首页 > 最新文献

Frontiers in Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Structurally unjust: how lay beliefs about racism relate to responses to racial inequality in the criminal legal system 结构上的不公正:非专业人士对种族主义的看法与应对刑事法律制度中种族不平等现象的关系
Pub Date : 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2024.1332527
Julian M. Rucker, Ajua Duker, J. Richeson
Racial inequality has been a persistent component of American society since its inception. The present research investigates how lay beliefs about the nature of racism—as primarily caused by prejudiced individuals or, rather, to structural factors (i.e., policies, institutional practices) that disadvantage members of marginalized racial groups—predict reactions to evidence of racial inequality in the criminal legal system (Studies 1–3). Specifically, the current research suggests that holding a more structural (vs. interpersonal) view of racism predicts a greater tendency to perceive racial inequality in criminal legal outcomes. Moreover, White Americans' lay beliefs regarding racism, coupled with their general degree of preference for societal hierarchy, predict support for policies that would impact disparities in the U.S. prison population. Together, this work suggests that an appreciation of structural racism plays an important role in how people perceive and respond to racial inequality.
自美国社会建立以来,种族不平等一直是其顽固的组成部分。本研究调查了外行人对种族主义性质的看法--主要是由有偏见的个人造成的,还是由不利于边缘化种族群体成员的结构性因素(即政策、制度惯例)造成的--如何预测对刑事法律系统中种族不平等证据的反应(研究1-3)。具体而言,目前的研究表明,持有结构性(相对于人际关系)种族主义观点的人更倾向于认为刑事法律结果中存在种族不平等。此外,美国白人对种族主义的非专业信念,加上他们对社会等级制度的普遍偏好程度,也预示着他们对影响美国监狱人口差异的政策的支持程度。总之,这项研究表明,对结构性种族主义的认识在人们如何看待和应对种族不平等方面发挥着重要作用。
{"title":"Structurally unjust: how lay beliefs about racism relate to responses to racial inequality in the criminal legal system","authors":"Julian M. Rucker, Ajua Duker, J. Richeson","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2024.1332527","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2024.1332527","url":null,"abstract":"Racial inequality has been a persistent component of American society since its inception. The present research investigates how lay beliefs about the nature of racism—as primarily caused by prejudiced individuals or, rather, to structural factors (i.e., policies, institutional practices) that disadvantage members of marginalized racial groups—predict reactions to evidence of racial inequality in the criminal legal system (Studies 1–3). Specifically, the current research suggests that holding a more structural (vs. interpersonal) view of racism predicts a greater tendency to perceive racial inequality in criminal legal outcomes. Moreover, White Americans' lay beliefs regarding racism, coupled with their general degree of preference for societal hierarchy, predict support for policies that would impact disparities in the U.S. prison population. Together, this work suggests that an appreciation of structural racism plays an important role in how people perceive and respond to racial inequality.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"26 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141353831","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Quantifying generalized trust in individuals and counties using language 用语言量化对个人和国家的普遍信任
Pub Date : 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2024.1384262
Salvatore Giorgi, Jason Jeffrey Jones, Anneke Buffone, J. Eichstaedt, P. Crutchley, D. Yaden, Jeanette Elstein, Mohammadzaman Zamani, Jennifer Kregor, Laura K Smith, Martin E. P. Seligman, Margaret L. Kern, L. Ungar, H. A. Schwartz
Trust is predictive of civic cooperation and economic growth. Recently, the U.S. public has demonstrated increased partisan division and a surveyed decline in trust in institutions. There is a need to quantify individual and community levels of trust unobtrusively and at scale. Using observations of language across more than 16,000 Facebook users, along with their self-reported generalized trust score, we develop and evaluate a language-based assessment of generalized trust. We then apply the assessment to more than 1.6 billion geotagged tweets collected between 2009 and 2015 and derive estimates of trust across 2,041 U.S. counties. We find generalized trust was associated with more affiliative words (love, we, and friends) and less angry words (hate and stupid) but only had a weak association with social words primarily driven by strong negative associations with general othering terms (“they” and “people”). At the county level, associations with the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and Gallup surveys suggest that people in high-trust counties were physically healthier and more satisfied with their community and their lives. Our study demonstrates that generalized trust levels can be estimated from language as a low-cost, unobtrusive method to monitor variations in trust in large populations.
