首页 > 最新文献

International Journal of Behavioral Development最新文献

英文 中文
Adolescent leadership: Associations with exclusion of others, effortful control, and popularity 青少年的领导力:与排斥他人、努力控制和受欢迎程度的关系
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-15 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241259392
Luhao Wei, M. Shen, U. Purwono, D. French
Three-year longitudinal and concurrent associations of leadership with overt aggression, exclusion of others, effortful control (EC), popularity, unpopularity, prosocial behavior, and academic achievement were assessed in this study of Indonesian adolescents. Participants were initially assessed in the 10th grade ( n = 462, 256 girls; Mage = 15.14, SD = .62) and followed for 2 years. Concurrent analyses revealed that leadership was positively associated with exclusion of others, EC, and popularity and negatively associated with overt aggression at 12th grade. The positive associations between leadership and exclusion of others only emerged for youth with high levels of EC. Positive associations of leadership with academic achievement were found in the longitudinal analyses and concurrent analyses at the 11th and 12th grade and concurrent associations between prosocial behavior and leadership were found each year. These findings reflect Indonesian cultural norms in which leaders are expected to maintain interpersonal harmony and avoid overt conflict.
本研究评估了领导力与公开攻击、排斥他人、努力控制(EC)、受欢迎程度、不受欢迎程度、亲社会行为和学业成绩之间为期三年的纵向和并发关联。受试者最初在十年级时接受评估(n = 462,256 名女生;Mage = 15.14,SD = .62),并随访两年。同时进行的分析表明,在十二年级时,领导力与排斥他人、EC和受欢迎程度呈正相关,而与明显的攻击行为呈负相关。领导力与排斥他人之间的正相关只出现在EC水平较高的青少年身上。在 11 年级和 12 年级的纵向分析和并行分析中发现,领导力与学业成绩呈正相关,而亲社会行为与领导力之间每年都有并行关系。这些发现反映了印度尼西亚的文化规范,即领导者应保持人际和谐,避免公开冲突。
{"title":"Adolescent leadership: Associations with exclusion of others, effortful control, and popularity","authors":"Luhao Wei, M. Shen, U. Purwono, D. French","doi":"10.1177/01650254241259392","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241259392","url":null,"abstract":"Three-year longitudinal and concurrent associations of leadership with overt aggression, exclusion of others, effortful control (EC), popularity, unpopularity, prosocial behavior, and academic achievement were assessed in this study of Indonesian adolescents. Participants were initially assessed in the 10th grade ( n = 462, 256 girls; Mage = 15.14, SD = .62) and followed for 2 years. Concurrent analyses revealed that leadership was positively associated with exclusion of others, EC, and popularity and negatively associated with overt aggression at 12th grade. The positive associations between leadership and exclusion of others only emerged for youth with high levels of EC. Positive associations of leadership with academic achievement were found in the longitudinal analyses and concurrent analyses at the 11th and 12th grade and concurrent associations between prosocial behavior and leadership were found each year. These findings reflect Indonesian cultural norms in which leaders are expected to maintain interpersonal harmony and avoid overt conflict.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141335950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-concept at different stages of life: How do early and late adolescents and young, middle-aged, and older adults describe themselves? 不同人生阶段的自我概念:早期和晚期青少年以及青年、中年和老年人如何描述自己?
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241254103
Anni Tamm, Pirko Tõugu, Tiia Tulviste
The study aimed to find out which differences and similarities emerge in the self-concepts of early and late adolescents and young, middle-aged, and older adults. A total of 822 participants, including 530 adolescents aged 9–19 (over 50% were girls) and 292 adults aged 20–71 (over 80% were women), from Estonia provided their spontaneous self-descriptions. Early adolescents described their preferences, appearance, family, peers, and peripheral attributes more frequently and traits, identity, and global attributes less frequently than other age groups. Late adolescents’ self-descriptions included significantly more traits and fewer social roles, identity, and family mentions than those of adults. The three adult groups were similar in the use of different attributes to describe themselves. Overall, the study suggests that early adolescents describe themselves mainly through concrete, objective attributes, late adolescents through abstract psychological attributes, and adults through social–psychological attributes.
