首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Brain Research最新文献

英文 中文
Visual magnocellular and structure from motion perceptual deficits in a neurodevelopmental model of dorsal stream function 背流功能的神经发育模型中运动知觉缺陷的视觉大细胞和结构
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.005
Mafalda Mendes , Fátima Silva , Luísa Simões , Marta Jorge , Jorge Saraiva , Miguel Castelo-Branco

Williams syndrome (WS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder of genetic origin that has been used as a model to understand visual cognition. We have investigated early deficits in the afferent magnocellular pathway and their relation to abnormal visual dorsal processing in WS. A spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity task that is known to selectively activate that pathway was used in six WS subjects. Additionally, we have compared visual performance in 2D and 3D motion integration tasks. A novel 3D motion coherence task (using spheres with unpredictable axis of rotation) was used in order to investigate possible impairment of occipitoparietal areas that are known to be involved in 3D structure from motion (SFM) perception. We have found a significant involvement of low-level magnocellular maps in WS as assessed by the contrast sensitivity task. On the contrary, no significant differences were observed between WS and the control groups in the 2D motion integration tasks. However, all WS subjects were significantly impaired in the 3D SFM task. Our findings suggest that magnocellular damage may occur in addition to dorsal stream deficits in these patients. They are also consistent with recently described genetic and neuroanatomic abnormalities in retinotopic visual areas. Finally, selective SFM coherence deficits support the proposal that there is a specific pathway in the dorsal stream that is involved in motion processing of 3D surfaces, which seems to be impaired in this disorder.

威廉姆斯综合征(WS)是一种遗传来源的神经发育障碍,已被用作理解视觉认知的模型。我们研究了传入大细胞通路的早期缺陷及其与WS中视觉背侧加工异常的关系。在6名WS受试者中使用了一项已知可选择性激活该通路的时空对比敏感性任务。此外,我们还比较了2D和3D运动集成任务的视觉表现。一种新颖的3D运动一致性任务(使用具有不可预测旋转轴的球体)用于研究已知参与3D运动结构(SFM)感知的枕顶叶区域可能的损伤。我们发现通过对比敏感度任务评估的低水平巨细胞图在WS中的显著参与。相反,在2D运动整合任务中,WS组与对照组之间没有显著差异。然而,所有WS受试者在3D SFM任务中都明显受损。我们的研究结果表明,在这些患者中,除了背流缺陷外,还可能发生大细胞损伤。它们也与最近描述的视网膜异位视觉区域的遗传和神经解剖学异常一致。最后,选择性SFM一致性缺陷支持了背流中参与3D表面运动加工的特定通路的建议,该通路似乎在这种疾病中受损。
{"title":"Visual magnocellular and structure from motion perceptual deficits in a neurodevelopmental model of dorsal stream function","authors":"Mafalda Mendes ,&nbsp;Fátima Silva ,&nbsp;Luísa Simões ,&nbsp;Marta Jorge ,&nbsp;Jorge Saraiva ,&nbsp;Miguel Castelo-Branco","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Williams syndrome (WS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder of genetic origin that has been used as a model to understand visual cognition. We have investigated early deficits in the afferent magnocellular pathway and their relation to abnormal visual dorsal processing in WS. A spatiotemporal contrast sensitivity task that is known to selectively activate that pathway was used in six WS subjects. Additionally, we have compared visual performance in 2D and 3D motion integration tasks. A novel 3D motion coherence task (using spheres with unpredictable axis of rotation) was used in order to investigate possible impairment of occipitoparietal areas that are known to be involved in 3D structure from motion (SFM) perception. We have found a significant involvement of low-level magnocellular maps in WS as assessed by the contrast sensitivity task. On the contrary, no significant differences were observed between WS and the control groups in the 2D motion integration tasks. However, all WS subjects were significantly impaired in the 3D SFM task. Our findings suggest that magnocellular damage may occur in addition to </span>dorsal stream deficits in these patients. They are also consistent with recently described genetic and neuroanatomic abnormalities in retinotopic visual areas. Finally, selective SFM coherence deficits support the proposal that there is a specific pathway in the dorsal stream that is involved in motion processing of 3D surfaces, which seems to be impaired in this disorder.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 788-798"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25675262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 35
Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses 外向性和多巴胺基因的个体差异预测神经奖励反应
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018
Michael X. Cohen , Jennifer Young , Jong-Min Baek , Christopher Kessler , Charan Ranganath

