Alana Brown, Nicole J. Gervais, Laura Gravelsins, Sophia Zhao, Annie Duchesne, Jenny Rieck, Anna Mouzenian, Noelia Calvo, Negar Mazloum-Farzaghi, Rosanna Olsen, Morgan Barense, Zhuo Shao, Marcus Bernardini, Michelle Jacobson, M. Natasha Rajah, Cheryl Grady, Gillian Einstein
Early midlife bilateral salpingo-oophorectomy (BSO) is associated with greater Alzheimer's disease risk compared to spontaneous/natural menopause. Previously, we found that participants with BSO had lower volume in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and cornu ammonis 2/3 composite subfield (DG-CA2/3). We sought to extend those hippocampal subfield findings by assessing whether BSO affected volumes along the anteroposterior hippocampal axis, anterolateral entorhinal cortex, and perirhinal cortex subregions (Brodmann area (BA) 35 and 36). We also correlated volumes with key demographic and wellbeing-related factors (age, depressive mood, education), hormone therapy characteristics, and recognition memory performance. Early midlife participants with BSO (with and without 17β-estradiol therapy (ET)) and age-matched control participants with intact ovaries (AMC) completed high-resolution T2-weighted structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Medial temporal lobe volumes and Remember-Know task recognition memory performance were compared between groups—BSO (n = 23), BSO + ET (n = 28), AMC (n = 34) using univariate analyses. Multivariate Partial Least Squares (PLS) analyses were used to examine how volumes related to demographic and wellbeing-related factors, as well as hormone therapy characteristics. Relative to BSO + ET, BSO had lower posterior hippocampal and DG-CA2/3 volumes but greater perirhinal BA 36 volumes. Compared to age, depressive mood, and education, ET was the strongest positive predictor of hippocampal volumes and negative predictor of perirhinal BA 36 volumes. For BSO + ET, hippocampal volumes were negatively related to ET duration and positively related to concurrent progestogen therapy. Relative to AMC, BSO had greater anterolateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal BA 35 and BA 36 volumes. BSO groups (with and without ET) relied more on familiarity than recollection for successful recognition memory. BSO and ET may have distinct effects on medial temporal lobe volumes, with potential implications for memory processes affected by Alzheimer's disease risk.
与自然/自然绝经相比,中年早期双侧输卵管切除术(BSO)与更高的阿尔茨海默病风险有关。此前,我们发现,BSO 患者的海马齿状回和胼胝体 2/3 复合亚区(DG-CA2/3)体积较小。我们试图通过评估 BSO 是否会影响海马前后轴、前外侧内侧皮层和边缘皮层亚区(布罗德曼区 (BA) 35 和 36)的体积来扩展这些海马亚区的研究结果。我们还将海马体积与主要的人口统计学和福利相关因素(年龄、抑郁情绪、教育程度)、激素治疗特征和识别记忆能力相关联。患有BSO(接受或未接受17β-雌二醇治疗(ET))的中年早期患者和年龄匹配的卵巢完好的对照组(AMC)患者完成了高分辨率T2加权结构磁共振成像(MRI)。通过单变量分析比较了不同组别--BSO(23 人)、BSO + ET(28 人)和 AMC(34 人)--的颞叶内侧体积和 "记住-知道 "任务的识别记忆能力。使用多变量偏最小二乘法(PLS)分析来检验量与人口统计学和福利相关因素以及激素治疗特征的关系。与 BSO + ET 相比,BSO 的海马后部和 DG-CA2/3 体积较小,但脐周 BA 36 体积较大。与年龄、抑郁情绪和教育程度相比,ET对海马体积的正向预测作用最强,对脑周 BA 36 体积的负向预测作用最小。对于 BSO + ET,海马体积与 ET 持续时间呈负相关,与同时接受孕激素治疗呈正相关。与 AMC 相比,BSO 组的前外侧内侧皮层以及边缘 BA 35 和 BA 36 体积更大。BSO组(含ET和不含ET)的成功识别记忆更依赖于熟悉而非回忆。BSO和ET可能对内侧颞叶体积有不同的影响,这对受阿尔茨海默病风险影响的记忆过程具有潜在的意义。
{"title":"Effects of Early Midlife Ovarian Removal on Medial Temporal Lobe Gray Matter Volume and Recognition Memory","authors":"Alana Brown, Nicole J. Gervais, Laura Gravelsins, Sophia Zhao, Annie Duchesne, Jenny Rieck, Anna Mouzenian, Noelia Calvo, Negar Mazloum-Farzaghi, Rosanna Olsen, Morgan Barense, Zhuo Shao, Marcus Bernardini, Michelle Jacobson, M. Natasha Rajah, Cheryl Grady, Gillian Einstein","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70012","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70012","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early midlife bilateral salpingo-oophorectomy (BSO) is associated with