信任能够促进公民合作和经济增长。近来,美国公众的党派分歧加剧,对机构的信任度也在下降。因此,有必要对个人和社区的信任度进行大规模、不显眼的量化。通过观察 16,000 多名 Facebook 用户的语言及其自我报告的普遍信任分数,我们开发并评估了一种基于语言的普遍信任评估方法。然后,我们将该评估应用于 2009 年至 2015 年间收集的超过 16 亿条带有地理标记的推文,并得出美国 2041 个县的信任度估计值。我们发现广义信任与更多的关联词(爱、我们和朋友)和较少的愤怒词(恨和愚蠢)相关,但与社交词的关联较弱,这主要是由与一般他者化词语("他们 "和 "人们")的强烈负面关联驱动的。在县一级,与疾病控制和预防中心(CDC)和盖洛普调查的关联表明,高信任度县的人们身体更健康,对社区和生活更满意。我们的研究表明,可以从语言中估算出普遍信任度,这是一种低成本、无干扰的方法,可以监测大量人口中信任度的变化。
{"title":"Quantifying generalized trust in individuals and counties using language","authors":"Salvatore Giorgi, Jason Jeffrey Jones, Anneke Buffone, J. Eichstaedt, P. Crutchley, D. Yaden, Jeanette Elstein, Mohammadzaman Zamani, Jennifer Kregor, Laura K Smith, Martin E. P. Seligman, Margaret L. Kern, L. Ungar, H. A. Schwartz","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2024.1384262","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2024.1384262","url":null,"abstract":"Trust is predictive of civic cooperation and economic growth. Recently, the U.S. public has demonstrated increased partisan division and a surveyed decline in trust in institutions. There is a need to quantify individual and community levels of trust unobtrusively and at scale. Using observations of language across more than 16,000 Facebook users, along with their self-reported generalized trust score, we develop and evaluate a language-based assessment of generalized trust. We then apply the assessment to more than 1.6 billion geotagged tweets collected between 2009 and 2015 and derive estimates of trust across 2,041 U.S. counties. We find generalized trust was associated with more affiliative words (love, we, and friends) and less angry words (hate and stupid) but only had a weak association with social words primarily driven by strong negative associations with general othering terms (“they” and “people”). At the county level, associations with the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and Gallup surveys suggest that people in high-trust counties were physically healthier and more satisfied with their community and their lives. Our study demonstrates that generalized trust levels can be estimated from language as a low-cost, unobtrusive method to monitor variations in trust in large populations.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":" 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141371266","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The limited impact of adverse experiences on worldviews and ideologies 不利经历对世界观和意识形态的影响有限
Pub Date : 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2024.1375527
Felipe Vilanova, Damiao Soares Almeida-Segundo, Pablo Borges Moura, Felicia Pratto, Angelo Brandelli Costa
The impact of adverse experiences on clinical symptoms has been consistently demonstrated, but their impact on ideologies and worldviews has been rarely tested empirically. It has been long assumed that threatening experiences increase Dangerous World Beliefs (DWB) and Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), whereas scarcity experiences increase Competitive World Beliefs (CWB) and Social Dominance Orientation (SDO). Here we assess whether self-reports of these adverse experiences are associated with clinical symptoms, worldviews and ideologies across two distinct studies (Ntotal = 1,108). Study 1 comprised Brazilian youth (13–17 years old) and results indicated that adverse experiences are consistently associated with depression, anxiety and stress but only marginally associated with DWB, RWA, CWB and SDO. Study 2 comprised male prisoners with a higher degree of adverse experiences and similar results were observed, as adverse experiences were mostly unrelated to worldviews and ideologies. Comprehensively, this research challenges the theoretical foundations of worldviews and ideological development, posing questions to the existing models and advocating for new frameworks that promote a shift from models grounded in clinical assumptions to frameworks focusing on social influences.