Psychologists have linked the personality trait extraversion both to differences in reward sensitivity and to dopamine functioning, but little is known about how these differences are reflected in the functioning of the brain's dopaminergic neural reward system. Here, we show that individual differences in extraversion and the presence of the A1 allele on the dopamine D2 receptor gene predict activation magnitudes in the brain's reward system during a gambling task. In two functional MRI experiments, participants probabilistically received rewards either immediately following a behavioral response (Study 1) or after a 7.5 s anticipation period (Study 2). Although group activation maps revealed anticipation- and reward-related activations in the reward system, individual differences in extraversion and the presence of the D2 Taq1A allele predicted a significant amount of inter-subject variability in the magnitudes of reward-related, but not anticipation-related, activations. These results demonstrate a link between stable differences in personality, genetics, and brain functioning.

心理学家将性格外向性与奖励敏感性和多巴胺功能的差异联系起来,但对于这些差异如何反映在大脑多巴胺能神经奖励系统的功能中,人们知之甚少。在这里,我们展示了外向性的个体差异和多巴胺D2受体基因A1等位基因的存在,预测了在赌博任务中大脑奖励系统的激活程度。在两个功能性MRI实验中,参与者可能会在行为反应后立即获得奖励(研究1)或在7.5 s的预期期后获得奖励(研究2)。尽管群体激活图揭示了奖励系统中与预期和奖励相关的激活,但外向性的个体差异和D2 Taq1A等位基因的存在预测了奖励相关的程度的显著的主体间差异。但不是与预期相关的激活。这些结果表明,性格、基因和大脑功能的稳定差异之间存在联系。
{"title":"Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses","authors":"Michael X. Cohen ,&nbsp;Jennifer Young ,&nbsp;Jong-Min Baek ,&nbsp;Christopher Kessler ,&nbsp;Charan Ranganath","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Psychologists have linked the personality trait extraversion both to differences in reward sensitivity and to dopamine functioning, but little is known about how these differences are reflected in the functioning of the brain's </span>dopaminergic neural reward system. Here, we show that individual differences in extraversion and the presence of the A1 allele on the </span>dopamine D2 receptor gene predict activation magnitudes in the brain's reward system during a gambling task. In two functional MRI experiments, participants probabilistically received rewards either immediately following a behavioral response (Study 1) or after a 7.5 s anticipation period (Study 2). Although group activation maps revealed anticipation- and reward-related activations in the reward system, individual differences in extraversion and the presence of the D2 Taq1A allele predicted a significant amount of inter-subject variability in the magnitudes of reward-related, but not anticipation-related, activations. These results demonstrate a link between stable differences in personality, genetics, and brain functioning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 851-861"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.018","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25692473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 264
Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus- and error-related processing during a Go/No-Go Task 在“去”/“不去”任务中,饮酒会损害刺激和错误相关的处理
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.009
Craig Easdon , Aaron Izenberg , Maria L. Armilio , He Yu , Claude Alain

Alcohol consumption has been shown to increase the number of errors in tasks that require a high degree of cognitive control, such as a go/no-go task. The alcohol-related decline in performance may be related to difficulties in maintaining attention on the task at hand and/or deficits in inhibiting a prepotent response. To test these two accounts, we investigated the effects of alcohol on stimulus- and response-locked evoked potentials recorded during a go/no-go task that involved the withholding of key presses to rare targets. All participants performed the task prior to drinking and were then assigned randomly to either a control, low-dose, or moderate-dose treatment. Both doses of alcohol increased the number of errors relative to alcohol-free performance. Success in withholding a prepotent response was associated with an early-enhanced stimulus-locked negativity at inferior parietal sites, which was delayed when participants failed to inhibit the motor command. Moreover, low and moderate doses of alcohol reduced N170 and P3 amplitudes during go, no-go, and error trials. In comparison with the correct responses, errors generated large response-locked negative (Ne) and positive (Pe) waves at central sites. Both doses of alcohol reduced the Ne amplitude whereas the Pe amplitude decreased only after moderate doses of alcohol. These results are consistent with the interpretation that behavioral disinhibition following alcohol consumption involved alcohol-induced deficits in maintaining and allocating attention thereby affecting the processing of incoming stimuli and the recognition that an errant response has been made.

