《桥梁研究》特刊简介

D. Wacker, J. McComas, J. C. Borrero
{"title":"《桥梁研究》特刊简介","authors":"D. Wacker, J. McComas, J. C. Borrero","doi":"10.1037/H0100667","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"A unique aspect of behavior analysis as a discipline is the direct link between elements that are basic (operant mechanisms that underlie a response) and those that are applied (applications of those mechanisms to socially relevant behavior) (Wacker, 2000). This direct link provides opportunities for an almost seamless translation of knowledge gained in basic operant laboratories to significant social problems encountered in the community. For example, Kazdin (1978) noted a direct link in the 1960s between research conducted in laboratory settings and research conducted in behavior therapy. As studies were disseminated from operant laboratories, approaches to behavior therapy changed and, in a reciprocal fashion, studies in behavior therapy influenced studies conducted in operant labs. More recently, however, Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001) have noted that the \"bridges\" built in behavior analytic research are largely unidirectional. Specifically, what is learned in the nonhuman laboratory frequently informs behavior analytic practice. However, save a few exceptions, what occurs in practice infrequently directs nonhuman laboratory research. This state of affairs, as it seems to be, need not remain. Hake (1982) described studies that promoted reciprocal interactions between basic and applied behavior analysis as constituting bridge studies. Bridge studies provide analyses that increase our understanding of both operant mechanisms and socially meaningful behavior and thus provide the necessary links for viewing behavior analysis on a continuum rather than as separate categories of basic and applied research. The categorization of operant research as either basic or applied is often helpful because it permits consumers to determine which books, journals, and presentations may be of most interest to them. However, such categorization can also lead to the absence of the reciprocal relationship described by Kazdin (1978) and Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001). One outcome of this lack of reciprocity is that as practitioners, we may encounter problem behaviors that are resistant to change or treatment programs, and we have difficulty determining why the behavior is persisting or how to make fundamental changes to enhance the treatment's effectiveness. As applied researchers, we sometimes struggle with how to conduct additional studies to better understand the relation between the target behaviors of interest and the components within the treatment programs being conducted (Borrero, Vollmer, Samaha, Sloman, & Francisco, 2007). A disconnect between basic and applied behavior analysis leads toward descriptions of observed outcomes and away from analyses of why the outcomes occurred. It also leads to definitions of what constitutes a desirable treatment based on the structural components that comprise the treatment and away from functional analyses of the conditions under which any given treatment might be most effective. As an example, the phenomenon of maintenance is of critical importance to all applied programs, but even a cursory review of the applied literature will show only a few applied studies of maintenance, and even fewer studies that have attempted to analyze the conditions under which maintenance is most likely to occur or how it can be produced. Studies such as those in this special issue that focus on behavioral persistence (Dube, Ahearn, Lionello-DeNolf, & McIlvane), resurgence (Lattal & St. Peter-Pipken), and reinforcement contingencies (Vollmer, Samaha, & Sloman) offer analyses that have a direct and functional relationship with maintenance. We also have considerable evidence to suggest that a reinforcing stimulus may be considered a reinforcer only within specific boundaries (e.g., Meehl, 1950). An informed and evolving body of work that was spawned from traditional economic formulations of behavior offers a conceptual system that is consistent with the analysis of behavior and may inform application. …","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"10 1","pages":"234-237"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0000,"publicationDate":"2009-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":"0","resultStr":"{\"title\":\"Introduction to the BAT Special Issue on Bridge Studies\",\"authors\":\"D. Wacker, J. McComas, J. C. Borrero\",\"doi\":\"10.1037/H0100667\",\"DOIUrl\":null,\"url\":null,\"abstract\":\"A unique aspect of behavior analysis as a discipline is the direct link between elements that are basic (operant mechanisms that underlie a response) and those that are applied (applications of those mechanisms to socially relevant behavior) (Wacker, 2000). This direct link provides opportunities for an almost seamless translation of knowledge gained in basic operant laboratories to significant social problems encountered in the community. For example, Kazdin (1978) noted a direct link in the 1960s between research conducted in laboratory settings and research conducted in behavior therapy. As studies were disseminated from operant laboratories, approaches to behavior therapy changed and, in a reciprocal fashion, studies in behavior therapy influenced studies conducted in operant labs. More recently, however, Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001) have noted