greater Alzheimer's disease risk compared to spontaneous/natural menopause. Previously, we found that participants with BSO had lower volume in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and cornu ammonis 2/3 composite subfield (DG-CA2/3). We sought to extend those hippocampal subfield findings by assessing whether BSO affected volumes along the anteroposterior hippocampal axis, anterolateral entorhinal cortex, and perirhinal cortex subregions (Brodmann area (BA) 35 and 36). We also correlated volumes with key demographic and wellbeing-related factors (age, depressive mood, education), hormone therapy characteristics, and recognition memory performance. Early midlife participants with BSO (with and without 17β-estradiol therapy (ET)) and age-matched control participants with intact ovaries (AMC) completed high-resolution T2-weighted structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Medial temporal lobe volumes and Remember-Know task recognition memory performance were compared between groups—BSO (<i>n</i> = 23), BSO + ET (<i>n</i> = 28), AMC (<i>n</i> = 34) using univariate analyses. Multivariate Partial Least Squares (PLS) analyses were used to examine how volumes related to demographic and wellbeing-related factors, as well as hormone therapy characteristics. Relative to BSO + ET, BSO had lower posterior hippocampal and DG-CA2/3 volumes but greater perirhinal BA 36 volumes. Compared to age, depressive mood, and education, ET was the strongest positive predictor of hippocampal volumes and negative predictor of perirhinal BA 36 volumes. For BSO + ET, hippocampal volumes were negatively related to ET duration and positively related to concurrent progestogen therapy. Relative to AMC, BSO had greater anterolateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal BA 35 and BA 36 volumes. BSO groups (with and without ET) relied more on familiarity than recollection for successful recognition memory. BSO and ET may have distinct effects on medial temporal lobe volumes, with potential implications for memory processes affected by Alzheimer's disease risk.</p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70012","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143726933","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Rochele Castelo-Branco, Ana Paula de Castro de Araujo, Karen Cristina Pugliane, Luiz Eduardo Mateus Brandão, Ramón Hypolito Lima, Hindiael Belchior, Ywlliane da Silva Rodrigues da Meurer, Arthur Antunes Pereira-Costa, Flávio Freitas Barbosa
The ability to form different neural representations for similar inputs is a central process of episodic memory. Although the dorsal dentate gyrus and CA3 have been indicated as important in this phenomenon, the neuronal circuits underlying spatiotemporal memory processing with different levels of spatial similarity are still elusive. In this study, we measured the expression of the immediate early gene c-Fos to evaluate brain areas activated when rats recalled the temporal order of object locations in a task, with either high or low levels of spatial interference. Animals showed spatiotemporal memory in both conditions once they spent more time exploring the older object locations relative to the more recent ones. We found no difference in the levels of c-Fos expression between high and low spatial interference. However, the levels of c-Fos expression in CA2 positively correlated with the discrimination index in the low spatial interference condition. More importantly, functional network connectivity analysis revealed a wider and more interconnected neuronal circuit in conditions of high than in low spatial interference. Our study advances the understanding of brain networks recruited in episodic memory with different degrees of spatial similarity.
