{"title":"Changes in Taste Threshold, Perceived Intensity, Liking, and Preference in Pregnant Women: a Literature Review","authors":"Hugo Weenen, Annemarie Olsen, Evangelia Nanou, Esmée Moreau, Smita Nambiar, Carel Vereijken, Leilani Muhardi","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","DOIUrl":null,"url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies of changes in taste threshold, perceived intensity, liking, and preference during pregnancy were reviewed, because such changes have the potential to negatively impact nutrient intake in pregnant women.</p><p>Medline and Web of Science were searched; eligibility was based on inclusion, exclusion, and quality criteria.</p><p>Fourteen articles were included: 5 reported taste thresholds, 8 taste intensity, and 13 liking/preferences. Articles addressed sweet, salty, sour, and bitter, not umami. Results of self-reported changes suggested that many women experienced some sort of alteration in taste during pregnancy. Studies with real stimuli demonstrated that the only consistent finding for taste thresholds was that pregnant women showed higher thresholds for bitterness in their first trimester. For taste intensity, no consistent differences between pregnant women and controls were observed. However, over the course of pregnancy, salty intensity seemed to decrease, the intensity of other tastes did not change. During pregnancy, higher saltiness was liked more and salty snacks were consumed more, particularly in the second and/or third trimester. Drinks with lower sweetness were preferred and intake of sweet snacks was highest in the second trimester. Preference for sour and bitter did not seem to be affected.</p><p>Self-reports suggested that many women experienced some taste changes during pregnancy, while changes based on studies with real stimuli were limited.</p><p>Many women experienced a higher threshold for bitter perception in the first trimester, a preference for sweet snacks in the second trimester, and higher saltiness appetite in the second and third trimester of pregnancy, which may have nutritional consequences.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0000,"publicationDate":"2018-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","citationCount":"8","resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":null,"PeriodicalName":"Chemosensory Perception","FirstCategoryId":"3","ListUrlMain":"https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":null,"EPubDate":"","PubModel":"","JCR":"Q4","JCRName":"Neuroscience","Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Abstract
Studies of changes in taste threshold, perceived intensity, liking, and preference during pregnancy were reviewed, because such changes have the potential to negatively impact nutrient intake in pregnant women.
Medline and Web of Science were searched; eligibility was based on inclusion, exclusion, and quality criteria.
Fourteen articles were included: 5 reported taste thresholds, 8 taste intensity, and 13 liking/preferences. Articles addressed sweet, salty, sour, and bitter, not umami. Results of self-reported changes suggested that many women experienced some sort of alteration in taste during pregnancy. Studies with real stimuli demonstrated that the only consistent finding for taste thresholds was that pregnant women showed higher thresholds for bitterness in their first trimester. For taste intensity, no consistent differences between pregnant women and controls were observed. However, over the course of pregnancy, salty intensity seemed to decrease, the intensity of other tastes did not change. During pregnancy, higher saltiness was liked more and salty snacks were consumed more, particularly in the second and/or third trimester. Drinks with lower sweetness were preferred and intake of sweet snacks was highest in the second trimester. Preference for sour and bitter did not seem to be affected.
Self-reports suggested that many women experienced some taste changes during pregnancy, while changes based on studies with real stimuli were limited.
Many women experienced a higher threshold for bitter perception in the first trimester, a preference for sweet snacks in the second trimester, and higher saltiness appetite in the second and third trimester of pregnancy, which may have nutritional consequences.
我们回顾了怀孕期间味觉阈值、感知强度、喜好和偏好变化的研究,因为这些变化有可能对孕妇的营养摄入产生负面影响。检索Medline和Web of Science;入选资格基于纳入、排除和质量标准。包括14篇文章:5篇报道味觉阈值,8篇报道味觉强度,13篇报道喜欢/偏好。文章提到了甜、咸、酸、苦,而不是鲜味。自我报告的变化结果表明,许多女性在怀孕期间经历了某种味觉变化。对真实刺激的研究表明,在味觉阈值方面,唯一一致的发现是,孕妇在妊娠的前三个月对苦味的阈值更高。对于味觉强度,在孕妇和对照组之间没有观察到一致的差异。然而,在怀孕期间,咸的强度似乎降低了,其他口味的强度没有改变。在怀孕期间,人们更喜欢高盐,咸的零食也吃得更多,尤其是在怀孕的第二和/或第三个月。低甜度的饮料是首选,甜零食的摄入量在妊娠中期是最高的。对酸味和苦味的偏好似乎没有受到影响。自我报告表明,许多女性在怀孕期间经历了一些味觉变化,而基于真实刺激的研究的变化是有限的。许多女性在怀孕的前三个月对苦味的感知阈值更高,在怀孕的第二个三个月更喜欢吃甜食,在怀孕的第二个和第三个三个月更喜欢吃咸的东西,这可能会对营养产生影响。
期刊介绍:
Coverage in Chemosensory Perception includes animal work with implications for human phenomena and explores the following areas:
Identification of chemicals producing sensory response;
Identification of sensory response associated with chemicals;
Human in vivo response to chemical stimuli;
Human in vitro response to chemical stimuli;
Neuroimaging of chemosensory function;
Neurological processing of chemoreception;
Chemoreception mechanisms;
Psychophysics of chemoperception;
Trigeminal function;
Multisensory perception;
Contextual effect on chemoperception;
Behavioral response to chemical stimuli;
Physiological factors affecting and contributing to chemoperception;
Flavor and hedonics;
Memory and chemoperception.