that the \\\"bridges\\\" built in behavior analytic research are largely unidirectional. Specifically, what is learned in the nonhuman laboratory frequently informs behavior analytic practice. However, save a few exceptions, what occurs in practice infrequently directs nonhuman laboratory research. This state of affairs, as it seems to be, need not remain. Hake (1982) described studies that promoted reciprocal interactions between basic and applied behavior analysis as constituting bridge studies. Bridge studies provide analyses that increase our understanding of both operant mechanisms and socially meaningful behavior and thus provide the necessary links for viewing behavior analysis on a continuum rather than as separate categories of basic and applied research. The categorization of operant research as either basic or applied is often helpful because it permits consumers to determine which books, journals, and presentations may be of most interest to them. However, such categorization can also lead to the absence of the reciprocal relationship described by Kazdin (1978) and Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001). One outcome of this lack of reciprocity is that as practitioners, we may encounter problem behaviors that are resistant to change or treatment programs, and we have difficulty determining why the behavior is persisting or how to make fundamental changes to enhance the treatment's effectiveness. As applied researchers, we sometimes struggle with how to conduct additional studies to better understand the relation between the target behaviors of interest and the components within the treatment programs being conducted (Borrero, Vollmer, Samaha, Sloman, & Francisco, 2007). A disconnect between basic and applied behavior analysis leads toward descriptions of observed outcomes and away from analyses of why the outcomes occurred. It also leads to definitions of what constitutes a desirable treatment based on the structural components that comprise the treatment and away from functional analyses of the conditions under which any given treatment might be most effective. As an example, the phenomenon of maintenance is of critical importance to all applied programs, but even a cursory review of the applied literature will show only a few applied studies of maintenance, and even fewer studies that have attempted to analyze the conditions under which maintenance is most likely to occur or how it can be produced. Studies such as those in this special issue that focus on behavioral persistence (Dube, Ahearn, Lionello-DeNolf, & McIlvane), resurgence (Lattal & St. Peter-Pipken), and reinforcement contingencies (Vollmer, Samaha, & Sloman) offer analyses that have a direct and functional relationship with maintenance. We also have considerable evidence to suggest that a reinforcing stimulus may be considered a reinforcer only within specific boundaries (e.g., Meehl, 1950). An informed and evolving body of work that was spawned from traditional economic formulations of behavior offers a conceptual system that is consistent with the analysis of behavior and may inform application. …\",\"PeriodicalId\":88717,\"journal\":{\"name\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"volume\":\"10 1\",\"pages\":\"234-237\"},\"PeriodicalIF\":0.0000,\"publicationDate\":\"2009-03-22\",\"publicationTypes\":\"Journal Article\",\"fieldsOfStudy\":null,\"isOpenAccess\":false,\"openAccessPdf\":\"\",\"citationCount\":\"0\",\"resultStr\":null,\"platform\":\"Semanticscholar\",\"paperid\":null,\"PeriodicalName\":\"The behavior analyst today\",\"FirstCategoryId\":\"1085\",\"ListUrlMain\":\"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100667\",\"RegionNum\":0,\"RegionCategory\":null,\"ArticlePicture\":[],\"TitleCN\":null,\"AbstractTextCN\":null,\"PMCID\":null,\"EPubDate\":\"\",\"PubModel\":\"\",\"JCR\":\"\",\"JCRName\":\"\",\"Score\":null,\"Total\":0}","platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"The behavior analyst today","FirstCategoryId":"1085","ListUrlMain":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100667","RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":null,"ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"","JCRName":"","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0

摘要

作为一门学科,行为分析的一个独特之处在于基本要素(反应背后的操作机制)和应用要素(将这些机制应用于社会相关行为)之间的直接联系(Wacker, 2000)。这种直接联系提供了几乎无缝地将基础操作实验室获得的知识转化为社区遇到的重大社会问题的机会。例如,Kazdin(1978)指出,在20世纪60年代,在实验室环境中进行的研究与在行为治疗中进行的研究之间存在直接联系。随着研究从操作性实验室传播,行为治疗的方法发生了变化,并且以相互的方式,行为治疗的研究影响了操作性实验室进行的研究。然而,最近,Vollmer和Hackenberg(2001)指出,行为分析研究中建立的“桥梁”在很大程度上是单向的。具体来说,在非人类实验室中所学到的东西经常会影响行为分析实践。然而,除了少数例外,实践中发生的事情很少指导非人类的实验室研究。看来,这种事态不必再持续下去了。Hake(1982)将促进基础行为分析和应用行为分析之间相互作用的研究描述为构成桥梁研究。桥梁研究提供的分析增加了我们对操作机制和社会意义行为的理解,从而为将行为分析视为一个连续体而不是作为基础研究和应用研究的单独类别提供了必要的联系。将操作性研究分为基础研究和应用研究通常是有帮助的,因为它允许消费者确定哪些书籍、期刊和演示文稿可能是他们最感兴趣的。然而,这样的分类也会导致Kazdin(1978)和Vollmer and Hackenberg(2001)所描述的互惠关系的缺失。缺乏互惠的一个结果是,作为从业者,我们可能会遇到抗拒改变或治疗方案的问题行为,我们很难确定为什么这种行为会持续下去,或者如何做出根本的改变来提高治疗的有效性。作为应用研究人员,我们有时会纠结于如何进行额外的研究,以更好地理解感兴趣的目标行为与正在进行的治疗方案中的组成部分之间的关系(Borrero, Vollmer, Samaha, Sloman, & Francisco, 2007)。基本行为分析和应用行为分析之间的脱节导致了对观察结果的描述,而远离了对结果发生的原因的分析。它还导致根据构成治疗的结构成分来定义什么是理想的治疗,而远离对任何给定治疗可能最有效的条件的功能分析。例如,维护现象对所有应用程序都是至关重要的,但即使是对应用文献的粗略回顾也只会显示出很少的维护应用研究,甚至更少的研究试图分析最有可能发生维护的条件或如何产生维护。本期特约关注行为持续性(Dube, Ahearn, Lionello-DeNolf, & McIlvane)、复苏(Lattal & St. Peter-Pipken)和强化偶发(Vollmer, Samaha, & Sloman)的研究提供了与维持有直接和功能关系的分析。我们也有相当多的证据表明,只有在特定的界限内,强化刺激才能被认为是强化物(例如,Meehl, 1950)。从传统的行为经济学公式中衍生出来的知识丰富和不断发展的工作体系提供了一个与行为分析一致的概念系统,并可能为应用提供信息。...