这项研究旨在找出早期和晚期青少年以及青年、中年和老年人自我概念的异同。共有来自爱沙尼亚的 822 名参与者提供了自发的自我描述,其中包括 530 名 9-19 岁的青少年(50% 以上为女孩)和 292 名 20-71 岁的成年人(80% 以上为女性)。与其他年龄组相比,早期青少年更经常描述自己的喜好、外表、家庭、同伴和周边属性,而对特质、身份和整体属性的描述则较少。与成年人相比,晚期青少年在自我描述中对特质的描述明显较多,而对社会角色、身份和家庭的描述则较少。在使用不同属性描述自己方面,三个成人组的情况相似。总之,研究表明,早期青少年主要通过具体的客观属性来描述自己,晚期青少年通过抽象的心理属性来描述自己,而成年人则通过社会心理属性来描述自己。
{"title":"Self-concept at different stages of life: How do early and late adolescents and young, middle-aged, and older adults describe themselves?","authors":"Anni Tamm, Pirko Tõugu, Tiia Tulviste","doi":"10.1177/01650254241254103","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241254103","url":null,"abstract":"The study aimed to find out which differences and similarities emerge in the self-concepts of early and late adolescents and young, middle-aged, and older adults. A total of 822 participants, including 530 adolescents aged 9–19 (over 50% were girls) and 292 adults aged 20–71 (over 80% were women), from Estonia provided their spontaneous self-descriptions. Early adolescents described their preferences, appearance, family, peers, and peripheral attributes more frequently and traits, identity, and global attributes less frequently than other age groups. Late adolescents’ self-descriptions included significantly more traits and fewer social roles, identity, and family mentions than those of adults. The three adult groups were similar in the use of different attributes to describe themselves. Overall, the study suggests that early adolescents describe themselves mainly through concrete, objective attributes, late adolescents through abstract psychological attributes, and adults through social–psychological attributes.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141171468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying the human component model of parenting to other primates: Developmental patterns of mother-child interactions across primate species 在其他灵长类动物中应用人类育儿模式:灵长类动物中母子互动的发展模式
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241255675
Katja Liebal, Manuela Ersson-Lembeck, Federica Amici, Martin Schultze, Manfred Holodynski
The component model of human parenting has been extensively used to study parents’ interactions with their offspring and to examine variation across cultural contexts. The current study applies this model to nonhuman primates to investigate which forms of parenting humans share with other primates and how these interactions change over infants’ first year of life. We repeatedly observed 52 mother-infant pairs, including humans ( N = 11), chimpanzees and bonobos ( N = 21), and several species of small apes ( N = 20), during different daily activities when infants were 1, 6, and 12 months of age. Humans differed from apes in their higher probability of face-to-face contact and the use of object stimulation. Moreover, parenting seemed to be characterized by more variability within humans than within and possibly between ape species. Overall, the component model of parenting appears to be an effective tool to study the functional systems of parenting behavior in a comparative developmental perspective, by allowing direct comparisons between human and non-human primate species across development.
人类养育子女的组成部分模型已被广泛用于研究父母与子女的互动,以及不同文化背景下的差异。本研究将这一模型应用于非人灵长类动物,研究人类与其他灵长类动物共享的养育形式,以及这些互动在婴儿出生后第一年内的变化情况。我们反复观察了52对母婴,包括人类(11对)、黑猩猩和倭黑猩猩(21对)以及几种小型类人猿(20对)在婴儿1、6和12个月大时的不同日常活动。人类与类人猿的不同之处在于,人类更倾向于面对面接触和使用物体刺激。此外,与猿类相比,人类内部养育子女的变异性似乎更大,猿类之间也可能存在这种差异。总之,养育子女的组成部分模型似乎是一种有效的工具,可以从比较发展的角度研究养育子女行为的功能系统,从而对人类和非人灵长类物种在整个发展过程中的情况进行直接比较。
{"title":"Applying the human component model of parenting to other primates: Developmental patterns of mother-child interactions across primate species","authors":"Katja Liebal, Manuela Ersson-Lembeck, Federica Amici, Martin Schultze, Manfred Holodynski","doi":"10.1177/01650254241255675","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241255675","url":null,"abstract":"The component model of human parenting has been extensively used to study parents’ interactions with their offspring and to examine variation across cultural contexts. The current study applies this model to nonhuman primates to investigate which forms of parenting humans share with other primates and how these interactions change over infants’ first year of life. We repeatedly observed 52 mother-infant pairs, including humans ( N = 11), chimpanzees and bonobos ( N = 21), and several species of small apes ( N = 20), during different daily activities when infants were 1, 6, and 12 months of age. Humans differed from apes in their higher probability of face-to-face contact and the use of object stimulation. Moreover, parenting seemed to be characterized by more variability within humans than within and possibly between ape species. Overall, the component model of parenting appears to be an effective tool to study the functional systems of parenting behavior in a comparative developmental perspective, by allowing direct comparisons between human and non-human primate species across development.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141104951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early audiovisual language discrimination: Monolingual and bilingual infants’ differences in language switch detection 早期视听语言辨别能力:单语和双语婴儿在语言转换检测方面的差异
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-18 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241252795
Joan Birulés, Ferran Pons, Laura Bosch