不良经历对临床症状的影响已被不断证实,但其对意识形态和世界观的影响却很少被实证检验。长期以来,人们一直认为威胁性经历会增加危险世界信念(DWB)和右翼专制主义(RWA),而稀缺性经历会增加竞争世界信念(CWB)和社会支配取向(SDO)。在此,我们通过两项不同的研究(总人数 = 1,108)来评估这些不良经历的自我报告是否与临床症状、世界观和意识形态相关。研究 1 的对象是巴西青少年(13-17 岁),结果表明,不良经历始终与抑郁、焦虑和压力有关,但与 DWB、RWA、CWB 和 SDO 的关系不大。研究 2 的对象是不良经历程度较高的男性囚犯,也观察到了类似的结果,因为不良经历大多与世界观和意识形态无关。总之,这项研究对世界观和意识形态发展的理论基础提出了挑战,对现有模型提出了质疑,并倡导建立新的框架,促进从基于临床假设的模型向注重社会影响的框架转变。
{"title":"The limited impact of adverse experiences on worldviews and ideologies","authors":"Felipe Vilanova, Damiao Soares Almeida-Segundo, Pablo Borges Moura, Felicia Pratto, Angelo Brandelli Costa","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2024.1375527","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2024.1375527","url":null,"abstract":"The impact of adverse experiences on clinical symptoms has been consistently demonstrated, but their impact on ideologies and worldviews has been rarely tested empirically. It has been long assumed that threatening experiences increase Dangerous World Beliefs (DWB) and Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), whereas scarcity experiences increase Competitive World Beliefs (CWB) and Social Dominance Orientation (SDO). Here we assess whether self-reports of these adverse experiences are associated with clinical symptoms, worldviews and ideologies across two distinct studies (Ntotal = 1,108). Study 1 comprised Brazilian youth (13–17 years old) and results indicated that adverse experiences are consistently associated with depression, anxiety and stress but only marginally associated with DWB, RWA, CWB and SDO. Study 2 comprised male prisoners with a higher degree of adverse experiences and similar results were observed, as adverse experiences were mostly unrelated to worldviews and ideologies. Comprehensively, this research challenges the theoretical foundations of worldviews and ideological development, posing questions to the existing models and advocating for new frameworks that promote a shift from models grounded in clinical assumptions to frameworks focusing on social influences.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"31 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140694047","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Discriminatory, yet socially accepted? Targets’ perceptions of subtle and blatant expressions of ethno-racial prejudice 歧视,但又被社会所接受?目标群体对民族-种族偏见的微妙和公然表达方式的看法
Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2024.1343514
Franziska A. Stanke, Niclas Kuper, Karolina Fetz, Gerald Echterhoff
Extant research has studied prejudice expressions mainly from the majority perspective. We examined whether prejudice expressions conceptualized as subtle (vs. blatant) are perceived differently by their targets.Participants who self-identified as potential targets of ethno-racial or religious prejudice (e.g., anti-Muslim, anti-Asian, anti-Arab, anti-Slavic, and anti-Black prejudice), answered questions about verbal expressions of prejudice taken from the subtle and blatant prejudice scales in an online survey.Items of the subtle (vs. blatant) prejudice scale were rated as more socially accepted and less discriminatory, elicited less negative emotions, and were reported to be experienced more often. Subtle expressions of prejudice were not more familiar to participants than blatant ones. Remarkably, blatant prejudice expressions were also perceived as relatively socially accepted and subtle prejudices as relatively discriminatory, as indicated by mean ratings above the scale midpoint. Lower discrimination ratings of subtle (vs. blatant) prejudice expressions were mainly due to perceptions of expressions exaggerating cultural differences. Exploratory analyses indicate that participants who reported more (vs. less) frequent overall exposure to the prejudice expressions perceived subtle and blatant stimuli as similarly discriminatory. This finding is compatible with the idea that individuals factor their personal experiences with prejudice into their assessments of discrimination.We discuss implications for interventions, especially regarding the social acceptability of blatant prejudice and promoting awareness of the discriminatory impact of subtle prejudice.