本文章由计算机程序翻译,如有差异,请以英文原文为准。
查看原文
分享 分享
微信好友 朋友圈 QQ好友 复制链接
本刊更多论文
Introduction to the BAT Special Issue on Bridge Studies
A unique aspect of behavior analysis as a discipline is the direct link between elements that are basic (operant mechanisms that underlie a response) and those that are applied (applications of those mechanisms to socially relevant behavior) (Wacker, 2000). This direct link provides opportunities for an almost seamless translation of knowledge gained in basic operant laboratories to significant social problems encountered in the community. For example, Kazdin (1978) noted a direct link in the 1960s between research conducted in laboratory settings and research conducted in behavior therapy. As studies were disseminated from operant laboratories, approaches to behavior therapy changed and, in a reciprocal fashion, studies in behavior therapy influenced studies conducted in operant labs. More recently, however, Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001) have noted that the "bridges" built in behavior analytic research are largely unidirectional. Specifically, what is learned in the nonhuman laboratory frequently informs behavior analytic practice. However, save a few exceptions, what occurs in practice infrequently directs nonhuman laboratory research. This state of affairs, as it seems to be, need not remain. Hake (1982) described studies that promoted reciprocal interactions between basic and applied behavior analysis as constituting bridge studies. Bridge studies provide analyses that increase our understanding of both operant mechanisms and socially meaningful behavior and thus provide the necessary links for viewing behavior analysis on a continuum rather than as separate categories of basic and applied research. The categorization of operant research as either basic or applied is often helpful because it permits consumers to determine which books, journals, and presentations may be of most interest to them. However, such categorization can also lead to the absence of the reciprocal relationship described by Kazdin (1978) and Vollmer and Hackenberg (2001). One outcome of this lack of reciprocity is that as practitioners, we may encounter problem behaviors that are resistant to change or treatment programs, and we have difficulty determining why the behavior is persisting or how to make fundamental changes to enhance the treatment's effectiveness. As applied researchers, we sometimes struggle with how to conduct additional studies to better understand the relation between the target behaviors of interest and the components within the treatment programs being conducted (Borrero, Vollmer, Samaha, Sloman, & Francisco, 2007). A disconnect between basic and applied behavior analysis leads toward descriptions of observed outcomes and away from analyses of why the outcomes occurred. It also leads to definitions of what constitutes a desirable treatment based on the structural components that comprise the treatment and away from functional analyses of the conditions under which any given treatment might be most effective. As an example, the phenomenon of maintenance is of critical importance to all applied programs, but even a cursory review of the applied literature will show only a few applied studies of maintenance, and even fewer studies that have attempted to analyze the conditions under which maintenance is most likely to occur or how it can be produced. Studies such as those in this special issue that focus on behavioral persistence (Dube, Ahearn, Lionello-DeNolf, & McIlvane), resurgence (Lattal & St. Peter-Pipken), and reinforcement contingencies (Vollmer, Samaha, & Sloman) offer analyses that have a direct and functional relationship with maintenance. We also have considerable evidence to suggest that a reinforcing stimulus may be considered a reinforcer only within specific boundaries (e.g., Meehl, 1950). An informed and evolving body of work that was spawned from traditional economic formulations of behavior offers a conceptual system that is consistent with the analysis of behavior and may inform application. …
求助全文
通过发布文献求助,成功后即可免费获取论文全文。 去求助
来源期刊
自引率
0.00%
发文量
0
期刊最新文献
Functional and morphological maturation of the full-sized and mini-pig corpus luteum by programmed cell death mechanism. Procedural aspects that control discounting rates when using the fill-in-the-blank and multiple-choice methods On the sequential and concurrent presentation of trials establishing prerequisites for emergent relations. Using SAFMEDS and direct instruction to teach the model of hierarchical complexity The zeitgeist of behavior analytic research in the 21st century: A keyword analysis.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
已复制链接
已复制链接
快去分享给好友吧!
我知道了
×
扫码分享
扫码分享
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1