{"title":"Brain Networks Differ According to Levels of Interference in Spatiotemporal Processing","authors":"Rochele Castelo-Branco, Ana Paula de Castro de Araujo, Karen Cristina Pugliane, Luiz Eduardo Mateus Brandão, Ramón Hypolito Lima, Hindiael Belchior, Ywlliane da Silva Rodrigues da Meurer, Arthur Antunes Pereira-Costa, Flávio Freitas Barbosa","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70011","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to form different neural representations for similar inputs is a central process of episodic memory. Although the dorsal dentate gyrus and CA3 have been indicated as important in this phenomenon, the neuronal circuits underlying spatiotemporal memory processing with different levels of spatial similarity are still elusive. In this study, we measured the expression of the immediate early gene c-Fos to evaluate brain areas activated when rats recalled the temporal order of object locations in a task, with either high or low levels of spatial interference. Animals showed spatiotemporal memory in both conditions once they spent more time exploring the older object locations relative to the more recent ones. We found no difference in the levels of c-Fos expression between high and low spatial interference. However, the levels of c-Fos expression in CA2 positively correlated with the discrimination index in the low spatial interference condition. More importantly, functional network connectivity analysis revealed a wider and more interconnected neuronal circuit in conditions of high than in low spatial interference. Our study advances the understanding of brain networks recruited in episodic memory with different degrees of spatial similarity.</p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70011","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143689650","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Sarah L. Aghjayan, Sarah E. Polk, Hayley S. Ripperger, Haiqing Huang, Lu Wan, Thomas Kamarck, Anna L. Marsland, Chaeryon Kang, Michelle W. Voss, Bradley P. Sutton, Lauren E. Oberlin, Jeffrey M. Burns, Eric D. Vidoni, Edward McAuley, Charles H. Hillman, Arthur F. Kramer, Kirk I. Erickson
Different tasks of episodic memory (EM) are only moderately correlated with each other. Furthermore, various EM tasks exhibit disproportional relationships with the hippocampus. This study examined the covariance structure of EM tasks and assessed whether this structure relates differently to hippocampal volume (HV) in a sample of 648 cognitively unimpaired older adults (mean age = 69.88). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and linear regression models were used to test the associations between the observed factors of EM and HV. A model with three first-order subfactors (immediate verbal recall, delayed verbal recall, and visuospatial) derived from a second-order EM domain factor satisfied model fit (χ2p value ≥ 0.05, CFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08, SRMR < 0.08). Total, left, and right HV explained a similar amount of variance in all EM subfactors. CA1, CA3, subiculum, and entorhinal cortex volume were associated with all subfactors, while CA2 and dentate gyrus volume were not associated with EM. These results suggest that EM tasks are measuring the same construct, but different complex processes contribute to EM. Furthermore, HV accounted for a small portion of the variance in EM, suggesting that HV might not be a useful marker of EM in cognitively unimpaired older adults. Finally, this study provides evidence that various hippocampal subfield volumes may not be purely associated with any one aspect of EM processing.