研究表明,在一些需要高度认知控制的任务中,饮酒会增加出错的次数,比如去或不去的任务。与酒精相关的表现下降可能与在手头任务上保持注意力的困难和/或抑制阳性反应的缺陷有关。为了验证这两种说法,我们研究了酒精对刺激锁定和反应锁定的诱发电位的影响,这些诱发电位记录在一个“走”/“不走”任务中,该任务涉及对稀有目标暂停按键。所有参与者都在饮酒前完成了这项任务,然后随机分配到对照组、低剂量组或中剂量组。相对于不含酒精的表现,两种剂量的酒精都增加了错误的数量。成功抑制强性反应与下顶叶部位早期增强的刺激锁定负性有关,当参与者未能抑制运动命令时,这种负性被延迟。此外,在进行、不进行和错误试验中,低剂量和中等剂量的酒精降低了N170和P3的振幅。与正确的响应相比,错误在中心位置产生了大的响应锁定的负(Ne)和正(Pe)波。两种剂量的酒精都降低了Ne振幅,而只有中等剂量的酒精才降低了Pe振幅。这些结果与以下解释一致:饮酒后的行为去抑制涉及酒精引起的注意力维持和分配缺陷,从而影响对传入刺激的处理和对错误反应的认识。
{"title":"Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus- and error-related processing during a Go/No-Go Task","authors":"Craig Easdon ,&nbsp;Aaron Izenberg ,&nbsp;Maria L. Armilio ,&nbsp;He Yu ,&nbsp;Claude Alain","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.009","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.009","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Alcohol consumption has been shown to increase the number of errors in tasks that require a high degree of cognitive control, such as a go/no-go task. The alcohol-related decline in performance may be related to difficulties in maintaining attention on the task at hand and/or deficits in inhibiting a prepotent response. To test these two accounts, we investigated the effects of alcohol on stimulus- and response-locked evoked potentials recorded during a go/no-go task that involved the withholding of key presses to rare targets. All participants performed the task prior to drinking and were then assigned randomly to either a control, low-dose, or moderate-dose treatment. Both doses of alcohol increased the number of errors relative to alcohol-free performance. Success in withholding a prepotent response was associated with an early-enhanced stimulus-locked negativity at inferior parietal sites, which was delayed when participants failed to inhibit the motor command. Moreover, low and moderate doses of alcohol reduced N170 and P3 amplitudes during go, no-go, and error trials. In comparison with the correct responses, errors generated large response-locked negative (Ne) and positive (Pe) waves at central sites. Both doses of alcohol reduced the Ne amplitude whereas the Pe amplitude decreased only after moderate doses of alcohol. These results are consistent with the interpretation that behavioral disinhibition following alcohol consumption involved alcohol-induced deficits in maintaining and allocating attention thereby affecting the processing of incoming stimuli and the recognition that an errant response has been made.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 873-883"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25675261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 112
Attention and selection for predictive smooth pursuit eye movements 注意和选择预测平滑追求眼球运动
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.016
E. Poliakoff , C.J.S. Collins , G.R. Barnes

Humans cannot typically produce smooth eye movements in the absence of a moving stimulus. However, they can produce predictive smooth eye movements if they expect a target of a known velocity to reappear. Here, we observed that participants could extract velocity information from two simultaneously presented moving targets in order to produce a subsequent predictive smooth eye movement for one of the two targets. Subjects fixated a stationary cross during the presentation of two targets, moving rightward at different velocities. In the next presentation, a single target was presented, which participants tracked with their eyes. A static cue, presented 700 ms before the moving target, indicated which of the two targets would be presented. Predictive eye movements were of an appropriate velocity, even when participants did not know in advance which of the two targets would subsequently be cued. However, the scaling of predictive eye velocity was marginally less accurate in this divided attention condition than when participants knew the identity of the cued target in advance, or a single target was presented during fixation. In a second experiment, we found that the velocity cued on the previous trial had a greater effect than the uncued velocity on the current trial. The negligible effect of the uncued velocity indicates that participants were extremely effective at selectively reproducing one of two recently viewed velocities. However, other influences, such as past history, also affected predictive smooth eye movements.

