虑过的。
Archerfish foraging success varies with immediate competition level but not group size.
Group living can lead to kleptoparasitism, the theft of resources by competitors. Under such conditions, foragers may alter their behavior to minimize competition. However, it is unclear how such behavioral changes impact foraging performance. Archerfish (Toxotes spp.) are a good model for investigating the behavioral responses to kleptoparasitism, as their hunting method (shooting waterjets at insects perched above the water) leaves them vulnerable to theft. They must hit the target prey with sufficient force to dislodge it; thus, the prey may land some distance away from the shooter. Kleptoparasitism rates increase with group size in archerfish, and individuals alter their behavior around conspecifics. We investigated whether group size affected shooting success, using 7-spot archerfish T. chatareus. We considered a fish's shot to be successful if it knocked a fly, placed on a transparent platform above the tank, into the water. The probability of shooting success was modeled as a function of group size, aiming duration, nearest neighbor distance and position, and trial number. We found no effect of group size, aiming duration, or nearest neighbor distance or position on shooting success. Shooting success increased as trials progressed, likely due to the fish becoming more familiar with the task. We also found no change in the kleptoparasitism rate between group sizes. Instead, the likelihood of the shooter consuming the prey depended on the types of competition present at the time of shooting. We suggest that archerfish shooting behavior can be influenced by the presence of conspecifics in ways not previously considered.
期刊介绍:
Studies on the whole range of behaving organisms, including plants, invertebrates, vertebrates, and humans, are included.
Behavioral Ecology construes the field in its broadest sense to include 1) the use of ecological and evolutionary processes to explain the occurrence and adaptive significance of behavior patterns; 2) the use of behavioral processes to predict ecological patterns, and 3) empirical, comparative analyses relating behavior to the environment in which it occurs.