{"title":"Associations Between Episodic Memory and Hippocampal Volume in Late Adulthood","authors":"Sarah L. Aghjayan, Sarah E. Polk, Hayley S. Ripperger, Haiqing Huang, Lu Wan, Thomas Kamarck, Anna L. Marsland, Chaeryon Kang, Michelle W. Voss, Bradley P. Sutton, Lauren E. Oberlin, Jeffrey M. Burns, Eric D. Vidoni, Edward McAuley, Charles H. Hillman, Arthur F. Kramer, Kirk I. Erickson","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70010","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Different tasks of episodic memory (EM) are only moderately correlated with each other. Furthermore, various EM tasks exhibit disproportional relationships with the hippocampus. This study examined the covariance structure of EM tasks and assessed whether this structure relates differently to hippocampal volume (HV) in a sample of 648 cognitively unimpaired older adults (mean age = 69.88). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and linear regression models were used to test the associations between the observed factors of EM and HV. A model with three first-order subfactors (immediate verbal recall, delayed verbal recall, and visuospatial) derived from a second-order EM domain factor satisfied model fit (<i>χ</i><sup>2</sup> <i>p</i> value ≥ 0.05, CFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08, SRMR < 0.08). Total, left, and right HV explained a similar amount of variance in all EM subfactors. CA1, CA3, subiculum, and entorhinal cortex volume were associated with all subfactors, while CA2 and dentate gyrus volume were not associated with EM. These results suggest that EM tasks are measuring the same construct, but different complex processes contribute to EM. Furthermore, HV accounted for a small portion of the variance in EM, suggesting that HV might not be a useful marker of EM in cognitively unimpaired older adults. Finally, this study provides evidence that various hippocampal subfield volumes may not be purely associated with any one aspect of EM processing.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143690165","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
G. Sotelo-Parrilla, B. Quintero, I. Trujillo, F. Rodríguez, C. Salas, A. Gómez
Transitive inference, a process that involves drawing logical conclusions based on preliminary information, is considered a cornerstone of human deductive reasoning. Furthermore, transitive inference is a clear instance of representational flexibility as it implies the novel expression of learned information. In mammals and birds, both episodic memory and transitive inference critically depend on the integrity of the hippocampus. Comparative neurobiological evidence indicates that a hippocampus homologue can also be found in the telencephalic pallium of teleost fish. Here, we investigated whether goldfish demonstrate inferential behavior in a standard transitive inference task, and whether the hippocampal pallium of goldfish, akin to the hippocampus in mammals and birds, plays a role in transitive responding. We trained goldfish with hippocampal pallium lesions and sham-operated controls on a series of overlapping two-item visual premise pairs: A+B−, B+C−, C+D−, D+E−. The sham-operated animals readily learned the premise pair discriminations and responded transitively during the crucial test involving a novel pair of nonadjacent elements (B vs. D). However, hippocampal pallium-lesioned goldfish were impaired in the critical transitive inference test, although they successfully learned to discriminate the premise pairs. These findings suggest that a relational memory function, which supports the novel expression of learned information, could be a primitive feature of the vertebrate hippocampus. Such outcome contributes significantly to the ongoing debate regarding the evolutionary origins of episodic memory in vertebrates.
{"title":"Hippocampal Pallium Lesion Impairs Transitive Inference in Goldfish","authors":"G. Sotelo-Parrilla, B. Quintero, I. Trujillo, F. Rodríguez, C. Salas, A. Gómez","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70007","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Transitive inference, a process that involves drawing logical conclusions based on preliminary information, is considered a cornerstone of human deductive reasoning. Furthermore, transitive inference is a clear instance of representational flexibility as it implies the novel expression of learned information. In mammals and birds, both episodic memory and transitive inference critically depend on the integrity of the hippocampus. Comparative neurobiological evidence indicates that a hippocampus homologue can also be found in the telencephalic pallium of teleost fish. Here, we investigated whether goldfish demonstrate inferential behavior in a standard transitive inference task, and whether the hippocampal pallium of goldfish, akin to the hippocampus in mammals and birds, plays a role in transitive responding. We trained goldfish with hippocampal pallium lesions and sham-operated controls on a series of overlapping two-item visual premise pairs: A+B−, B+C−, C+D−, D+E−. The sham-operated animals readily learned the premise pair discriminations and responded transitively during the crucial test involving a novel pair of nonadjacent elements (B vs. D). However, hippocampal pallium-lesioned goldfish were impaired in the critical transitive inference test, although they successfully learned to discriminate the premise pairs. These findings suggest that a relational memory function, which supports the novel expression of learned information, could be a primitive feature of the vertebrate hippocampus. Such outcome contributes significantly to the ongoing debate regarding the evolutionary origins of episodic memory in vertebrates.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143638788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Maria Carmen Lorduy, Ana María Insausti Serrano, Mónica Muñoz López, Laura Wisse, Paul A. Yushkevich, David G. Amaral