在没有运动刺激的情况下,人类通常无法产生平滑的眼球运动。然而,如果他们预计一个已知速度的目标会再次出现,他们可以产生预测性的平滑眼球运动。在这里,我们观察到参与者可以从同时呈现的两个运动目标中提取速度信息,以便对两个目标中的一个产生随后的预测平滑眼球运动。在展示两个目标时,受试者盯着一个静止的十字,以不同的速度向右移动。在接下来的演示中,展示了一个单一的目标,参与者用眼睛跟踪它。在移动目标出现前700毫秒出现一个静态线索,表明将出现两个目标中的哪一个。即使参与者事先不知道两个目标中的哪一个随后会被提示,预测眼球运动的速度也是适当的。然而,在这种分散注意条件下,预测眼速的缩放精度略低于参与者事先知道提示目标的身份或在注视过程中呈现单个目标。在第二个实验中,我们发现先前试验中提示的速度比当前试验中未提示的速度有更大的影响。未提示速度的影响可以忽略不计,这表明参与者在选择性地再现最近看到的两个速度中的一个方面非常有效。然而,其他因素,如过去的历史,也会影响预测的眼球平滑运动。
{"title":"Attention and selection for predictive smooth pursuit eye movements","authors":"E. Poliakoff ,&nbsp;C.J.S. Collins ,&nbsp;G.R. Barnes","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.016","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.016","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Humans cannot typically produce smooth eye movements in the absence of a moving stimulus. However, they can produce predictive smooth eye movements if they expect a target of a known velocity to reappear. Here, we observed that participants could extract velocity information from two simultaneously presented moving targets in order to produce a subsequent predictive smooth eye movement for one of the two targets. Subjects fixated a stationary cross during the presentation of two targets, moving rightward at different velocities. In the next presentation, a single target was presented, which participants tracked with their eyes. A static cue, presented 700 ms before the moving target, indicated which of the two targets would be presented. Predictive eye movements were of an appropriate velocity, even when participants did not know in advance which of the two targets would subsequently be cued. However, the scaling of predictive eye velocity was marginally less accurate in this divided attention condition than when participants knew the identity of the cued target in advance, or a single target was presented during fixation. In a second experiment, we found that the velocity cued on the previous trial had a greater effect than the uncued velocity on the current trial. The negligible effect of the uncued velocity indicates that participants were extremely effective at selectively reproducing one of two recently viewed velocities. However, other influences, such as past history, also affected predictive smooth eye movements.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 688-700"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.016","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25651556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 20
Assessment of the effects of the cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitor rofecoxib on visuospatial learning and hippocampal cell death following kainate-induced seizures in the rat 环氧化酶-2抑制剂罗非昔布对卡因酸钠诱发癫痫大鼠视觉空间学习和海马细胞死亡影响的评估
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.017
Tina Kunz , Niklas Marklund , Lars Hillered , Ernst H. Oliw

Kainate-induced seizures result in hippocampal neurodegeneration and spatial learning deficits in rodents. Previous studies show that rofecoxib, a selective cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitor, protects against kainate-induced hippocampal cell death 3 days after seizures. Our aim was to determine whether rofecoxib attenuates visuospatial learning deficits and late neuronal death after kainate-induced seizures. Seizures were induced in Sprague–Dawley rats with kainic acid (10 mg/kg, i.p.). Eight hours later, animals received rofecoxib (10 mg/kg; n = 15) or vehicle (dimethylsulfoxide, n = 11). Animals were then treated daily for additional 2 or 9 days. Visuospatial learning was assessed in the Morris water maze (MWM) on days 5–9 after seizures. Seizure animals learned the MWM task significantly slower than non-seizure controls, but seizure animals showed higher swim speed (P < 0.05). Seizure animals receiving rofecoxib for 2 days showed no significant improvement in acquisition of the task compared to the vehicle group, even though mean latencies in the rofecoxib group were shorter from the third trial day onwards. This tendency was lost when rofecoxib was given for 9 days. TdT-mediated dUTP nick end labelling showed cell death in limbic structures 9 days after seizures. The time course of kainate-induced hippocampal cell death might be delayed by rofecoxib treatment, as the attenuation of cell death observed 3 days after seizures was no longer present after 9 days. We conclude that even though increasing evidence points to an injurious role of cyclooxygenase-2 products in acute brain injury processes, rofecoxib treatment failed to attenuate seizure-induced visuospatial learning deficits and the late phase of hippocampal neurodegeneration.