Successful language learning in bilinguals requires the differentiation of two language systems. The capacity to discriminate rhythmically close languages has been reported in 4-month-olds using auditory-only stimuli. This research offers a novel perspective on early language discrimination using audiovisual material. Monolingual and bilingual infants were first habituated to a face talking in the participants’ native language (or the more frequent language in bilingual contexts) and then tested on two successive language switches by the same speaker, with a close and a distant language. Code-switching exposure was indexed from parental questionnaires. Results revealed that while monolinguals could detect both the close- and distant-language switch, bilinguals only reacted to the distant language, regardless of home code-switching experience. In the temporal dimension, the analyses showed that language switch detection required at least 10 s, suggesting that the audiovisual presentation (here the same speaker switching languages) slowed down or even hindered the language switch detection. These results suggest that the detection of a multimodal close-language switch is a challenging task, especially for bilingual infants exposed to phonologically and rhythmically close languages. The current research sets the ground for further studies exploring the role of indexical cues and selective attention processes on language switch detection.
双语者成功的语言学习需要两个语言系统的分化。有报告称,4 个月大的婴儿已能利用纯听觉刺激分辨节奏接近的语言。这项研究为利用视听材料进行早期语言辨别提供了一个新的视角。研究人员首先让单语和双语婴儿习惯于用母语(或在双语环境中使用频率较高的语言)说话的面孔,然后对同一说话者连续两次语言转换进行测试,测试语言分别为近义词和远义词。父母的问卷调查显示了受试者的语码转换情况。结果显示,虽然单语儿童可以检测到近义词和远义词的转换,但双语儿童只对远义词做出反应,而与家庭中的代码转换经历无关。在时间维度上,分析表明,语言转换检测至少需要 10 秒钟,这表明视听演示(此处为同一说话者的语言转换)减慢甚至阻碍了语言转换检测。这些结果表明,检测多模态近似语言转换是一项具有挑战性的任务,尤其是对于接触语音和节奏近似语言的双语婴儿而言。目前的研究为进一步探讨索引线索和选择性注意过程对语言转换检测的作用奠定了基础。
{"title":"Early audiovisual language discrimination: Monolingual and bilingual infants’ differences in language switch detection","authors":"Joan Birulés, Ferran Pons, Laura Bosch","doi":"10.1177/01650254241252795","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241252795","url":null,"abstract":"Successful language learning in bilinguals requires the differentiation of two language systems. The capacity to discriminate rhythmically close languages has been reported in 4-month-olds using auditory-only stimuli. This research offers a novel perspective on early language discrimination using audiovisual material. Monolingual and bilingual infants were first habituated to a face talking in the participants’ native language (or the more frequent language in bilingual contexts) and then tested on two successive language switches by the same speaker, with a close and a distant language. Code-switching exposure was indexed from parental questionnaires. Results revealed that while monolinguals could detect both the close- and distant-language switch, bilinguals only reacted to the distant language, regardless of home code-switching experience. In the temporal dimension, the analyses showed that language switch detection required at least 10 s, suggesting that the audiovisual presentation (here the same speaker switching languages) slowed down or even hindered the language switch detection. These results suggest that the detection of a multimodal close-language switch is a challenging task, especially for bilingual infants exposed to phonologically and rhythmically close languages. The current research sets the ground for further studies exploring the role of indexical cues and selective attention processes on language switch detection.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141060802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental reactions to child negative emotions and child behavioral adjustment: The moderating role of child inhibitory control 父母对孩子负面情绪的反应与孩子的行为适应:儿童抑制控制的调节作用
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241250048
Mengjiao Wang, Xi Chen, Shanyun Zheng
According to the heuristic model of emotion socialization by Eisenberg et al., parental reactions to children’s negative emotions (RCNE) have an important role in this socialization process; however, its effects on children’s social-emotional outcomes may be moderated by the children’s temperament. This longitudinal study verified this proposition by testing maternal and paternal RCNE as predictors of preschoolers’ behavioral adjustment and child inhibitory control as a moderator of these associations. Mothers and fathers of 113 Chinese children ( Mage = 57.41 months, SD = 7.16 months) reported on their RCNE and child inhibitory control at Time 1 (T1) and reported on child behavioral adjustment (i.e., externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and prosocial behaviors) both at T1 and 6 months later (Time 2, T2). Multiple linear regression analyses showed that after controlling for child behavioral adjustment at T1, child age, and paternal education level, (a) paternal supportive reactions negatively predicted child externalizing problems and positively predicted child prosocial behaviors, and (b) the associations between parental RCNE and child behavioral adjustment were moderated by child inhibitory control. Specifically, only for children low on inhibitory control, paternal supportive reactions negatively predicted child externalizing problems, and maternal supportive reactions positively predicted child prosocial behaviors.