<p>Highly respected hippocampal neuroanatomist Ricardo Insausti died on the morning of December 26th, 2024. For the previous 2 years, he carried out a heroic battle with cancer while still teaching, collaborating, and writing. His research made huge contributions to the neuroscience of the medial temporal lobe, and he led efforts to bring consistent nomenclature to the hippocampal formation of the rodent, primate, and human brains.</p><p>As a teenager, Ricardo was part of the swimming team and enjoyed snorkeling and spearfishing when he went to the beach. He also liked Basque pelota, more as an observer than as a participant. During the weekends, he liked to play MUS, the most widely played card game in Spain that originated in the Basque country, with his older brother Jesús against his father, and his other brother Santos José. Ricardo's interest in biology was evident even as a child. But his interest in research began when he was 15 years old, when his high school biology teacher encouraged him, and other students, to do experiments and took them on field trips to, for example, collect water from puddles, which they later examined under the microscope searching for microorganisms.</p><p>He began his medical studies at the University of Navarra and early on took part in studies of the nervous system in the Department of Anatomy. He immediately fell in love with the human brain and began volunteering in the Department of Neurosurgery at the Hospital of Navarra. Two years before finishing his medical studies, he already began carrying out research for his Ph.D. on the rodent trigeminal somatosensory system, specifically investigating the pain pathways to the brainstem reticular formation, which he completed in 1978. During that summer, he worked for 3 months as a village doctor in Peralta, a small town in Navarra. Although brief, he always remembered this period of clinical service fondly.</p><p>Subsequently, Ricardo was obliged to do mandatory military service, which lasted 15 months. His service was carried out in the city of Valladolid, which is about 200 miles southwest of Pamplona. He did his service at the Military Hospital but also spent many afternoons in the Anatomy Department of the University of Valladolid, keeping up to date on scientific advances.</p><p>After he finished military service, he thought of pursuing a professional career as a neurosurgeon and combining it with basic research. To do so, he took a national exam that would allow him an internship in a hospital to specialize in neurosurgery. He passed the exam, but the Hospital of Navarra did not offer places in neurosurgery. This meant that he would have had to go to the Hospital of Zaragoza, 120 miles from Pamplona. Since he would not have been able to keep doing research at his university, he was faced with the difficult decision of pursuing being a neurosurgeon or doing research.</p><p>Ultimately, he decided to focus on basic brain research and to teach gross anatomy in
{"title":"Remembering Ricardo Serrano Insausti 1954–2024","authors":"Maria Carmen Lorduy, Ana María Insausti Serrano, Mónica Muñoz López, Laura Wisse, Paul A. Yushkevich, David G. Amaral","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70005","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Highly respected hippocampal neuroanatomist Ricardo Insausti died on the morning of December 26th, 2024. For the previous 2 years, he carried out a heroic battle with cancer while still teaching, collaborating, and writing. His research made huge contributions to the neuroscience of the medial temporal lobe, and he led efforts to bring consistent nomenclature to the hippocampal formation of the rodent, primate, and human brains.</p><p>As a teenager, Ricardo was part of the swimming team and enjoyed snorkeling and spearfishing when he went to the beach. He also liked Basque pelota, more as an observer than as a participant. During the weekends, he liked to play MUS, the most widely played card game in Spain that originated in the Basque country, with his older brother Jesús against his father, and his other brother Santos José. Ricardo's interest in biology was evident even as a child. But his interest in research began when he was 15 years old, when his high school biology teacher encouraged him, and other students, to do experiments and took them on field trips to, for example, collect water from puddles, which they later examined under the microscope searching for microorganisms.</p><p>He began his medical studies at the University of Navarra and early on took part in studies of the nervous system in the Department of Anatomy. He immediately fell in love with the human brain and began volunteering in the Department of Neurosurgery at the Hospital of Navarra. Two years before finishing his medical studies, he already began carrying out research for his Ph.D. on the rodent trigeminal somatosensory system, specifically investigating the pain pathways to the brainstem reticular formation, which he completed in 1978. During that summer, he worked for 3 months as a village doctor in Peralta, a small town in Navarra. Although brief, he always remembered this period of clinical service fondly.