卡因酸诱发的癫痫发作导致啮齿动物海马神经变性和空间学习缺陷。先前的研究表明,罗非昔布是一种选择性环氧化酶-2抑制剂,可防止癫痫发作后3天海碱盐诱导的海马细胞死亡。我们的目的是确定罗非昔布是否减轻卡因酸钠诱发癫痫发作后的视觉空间学习缺陷和晚期神经元死亡。kainic酸(10 mg/kg, ig)诱导Sprague-Dawley大鼠癫痫发作。8小时后,动物接受罗非昔布(10 mg/kg;N = 15)或载体(二甲亚砜,N = 11)。然后每天给动物额外治疗2或9天。在癫痫发作后第5-9天用Morris水迷宫(MWM)评估视觉空间学习。癫痫动物学习MWM任务的速度明显慢于非癫痫动物,但癫痫动物表现出更高的游泳速度(P <0.05)。与车辆组相比,服用罗非昔布2天的癫痫发作动物在获得任务方面没有显着改善,尽管罗非昔布组的平均潜伏期从试验第三天开始较短。当给予罗非昔布9天后,这种倾向消失。tdt介导的dUTP缺口末端标记显示癫痫发作后9天边缘结构细胞死亡。罗非昔布治疗可能会延迟海钠盐诱导的海马细胞死亡的时间过程,因为癫痫发作后3天观察到的细胞死亡衰减在9天后不再存在。我们得出结论,尽管越来越多的证据表明环氧化酶-2产物在急性脑损伤过程中具有损伤作用,但罗非昔布治疗未能减轻癫痫引起的视觉空间学习缺陷和海马神经退行性变的晚期。
{"title":"Assessment of the effects of the cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitor rofecoxib on visuospatial learning and hippocampal cell death following kainate-induced seizures in the rat","authors":"Tina Kunz ,&nbsp;Niklas Marklund ,&nbsp;Lars Hillered ,&nbsp;Ernst H. Oliw","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.017","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.017","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Kainate-induced seizures result in hippocampal neurodegeneration<span> and spatial learning deficits in rodents. Previous studies show that rofecoxib, a selective cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitor, protects against kainate-induced hippocampal cell death 3 days after seizures. Our aim was to determine whether rofecoxib attenuates visuospatial learning deficits and late </span></span>neuronal death<span> after kainate-induced seizures. Seizures were induced in Sprague–Dawley rats with kainic acid (10 mg/kg, i.p.). Eight hours later, animals received rofecoxib (10 mg/kg; </span></span><em>n</em> = 15) or vehicle (dimethylsulfoxide, <em>n</em><span> = 11). Animals were then treated daily for additional 2 or 9 days. Visuospatial learning was assessed in the Morris water maze (MWM) on days 5–9 after seizures. Seizure animals learned the MWM task significantly slower than non-seizure controls, but seizure animals showed higher swim speed (</span><em>P</em> &lt; 0.05). Seizure animals receiving rofecoxib for 2 days showed no significant improvement in acquisition of the task compared to the vehicle group, even though mean latencies in the rofecoxib group were shorter from the third trial day onwards. This tendency was lost when rofecoxib was given for 9 days. TdT-mediated dUTP nick end labelling showed cell death in limbic structures 9 days after seizures. The time course of kainate-induced hippocampal cell death might be delayed by rofecoxib treatment, as the attenuation of cell death observed 3 days after seizures was no longer present after 9 days. We conclude that even though increasing evidence points to an injurious role of cyclooxygenase-2 products in acute brain injury processes, rofecoxib treatment failed to attenuate seizure-induced visuospatial learning deficits and the late phase of hippocampal neurodegeneration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 826-832"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.017","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25670032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Free colour illustations in the online version of articles 免费彩色插图在文章的在线版本
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/S0926-6410(05)00304-6
{"title":"Free colour illustations in the online version of articles","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/S0926-6410(05)00304-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(05)00304-6","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Page iv"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/S0926-6410(05)00304-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"137409550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Action selectivity in parietal and temporal cortex 顶叶和颞叶皮层的作用选择性
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.017
U. Noppeney, O. Josephs, S. Kiebel, K.J. Friston, C.J. Price

The sensory–action theory proposes that the neural substrates underlying action representations are related to a visuomotor action system encompassing the left ventral premotor cortex, the anterior intraparietal (AIP) and left posterior middle temporal gyrus (LPMT). Using fMRI, we demonstrate that semantic decisions on action, relative to non-action words, increased activation in the left AIP and LPMT irrespective of whether the words were presented in a written or spoken form. Left AIP and LPMT might thus play the role of amodal semantic regions that can be activated via auditory as well as visual input. Left AIP and LPMT did not distinguish between different types of actions such as hand actions and whole body movements, although a right STS region responded selectively to whole body movements.