根据艾森伯格(Eisenberg)等人提出的情绪社会化启发式模型,父母对儿童负面情绪的反应(RCNE)在这一社会化过程中起着重要作用;然而,它对儿童社会情绪结果的影响可能会受到儿童气质的调节。本纵向研究通过测试母亲和父亲的 RCNE 对学龄前儿童行为适应的预测作用,以及儿童抑制控制对这些关联的调节作用,验证了这一观点。113 名中国儿童(年龄:57.41 个月,标准差:7.16 个月)的父亲和母亲在时间 1(T1)报告了他们的 RCNE 和儿童抑制控制能力,并在时间 1 和 6 个月后(时间 2,T2)报告了儿童的行为适应情况(即外化问题、内化问题和亲社会行为)。多元线性回归分析表明,在控制了儿童在第一阶段的行为适应、儿童年龄和父亲教育水平后,(a)父亲的支持性反应对儿童的外化问题有负向预测作用,而对儿童的亲社会行为有正向预测作用;(b)父母的 RCNE 与儿童行为适应之间的关联受儿童抑制性控制的调节。具体来说,只有抑制控制能力低的儿童,父亲的支持性反应才会对儿童的外部化问题产生负面影响,而母亲的支持性反应则会对儿童的亲社会行为产生正面影响。
{"title":"Parental reactions to child negative emotions and child behavioral adjustment: The moderating role of child inhibitory control","authors":"Mengjiao Wang, Xi Chen, Shanyun Zheng","doi":"10.1177/01650254241250048","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241250048","url":null,"abstract":"According to the heuristic model of emotion socialization by Eisenberg et al., parental reactions to children’s negative emotions (RCNE) have an important role in this socialization process; however, its effects on children’s social-emotional outcomes may be moderated by the children’s temperament. This longitudinal study verified this proposition by testing maternal and paternal RCNE as predictors of preschoolers’ behavioral adjustment and child inhibitory control as a moderator of these associations. Mothers and fathers of 113 Chinese children ( Mage = 57.41 months, SD = 7.16 months) reported on their RCNE and child inhibitory control at Time 1 (T1) and reported on child behavioral adjustment (i.e., externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and prosocial behaviors) both at T1 and 6 months later (Time 2, T2). Multiple linear regression analyses showed that after controlling for child behavioral adjustment at T1, child age, and paternal education level, (a) paternal supportive reactions negatively predicted child externalizing problems and positively predicted child prosocial behaviors, and (b) the associations between parental RCNE and child behavioral adjustment were moderated by child inhibitory control. Specifically, only for children low on inhibitory control, paternal supportive reactions negatively predicted child externalizing problems, and maternal supportive reactions positively predicted child prosocial behaviors.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140991803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children consider “who” and “what” when reasoning about rule changes: A comparative study of children from two cultures 儿童在推理规则变化时会考虑 "谁 "和 "什么":两种文化儿童的比较研究
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-05 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241246238
Rui Guo, Dandan Li, Xin Zhao
Children’s normative knowledge develops early. While prior studies focus on rule compliance and violation, only limited research explores children’s views on rule changes, especially in non-Western cultures. This study investigates how Chinese children aged 4–7 ( N = 154) reason about rule changes, and compares their responses with US counterparts in the work of Zhao and Kushnir. Chinese children considered both “who” created the rules and “what” consequences the rule changes may bring about when judging changeability. For game rules, like US children, Chinese children considered both individual authority (including adult and peer authority) and collective agreement when judging who can change game rules. Compared to US children, Chinese children more often believe that the adult rule-maker’s child could also change the rule. Furthermore, although both Chinese and US children thought one could not change moral rules, Chinese children were less likely than US children to think one could change conventional rules. Exploratory analyses of children’s justifications suggest that Chinese children emphasize consequences more than US children when judging whether rules can be changed. This study offers valuable insights into the development of normative reasoning and cultural influences on children’s views on rule malleability.