</p><p>Subsequently, Ricardo was obliged to do mandatory military service, which lasted 15 months. His service was carried out in the city of Valladolid, which is about 200 miles southwest of Pamplona. He did his service at the Military Hospital but also spent many afternoons in the Anatomy Department of the University of Valladolid, keeping up to date on scientific advances.</p><p>After he finished military service, he thought of pursuing a professional career as a neurosurgeon and combining it with basic research. To do so, he took a national exam that would allow him an internship in a hospital to specialize in neurosurgery. He passed the exam, but the Hospital of Navarra did not offer places in neurosurgery. This meant that he would have had to go to the Hospital of Zaragoza, 120 miles from Pamplona. Since he would not have been able to keep doing research at his university, he was faced with the difficult decision of pursuing being a neurosurgeon or doing research.</p><p>Ultimately, he decided to focus on basic brain research and to teach gross anatomy in ","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143622583","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Zilong Ji, Eleonora Lomi, Kate Jeffery, Anna S. Mitchell, Neil Burgess
Grid and place cells typically fire at progressively earlier phases within each cycle of the theta rhythm as rodents run across their firing fields, a phenomenon known as theta phase precession. Here, we report theta phase precession relative to turning angle in theta-modulated head direction cells within the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (AVN). As rodents turn their heads, these cells fire at progressively earlier phases as head direction sweeps over their preferred tuning direction. The degree of phase precession increases with angular head velocity. Moreover, phase precession is more pronounced in those theta-modulated head direction cells that exhibit theta skipping, with a stronger theta-skipping effect correlating with a higher degree of phase precession. These findings are consistent with a ring attractor model that integrates external theta input with internal firing rate adaptation—a phenomenon we identified in head direction cells within AVN. Our results broaden the range of information known to be subject to neural phase coding and enrich our understanding of the neural dynamics supporting spatial orientation and navigation.
{"title":"Phase Precession Relative to Turning Angle in Theta-Modulated Head Direction Cells","authors":"Zilong Ji, Eleonora Lomi, Kate Jeffery, Anna S. Mitchell, Neil Burgess","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70008","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Grid and place cells typically fire at progressively earlier phases within each cycle of the theta rhythm as rodents run across their firing fields, a phenomenon known as theta phase precession. Here, we report theta phase precession relative to turning angle in theta-modulated head direction cells within the anteroventral thalamic nucleus (AVN). As rodents turn their heads, these cells fire at progressively earlier phases as head direction sweeps over their preferred tuning direction. The degree of phase precession increases with angular head velocity. Moreover, phase precession is more pronounced in those theta-modulated head direction cells that exhibit theta skipping, with a stronger theta-skipping effect correlating with a higher degree of phase precession. These findings are consistent with a ring attractor model that integrates external theta input with internal firing rate adaptation—a phenomenon we identified in head direction cells within AVN. Our results broaden the range of information known to be subject to neural phase coding and enrich our understanding of the neural dynamics supporting spatial orientation and navigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70008","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143602509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Claudia R. Diaz, Nancy S. Hong, Kortney Dyck, Mason J. Gibb, Maleeha Panjwani, Kyle Szymanski, Robert J. McDonald
Long-term potentiation (LTP) is proposed to be the molecular mechanism underlying learning and memory in the brain. A key event for LTP is the influx of calcium into post-synaptic neurons via multiple ion channel control systems. One such system involves N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs), which were originally believed to be essential for LTP and new learning. Recent studies have demonstrated that hippocampal NMDARs are critical for learning new spatial information in a novel environment; however, when pre-training occurs prior to new spatial learning, these receptors are not needed. Additionally, researchers have shown that activation of voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs) and their associated calcium influx can induce LTP independent of NMDARs. These findings led to the idea that the amount of calcium required for learning in hippocampus depends on whether the new learning takes place in a novel or familiar environment, with a novel environment demanding greater calcium influx. It was hypothesized that to impair new learning in a familiar environment both NMDARs and VGCCs would need to be blocked. Long-Evans rats were trained in a three-phase version of the Morris water task, which included pre-training, new learning mass-training, and a probe test. Prior to mass-training, intrahippocampal VGCCs were blocked individually or in combination with NMDARs blockade to evaluate their effects on the rats learning and memory. The results showed that blocking both NMDARs and VGCCs simultaneously impaired new spatial learning with familiar information, whereas VGCC blockade alone did not.