感觉-动作理论认为,动作表征背后的神经基质与视觉运动动作系统有关,该系统包括左腹侧运动前皮层、顶叶前部(AIP)和左后颞中回(LPMT)。使用功能磁共振成像,我们证明了动作的语义决定,相对于非动作词,增加了左AIP和LPMT的激活,无论这些词是以书面形式还是口头形式呈现的。因此,左AIP和LPMT可能发挥了模态语义区域的作用,该区域可以通过听觉和视觉输入激活。尽管右侧STS区域对全身运动有选择性反应,但左侧的AIP和LPMT并不能区分不同类型的动作,如手部动作和全身运动。
{"title":"Action selectivity in parietal and temporal cortex","authors":"U. Noppeney,&nbsp;O. Josephs,&nbsp;S. Kiebel,&nbsp;K.J. Friston,&nbsp;C.J. Price","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.017","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.017","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The sensory–action theory proposes that the neural substrates<span> underlying action representations are related to a visuomotor action system encompassing the left ventral premotor cortex<span>, the anterior intraparietal (AIP) and left posterior middle temporal gyrus (LPMT). Using fMRI, we demonstrate that semantic decisions on action, relative to non-action words, increased activation in the left AIP and LPMT irrespective of whether the words were presented in a written or spoken form. Left AIP and LPMT might thus play the role of amodal semantic regions that can be activated via auditory as well as visual input. Left AIP and LPMT did not distinguish between different types of actions such as hand actions and whole body movements, although a right STS region responded selectively to whole body movements.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 641-649"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.017","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25651015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 113
Imagery of motor actions: Differential effects of kinesthetic and visual–motor mode of imagery in single-trial EEG 运动动作的意象:单次脑电图中动觉和视觉-运动意象模式的不同影响
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.014
Christa Neuper , Reinhold Scherer , Miriam Reiner , Gert Pfurtscheller

Single-trial motor imagery classification is an integral part of a number of brain–computer interface (BCI) systems. The possible significance of the kind of imagery, involving rather kinesthetic or visual representations of actions, was addressed using the following experimental conditions: kinesthetic motor imagery (MIK), visual–motor imagery (MIV), motor execution (ME) and observation of movement (OOM). Based on multi-channel EEG recordings in 14 right-handed participants, we applied a learning classifier, the distinction sensitive learning vector quantization (DSLVQ) to identify relevant features (i.e., frequency bands, electrode sites) for recognition of the respective mental states. For ME and OOM, the overall classification accuracies were about 80%. The rates obtained for MIK (67%) were better than the results of MIV (56%). Moreover, the focus of activity during kinesthetic imagery was found close to the sensorimotor hand area, whereas visual–motor imagery did not reveal a clear spatial pattern. Consequently, to improve motor-imagery-based BCI control, user training should emphasize kinesthetic experiences instead of visual representations of actions.

单次实验运动意象分类是许多脑机接口(BCI)系统的重要组成部分。这种涉及动作的动觉或视觉表征的意象的可能意义,是通过以下实验条件来解决的:动觉运动意象(MIK)、视觉运动意象(MIV)、运动执行(ME)和运动观察(OOM)。基于14名右利手被试的多通道脑电记录,我们采用区分敏感学习向量量化(DSLVQ)学习分类器来识别相关特征(即频带、电极位置)以识别各自的心理状态。对于ME和OOM,总体分类准确率约为80%。MIK的检出率(67%)优于MIV(56%)。此外,在动觉想象过程中,活动的焦点被发现靠近手部感觉运动区域,而视觉运动想象没有显示出清晰的空间模式。因此,为了改善基于运动图像的BCI控制,用户训练应该强调动觉体验,而不是动作的视觉表现。
{"title":"Imagery of motor actions: Differential effects of kinesthetic and visual–motor mode of imagery in single-trial EEG","authors":"Christa Neuper ,&nbsp;Reinhold Scherer ,&nbsp;Miriam Reiner ,&nbsp;Gert Pfurtscheller","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.014","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Single-trial motor imagery classification is an integral part of a number of brain–computer interface (BCI) systems. The possible significance of the kind of imagery, involving rather kinesthetic or visual representations of actions, was addressed using the following experimental conditions: kinesthetic motor imagery (MIK), visual–motor imagery (MIV), motor execution (ME) and observation of movement (OOM). Based on multi-channel EEG recordings in 14 right-handed participants, we applied a learning classifier, the distinction sensitive learning vector quantization (DSLVQ) to identify relevant features (i.e., frequency bands, electrode sites) for recognition of the respective mental states. For ME and OOM, the overall classification accuracies were about 80%. The rates obtained for MIK (67%) were better than the results of MIV (56%). Moreover, the focus of activity during kinesthetic imagery was found close to the sensorimotor hand area, whereas visual–motor imagery did not reveal a clear spatial pattern. Consequently, to improve motor-imagery-based BCI control, user training should emphasize kinesthetic experiences instead of visual representations of actions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 668-677"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.014","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25658654","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 678
Cerebral processes in mental transformations of body parts: Recognition prior to rotation 身体部位心理转换中的大脑过程:旋转前的识别
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.024
Leila S. Overney , Christoph M. Michel , Irina M. Harris , Alan J. Pegna