现有研究主要从多数人的角度研究偏见的表现形式。我们研究了被概念化为微妙(与明目张胆)的偏见表达方式是否会被目标群体视为不同的表达方式、在一项在线调查中,自我认同为民族-种族或宗教偏见(如反穆斯林、反亚洲人、反阿拉伯人、反斯拉夫人和反黑人偏见)潜在目标的参与者回答了有关偏见的口头表达的问题,这些问题来自微妙和明显偏见量表。参与者并不比明目张胆的偏见更熟悉微妙的偏见表现形式。值得注意的是,明目张胆的偏见表现形式也被认为是相对被社会接受的,而微妙的偏见表现形式则被认为是相对具有歧视性的,这表现在平均评分高于量表中点。对微妙的(相对于明显的)偏见表现形式的歧视评分较低,主要是由于人们认为这些表现形式夸大了文化差异。探索性分析表明,那些表示总体上接触偏见表现的频率较高(与较低)的受试者认为微妙的和明显的刺激具有类似的歧视性。我们讨论了干预措施的意义,特别是关于社会对公然偏见的可接受性以及提高对微妙偏见的歧视性影响的认识。
{"title":"Discriminatory, yet socially accepted? Targets’ perceptions of subtle and blatant expressions of ethno-racial prejudice","authors":"Franziska A. Stanke, Niclas Kuper, Karolina Fetz, Gerald Echterhoff","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2024.1343514","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2024.1343514","url":null,"abstract":"Extant research has studied prejudice expressions mainly from the majority perspective. We examined whether prejudice expressions conceptualized as subtle (vs. blatant) are perceived differently by their targets.Participants who self-identified as potential targets of ethno-racial or religious prejudice (e.g., anti-Muslim, anti-Asian, anti-Arab, anti-Slavic, and anti-Black prejudice), answered questions about verbal expressions of prejudice taken from the subtle and blatant prejudice scales in an online survey.Items of the subtle (vs. blatant) prejudice scale were rated as more socially accepted and less discriminatory, elicited less negative emotions, and were reported to be experienced more often. Subtle expressions of prejudice were not more familiar to participants than blatant ones. Remarkably, blatant prejudice expressions were also perceived as relatively socially accepted and subtle prejudices as relatively discriminatory, as indicated by mean ratings above the scale midpoint. Lower discrimination ratings of subtle (vs. blatant) prejudice expressions were mainly due to perceptions of expressions exaggerating cultural differences. Exploratory analyses indicate that participants who reported more (vs. less) frequent overall exposure to the prejudice expressions perceived subtle and blatant stimuli as similarly discriminatory. This finding is compatible with the idea that individuals factor their personal experiences with prejudice into their assessments of discrimination.We discuss implications for interventions, especially regarding the social acceptability of blatant prejudice and promoting awareness of the discriminatory impact of subtle prejudice.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"31 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140714553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond close relationships: the positive effects of group relationships and group identification on health 超越亲密关系:群体关系和群体认同对健康的积极影响
Pub Date : 2024-01-22 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2024.1310755
Cristina Camilo, Maria Luísa Lima, Rita Moura, Fátima Quintal, Maria Palacin-Lois
It is well-known that social relationships positively impact health, but the direct and indirect ways these lead to better health outcomes remain understudied. Stemming from the concept of multiple levels of analysis, we address this problem by disentangling the links between different types of social relationships and health (including physical health, mental health, and well-being), and comparing the effects of multiple mediators based on two levels of analysis: interpersonal and group.We propose a model that uses loneliness and social support as proxies of personal ties (mediators between close relationships and health), and social integration and multiple identities as proxies of group ties (mediators between group relationships and health). Likewise, we also propose a moderating effect of group identification on the association between group ties and health. We collected 848 responses through a self-report online survey and used structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis to assess the proposed model.Data yielded that close relationships are stronger predictors of better health outcomes than group relationships. Yet, more importantly, the results also provide strong evidence for the positive effects of the group-level variables. The effect of group relationships on health is significantly positive, and this association is stronger for those who identify more with their group.Our findings are closely aligned with the notion that increasing and improving people's social network is an effective way to contribute to better social determinants of health, while demonstrating that it is important to consider which type of relationship is being fostered.