{"title":"Effects of Blocking Multiple Sources of Calcium in Hippocampus During Spatial Learning and Memory Using a Rapid Acquisition Variant of the Morris Water Task","authors":"Claudia R. Diaz, Nancy S. Hong, Kortney Dyck, Mason J. Gibb, Maleeha Panjwani, Kyle Szymanski, Robert J. McDonald","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70006","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Long-term potentiation (LTP) is proposed to be the molecular mechanism underlying learning and memory in the brain. A key event for LTP is the influx of calcium into post-synaptic neurons via multiple ion channel control systems. One such system involves <i>N</i>-methyl-<i>D</i>-aspartate receptors (NMDARs), which were originally believed to be essential for LTP and new learning. Recent studies have demonstrated that hippocampal NMDARs are critical for learning new spatial information in a novel environment; however, when pre-training occurs prior to new spatial learning, these receptors are not needed. Additionally, researchers have shown that activation of voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs) and their associated calcium influx can induce LTP independent of NMDARs. These findings led to the idea that the amount of calcium required for learning in hippocampus depends on whether the new learning takes place in a novel or familiar environment, with a novel environment demanding greater calcium influx. It was hypothesized that to impair new learning in a familiar environment both NMDARs and VGCCs would need to be blocked. Long-Evans rats were trained in a three-phase version of the Morris water task, which included pre-training, new learning mass-training, and a probe test. Prior to mass-training, intrahippocampal VGCCs were blocked individually or in combination with NMDARs blockade to evaluate their effects on the rats learning and memory. The results showed that blocking both NMDARs and VGCCs simultaneously impaired new spatial learning with familiar information, whereas VGCC blockade alone did not.</p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143602511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Christina Strauch, Juliane Böge, Olena Shchyglo, Valentyna Dubovyk, Denise Manahan-Vaughan
The cover image is based on the article The Suprapyramidal and Infrapyramidal Blades of the Dentate Gyrus Exhibit Different GluN Subunit Content and Dissimilar Frequency-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity In Vivo by Christina Strauch et al., https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70002.