There is growing evidence that the visual processing of human body stimuli is particular and distinct from that of other objects. This is due to implicit knowledge of anatomical and biomechanical constraints of the human body. The question arises whether body stimuli in which biomechanical constraints are violated are processed in the same way as realistic bodies. This study investigated the neural mechanisms of anatomically plausible and implausible body stimuli. Event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded in healthy participants during mental rotation of body parts. Subjects were shown pictures of body parts or whole bodies in which one element (finger, arm) could be anatomically accurate or inaccurate (e.g., left forearm attached to right upper arm). Furthermore, the body parts were rotated in 7 different orientations, from 0° to 180° in 30° increments, resulting in some possible and some impossible positions of the body parts. Analysis of the 123-channel ERPs was carried out by determining the successive segments of stable map topographies and comparing them between conditions. A particular segment appeared in the case of anatomically impossible postures at 190–230 ms followed by a segment reflecting mental rotation at 310–380 ms. Anatomically implausible positions are thus detected at a very early stage, before mental rotation occurs. Source estimations derived from the topographic data indicated that left occipital, bilateral frontal and two medial areas were activated in the case of impossible postures, whereas left parietal regions were strongly activated during mental rotation. This result contrasts with mental rotation of objects, which is considered to be a right parietal process.

越来越多的证据表明,人体刺激的视觉处理是特殊的,与其他物体的视觉处理不同。这是由于对人体解剖和生物力学约束的隐性知识。问题是,违反生物力学约束的身体刺激是否以与现实身体相同的方式处理。本研究探讨了解剖学上合理和不合理的身体刺激的神经机制。在身体部位的心理旋转过程中,记录了健康参与者的事件相关电位(ERP)。研究人员向受试者展示身体部位或全身的图片,其中一个部位(手指、手臂)在解剖学上可能是准确的,也可能是不准确的(例如,左前臂附着在右上臂上)。此外,身体部位在7个不同的方向上旋转,从0°到180°,以30°的增量,产生一些可能的和一些不可能的身体部位的位置。通过确定稳定地图地形的连续片段并在不同条件下进行比较,对123通道erp进行了分析。在解剖上不可能的姿势出现在190-230 ms时的一个特定的节段,随后在310-380 ms时出现一个反映精神旋转的节段。因此,在心理旋转发生之前,解剖学上不合理的姿势在很早的阶段就被发现了。来自地形数据的来源估计表明,在不可能的姿势下,左枕叶、双侧额叶和两个内侧区域被激活,而在精神旋转时,左顶叶区域被强烈激活。这一结果与客体的心理旋转形成对比,后者被认为是一个右顶叶过程。
{"title":"Cerebral processes in mental transformations of body parts: Recognition prior to rotation","authors":"Leila S. Overney ,&nbsp;Christoph M. Michel ,&nbsp;Irina M. Harris ,&nbsp;Alan J. Pegna","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.024","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.024","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>There is growing evidence that the visual processing of human body stimuli is particular and distinct from that of other objects. This is due to implicit knowledge of anatomical and biomechanical constraints of the human body. The question arises whether body stimuli in which biomechanical constraints are violated are processed in the same way as realistic bodies. This study investigated the neural mechanisms of anatomically plausible and implausible body stimuli. Event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded in healthy participants during mental rotation of body parts. Subjects were shown pictures of body parts or whole bodies in which one element (finger, arm) could be anatomically accurate or inaccurate (e.g., left forearm attached to right upper arm). Furthermore, the body parts were rotated in 7 different orientations, from 0° to 180° in 30° increments, resulting in some possible and some impossible positions of the body parts. Analysis of the 123-channel ERPs was carried out by determining the successive segments of stable map topographies and comparing them between conditions. A particular segment appeared in the case of anatomically impossible postures at 190–230 ms followed by a segment reflecting mental rotation at 310–380 ms. Anatomically implausible positions are thus detected at a very early stage, before mental rotation occurs. Source estimations derived from the topographic data indicated that left occipital, bilateral frontal and two medial areas were activated in the case of impossible postures, whereas left parietal regions were strongly activated during mental rotation. This result contrasts with mental rotation of objects, which is considered to be a right parietal process.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 722-734"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.024","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25689185","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 42
On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes 内感受性意识、情绪体验和脑过程之间的关系
Pub Date : 2005-12-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019
Olga Pollatos , Wladimir Kirsch , Rainer Schandry