众所周知,社会关系会对健康产生积极影响,但人们对社会关系如何直接或间接地改善健康状况仍缺乏深入研究。基于多层次分析的概念,我们通过分解不同类型的社会关系与健康(包括身体健康、心理健康和幸福感)之间的联系来解决这一问题,并根据人际关系和群体关系这两个分析层次来比较多种中介因素的影响。我们提出了一个模型,将孤独感和社会支持作为个人关系的替代物(亲密关系与健康之间的中介因素),将社会融合和多重身份作为群体关系的替代物(群体关系与健康之间的中介因素)。同样,我们还提出了群体认同对群体联系与健康之间关系的调节作用。我们通过自我报告在线调查收集了 848 份回复,并使用结构方程建模(SEM)分析来评估所提出的模型。数据显示,亲密关系比群体关系更能预测更好的健康结果。然而,更重要的是,研究结果还有力地证明了群体层面变量的积极影响。我们的研究结果与增加和改善人们的社会网络是促进更好的健康社会决定因素的有效方法这一观点密切相关,同时也证明了考虑促进哪种类型的关系是非常重要的。
{"title":"Beyond close relationships: the positive effects of group relationships and group identification on health","authors":"Cristina Camilo, Maria Luísa Lima, Rita Moura, Fátima Quintal, Maria Palacin-Lois","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2024.1310755","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2024.1310755","url":null,"abstract":"It is well-known that social relationships positively impact health, but the direct and indirect ways these lead to better health outcomes remain understudied. Stemming from the concept of multiple levels of analysis, we address this problem by disentangling the links between different types of social relationships and health (including physical health, mental health, and well-being), and comparing the effects of multiple mediators based on two levels of analysis: interpersonal and group.We propose a model that uses loneliness and social support as proxies of personal ties (mediators between close relationships and health), and social integration and multiple identities as proxies of group ties (mediators between group relationships and health). Likewise, we also propose a moderating effect of group identification on the association between group ties and health. We collected 848 responses through a self-report online survey and used structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis to assess the proposed model.Data yielded that close relationships are stronger predictors of better health outcomes than group relationships. Yet, more importantly, the results also provide strong evidence for the positive effects of the group-level variables. The effect of group relationships on health is significantly positive, and this association is stronger for those who identify more with their group.Our findings are closely aligned with the notion that increasing and improving people's social network is an effective way to contribute to better social determinants of health, while demonstrating that it is important to consider which type of relationship is being fostered.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139609205","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural variation in age perceptions and developmental transitions 年龄认知和发展过渡的文化差异
Pub Date : 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2023.1283643
Michelle E. Vargas, Alejandro Carrillo, H. Giasson, W. Chopik
As people age, they increasingly report feeling younger than their actual age and “push off” when they think older adulthood starts, presumably to create and maintain psychological distance from the stigma of being an older adult. However, to date, such age perceptions and attitudes have mostly been studied in Western cultures (e.g., the United States and Europe). However, cultures vary in their perception of older adulthood and aging, suggesting that the extent to which people engage in these distancing processes might differ across cultures. In the current study, we examined age differences in age perceptions and perceived developmental transitions (e.g., when does someone move from middle age to older adulthood?) in 13 countries with over one million people total. We found that age-group distancing was present in each country but that this pattern was less pronounced in South Korea. Results are discussed in the context of cross-cultural variation in aging attitudes and the mechanisms that give rise to variation in age perceptions.