{"title":"Cover Image, Volume 35, Issue 2","authors":"Christina Strauch, Juliane Böge, Olena Shchyglo, Valentyna Dubovyk, Denise Manahan-Vaughan","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70009","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The cover image is based on the article <i>The Suprapyramidal and Infrapyramidal Blades of the Dentate Gyrus Exhibit Different GluN Subunit Content and Dissimilar Frequency-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity In Vivo</i> by Christina Strauch et al., https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70002.\u0000\u0000 <figure>\u0000 <div><picture>\u0000 <source></source></picture><p></p>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </figure></p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":"C1"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143602657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Christina Strauch, Juliane Böge, Olena Shchyglo, Valentyna Dubovyk, Denise Manahan-Vaughan
The entorhinal cortex sends afferent information to the hippocampus by means of the perforant path (PP). The PP input to the dentate gyrus (DG) terminates in the suprapyramidal (sDG) and infrapyramidal (iDG) blades. Different electrophysiological stimulation patterns of the PP can generate hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Whether frequency-dependent synaptic plasticity differs in the sDG and iDG is unclear. Here, we compared medial PP–DG responses in freely behaving adult rats and found that synaptic plasticity in the sDG is broadly frequency dependent, whereby long-term depression (LTD, > 24 h) is induced with stimulation at 1 Hz, short-term depression (< 2 h) is triggered by 5 or 10 Hz, and long-term potentiation (LTP) of increasing magnitudes is induced by 200 and 400 Hz stimulation, respectively. By contrast, although the iDG expresses STD following 5 or 10 Hz stimulation, LTD induced by 1 Hz is weaker, LTP is not induced by 200 Hz and LTP induced by 400 Hz stimulation is significantly smaller in magnitude than LTP induced in sDG. Furthermore, the stimulus–response relationship of iDG is suppressed compared to sDG. These differences may arise from differences in granule cell properties, or the complement of NMDA receptors. Patch clamp recordings, in vitro, revealed reduced firing frequencies in response to high currents, and different action potential thresholds in iDG compared to sDG. Assessment of the expression of GluN subunits revealed significantly lower expression levels of GluN1, GluN2A, and GluN2B in the middle molecular layer of iDG compared to sDG. Taken together, these data indicate that synaptic plasticity in the iDG is weaker, less persistent and less responsive to afferent frequencies than synaptic plasticity in sDG. Effects may be mediated by weaker NMDA receptor expression and differences in neuronal responses in iDG versus sDG. These characteristics may explain reported differences in experience-dependent information processing in the suprapyramidal and infrapyramidal blades of the DG.
{"title":"The Suprapyramidal and Infrapyramidal Blades of the Dentate Gyrus Exhibit Different GluN Subunit Content and Dissimilar Frequency-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity In Vivo","authors":"Christina Strauch, Juliane Böge, Olena Shchyglo, Valentyna Dubovyk, Denise Manahan-Vaughan","doi":"10.1002/hipo.70002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.70002","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The entorhinal cortex sends afferent information to the hippocampus by means of the perforant path (PP). The PP input to the dentate gyrus (DG) terminates in the suprapyramidal (sDG) and infrapyramidal (iDG) blades. Different electrophysiological stimulation patterns of the PP can generate hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Whether frequency-dependent synaptic plasticity differs in the sDG and iDG is unclear. Here, we compared medial PP–DG responses in freely behaving adult rats and found that synaptic plasticity in the sDG is broadly frequency dependent, whereby long-term depression (LTD, > 24 h) is induced with stimulation at 1 Hz, short-term depression (< 2 h) is triggered by 5 or 10 Hz, and long-term potentiation (LTP) of increasing magnitudes is induced by 200 and 400 Hz stimulation, respectively. By contrast, although the iDG expresses STD following 5 or 10 Hz stimulation, LTD induced by 1 Hz is weaker, LTP is not induced by 200 Hz and LTP induced by 400 Hz stimulation is significantly smaller in magnitude than LTP induced in sDG. Furthermore, the stimulus–response relationship of iDG is suppressed compared to sDG. These differences may arise from differences in granule cell properties, or the complement of NMDA receptors. Patch clamp recordings, in vitro, revealed reduced firing frequencies in response to high currents, and different action potential thresholds in iDG compared to sDG. Assessment of the expression of GluN subunits revealed significantly lower expression levels of GluN1, GluN2A, and GluN2B in the middle molecular layer of iDG compared to sDG. Taken together, these data indicate that synaptic plasticity in the iDG is weaker, less persistent and less responsive to afferent frequencies than synaptic plasticity in sDG. Effects may be mediated by weaker NMDA receptor expression and differences in neuronal responses in iDG versus sDG. These characteristics may explain reported differences in experience-dependent information processing in the suprapyramidal and infrapyramidal blades of the DG.</p>","PeriodicalId":13171,"journal":{"name":"Hippocampus","volume":"35 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2025-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/hipo.70002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143481330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}