The perception of visceral signals plays a crucial role in many theories of emotions. The present study was designed to investigate the relationship between interoceptive awareness and emotion-related brain activity. 44 participants (16 male, 28 female) first underwent a heartbeat perception task and then were categorised either as good (n = 22) or poor heartbeat perceivers (n = 22). A total of 60 different pictures (pleasant, unpleasant, neutral) from the International Affective Picture System served as emotional stimuli. EEG (61 electrodes) and EOG were recorded during slide presentation. After each slide, the subjects had to rate emotional valence and arousal on a 9-point self-report scale. Good heartbeat perceivers scored the emotional slides significantly more arousing than poor heartbeat perceivers; no differences were found in the emotional valence ratings. The visually evoked potentials of good and poor heartbeat perceivers showed significant differences in the P300 and in the slow-wave latency ranges. Statistical analyses revealed significantly higher P300 mean amplitudes for good heartbeat perceivers (averaged across all 60 slides) than for poor heartbeat perceivers. In the slow-wave range, this effect was found for affective slides only. Heartbeat perception scores correlated significantly and positively with both the mean arousal rating as well as with the mean amplitudes in the P300 time window and the slow-wave window. Our results demonstrate a strong relationship between the perception of cardiac signals and the cortical processing of emotional stimuli, as would be postulated for example by the James–Lange theory of emotions.

对内脏信号的感知在许多情绪理论中起着至关重要的作用。本研究旨在探讨内感受性意识与情绪相关的大脑活动之间的关系。44名参与者(16名男性,28名女性)首先接受了心跳感知任务,然后被分为良好(n = 22)和较差的心跳感知者(n = 22)。来自国际情感图片系统的60张不同的图片(愉快的、不愉快的、中性的)作为情绪刺激。在幻灯片展示过程中记录EEG(61个电极)和EOG。每张幻灯片之后,受试者必须在9分制的自我报告量表上对情绪效价和觉醒进行评分。良好的心跳感知者比差的心跳感知者在情绪幻灯片上得分明显更高;在情绪效价评分上没有发现差异。视觉诱发电位在P300区和慢波潜伏期范围内存在显著差异。统计分析显示,良好的心跳感知者的P300平均幅度(所有60张幻灯片的平均值)明显高于差的心跳感知者。在慢波范围内,这种效应只存在于情感幻灯片上。心跳知觉得分与平均唤醒等级以及P300时间窗和慢波窗口的平均振幅均呈显著正相关。我们的研究结果表明,心脏信号的感知与情绪刺激的皮层处理之间存在着密切的关系,例如詹姆斯-兰格情绪理论就提出了这一假设。
{"title":"On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes","authors":"Olga Pollatos ,&nbsp;Wladimir Kirsch ,&nbsp;Rainer Schandry","doi":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The perception of visceral signals plays a crucial role in many theories of emotions. The present study was designed to investigate the relationship between interoceptive awareness and emotion-related brain activity. 44 participants (16 male, 28 female) first underwent a heartbeat perception task and then were categorised either as good (<em>n</em> = 22) or poor heartbeat perceivers (<em>n</em> = 22). A total of 60 different pictures (pleasant, unpleasant, neutral) from the International Affective Picture System served as emotional stimuli. EEG (61 electrodes) and EOG were recorded during slide presentation. After each slide, the subjects had to rate emotional valence and arousal on a 9-point self-report scale. Good heartbeat perceivers scored the emotional slides significantly more arousing than poor heartbeat perceivers; no differences were found in the emotional valence ratings. The visually evoked potentials of good and poor heartbeat perceivers showed significant differences in the P300 and in the slow-wave latency ranges. Statistical analyses revealed significantly higher P300 mean amplitudes for good heartbeat perceivers (averaged across all 60 slides) than for poor heartbeat perceivers. In the slow-wave range, this effect was found for affective slides only. Heartbeat perception scores correlated significantly and positively with both the mean arousal rating as well as with the mean amplitudes in the P300 time window and the slow-wave window. Our results demonstrate a strong relationship between the perception of cardiac signals and the cortical processing of emotional stimuli, as would be postulated for example by the James–Lange theory of emotions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100287,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Brain Research","volume":"25 3","pages":"Pages 948-962"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2005-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25699528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 279
期刊
Cognitive Brain Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1