随着年龄的增长,人们越来越多地表示感觉自己比实际年龄年轻,并在他们认为老年期开始时 "推后",这大概是为了与老年期的耻辱感保持心理距离。然而,迄今为止,对这种年龄认知和态度的研究主要集中在西方文化(如美国和欧洲)。然而,不同文化对老年期和老龄化的看法各不相同,这表明不同文化背景下的人们参与这些疏远过程的程度也可能不同。在本研究中,我们考察了 13 个国家(总人口超过 100 万)在年龄感知和感知发展过渡(例如,一个人何时从中年进入老年?我们发现,每个国家都存在年龄组疏远现象,但这种模式在韩国不太明显。研究结果将结合老龄化态度的跨文化差异以及导致年龄认知差异的机制进行讨论。
{"title":"Cultural variation in age perceptions and developmental transitions","authors":"Michelle E. Vargas, Alejandro Carrillo, H. Giasson, W. Chopik","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2023.1283643","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2023.1283643","url":null,"abstract":"As people age, they increasingly report feeling younger than their actual age and “push off” when they think older adulthood starts, presumably to create and maintain psychological distance from the stigma of being an older adult. However, to date, such age perceptions and attitudes have mostly been studied in Western cultures (e.g., the United States and Europe). However, cultures vary in their perception of older adulthood and aging, suggesting that the extent to which people engage in these distancing processes might differ across cultures. In the current study, we examined age differences in age perceptions and perceived developmental transitions (e.g., when does someone move from middle age to older adulthood?) in 13 countries with over one million people total. We found that age-group distancing was present in each country but that this pattern was less pronounced in South Korea. Results are discussed in the context of cross-cultural variation in aging attitudes and the mechanisms that give rise to variation in age perceptions.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"32 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139382622","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grand challenge: social psychology without hubris 巨大挑战:不自大的社会心理学
Pub Date : 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.3389/frsps.2023.1283272
John T. Jost
In this editorial, the Founding Field Chief Editor of Frontiers in Social Psychology expresses several ideas about the past, present, and possible future of social psychology, seeking to explain we need social psychology, why we need a new journal in social psychology, and what kind of journal in social psychology we need. The Editor argues for a rich, humanistic, interdisciplinary, philosophically informed social psychology devoted to addressing social problems in the illustrious traditions of John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, Gordon Allport, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, Morton Deutsch, and others. He suggests that disciplinary “crises” of practicality, historicity, and replicability may be more interconnected than is generally recognized. The Editor advocates a non-hubristic, theory-driven, multi-leveled analysis of human behavior that attends to both subjective and objective aspects of social, cultural, economic, and political contexts. Editorial priorities of the new journal include scientific rigor, social relevance, and intellectual humility.
在这篇社论中,《社会心理学前沿》的创刊主编表达了关于社会心理学的过去、现在和可能的未来的一些观点,试图解释我们需要社会心理学,为什么我们需要一本新的社会心理学期刊,以及我们需要什么样的社会心理学期刊。编者主张建立一个丰富的、人文的、跨学科的、哲学的社会心理学,致力于解决约翰-杜威(John Dewey)、库尔特-勒温(Kurt Lewin)、戈登-奥尔波特(Gordon Allport)、穆扎费尔-谢里夫(Muzafer Sherif)、所罗门-阿什(Solomon Asch)、莫顿-多伊奇(Morton Deutsch)等人的杰出传统中的社会问题。他认为,实用性、历史性和可复制性等学科 "危机 "之间的相互联系可能比人们普遍认识到的要多。编辑主张对人类行为进行非枢纽、理论驱动、多层次的分析,同时关注社会、文化、经济和政治背景的主观和客观方面。新期刊的编辑重点包括科学严谨性、社会相关性和知识谦逊性。
{"title":"Grand challenge: social psychology without hubris","authors":"John T. Jost","doi":"10.3389/frsps.2023.1283272","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/frsps.2023.1283272","url":null,"abstract":"In this editorial, the Founding Field Chief Editor of Frontiers in Social Psychology expresses several ideas about the past, present, and possible future of social psychology, seeking to explain we need social psychology, why we need a new journal in social psychology, and what kind of journal in social psychology we need. The Editor argues for a rich, humanistic, interdisciplinary, philosophically informed social psychology devoted to addressing social problems in the illustrious traditions of John Dewey, Kurt Lewin, Gordon Allport, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch, Morton Deutsch, and others. He suggests that disciplinary “crises” of practicality, historicity, and replicability may be more interconnected than is generally recognized. The Editor advocates a non-hubristic, theory-driven, multi-leveled analysis of human behavior that attends to both subjective and objective aspects of social, cultural, economic, and political contexts. Editorial priorities of the new journal include scientific rigor, social relevance, and intellectual humility.","PeriodicalId":503123,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Social Psychology","volume":"26 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139382693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Frontiers in Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1