儿童的规范性知识发展较早。以往的研究主要关注遵守规则和违反规则的情况,但对儿童对规则变化的看法,尤其是对非西方文化中儿童的看法的研究却十分有限。本研究调查了 4-7 岁的中国儿童(154 人)是如何推理规则变化的,并将他们的反应与美国儿童的反应进行了比较。中国儿童在判断规则的可变更性时,既考虑了 "谁 "制定了规则,也考虑了规则变更可能带来的 "什么 "后果。就游戏规则而言,与美国儿童一样,中国儿童在判断谁可以更改游戏规则时,既考虑了个人权威(包括成人权威和同伴权威),也考虑了集体协议。与美国儿童相比,中国儿童更倾向于认为成人规则制定者的孩子也可以改变规则。此外,尽管中国儿童和美国儿童都认为一个人不能改变道德规则,但中国儿童比美国儿童更不可能认为一个人可以改变常规规则。对儿童理由的探索性分析表明,在判断规则是否可以改变时,中国儿童比美国儿童更强调后果。本研究为规范推理的发展以及儿童对规则可塑性看法的文化影响提供了宝贵的见解。
{"title":"Children consider “who” and “what” when reasoning about rule changes: A comparative study of children from two cultures","authors":"Rui Guo, Dandan Li, Xin Zhao","doi":"10.1177/01650254241246238","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241246238","url":null,"abstract":"Children’s normative knowledge develops early. While prior studies focus on rule compliance and violation, only limited research explores children’s views on rule changes, especially in non-Western cultures. This study investigates how Chinese children aged 4–7 ( N = 154) reason about rule changes, and compares their responses with US counterparts in the work of Zhao and Kushnir. Chinese children considered both “who” created the rules and “what” consequences the rule changes may bring about when judging changeability. For game rules, like US children, Chinese children considered both individual authority (including adult and peer authority) and collective agreement when judging who can change game rules. Compared to US children, Chinese children more often believe that the adult rule-maker’s child could also change the rule. Furthermore, although both Chinese and US children thought one could not change moral rules, Chinese children were less likely than US children to think one could change conventional rules. Exploratory analyses of children’s justifications suggest that Chinese children emphasize consequences more than US children when judging whether rules can be changed. This study offers valuable insights into the development of normative reasoning and cultural influences on children’s views on rule malleability.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141011920","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of ignoring lags on developmental science: A re-analysis of meta-analyses using lag as moderator 忽略滞后对发展科学的影响:以滞后作为调节因素的元分析再分析
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241247155
Rachel M. Taylor, Noel A. Card
Longitudinal studies provide developmental science with invaluable information about how variables and the associations between variables change across time, but typically give limited attention to the length of time over which that change occurs. The present study re-analyzed data from previously published meta-analyses of longitudinal data across a broad range of developmental science to ascertain how lag may have impacted coefficients of stability ( kmeta-analyses = 6, kstudies = 157) and prediction ( kmeta-analyses = 15, kstudies = 270). We additionally analyzed how average participant age interacts with lag to test how the impact of lag might change across the lifespan. Findings indicate that conventional lags (e.g., 6 months, 12 months) were used at extremely high rates: More than 75% of lags were selected based on convention. Linear and nonlinear models indicated that lag moderated stability and predictive associations, although the significance, magnitude, and direction of this impact changed depending on the phenomenon under investigation. Average participant age interacted with lag in certain cases, providing a possibility for more time-specific developmental theory. However, these results should not be considered conclusive due to the high number of conventional lags in our sample, which likely restricted both variability in lags and the length of those lags. Future longitudinal studies should measure phenomena at varying lags, and future meta-analysts should consider both lag and average participant age when synthesizing longitudinal research. Both practices would enable developmental science to determine the interval over which a phenomenon occurs and facilitate advancements in developmental theory.
纵向研究为发展科学提供了有关变量和变量之间的关联如何随时间变化的宝贵信息,但通常对发生变化的时间长度关注有限。本研究重新分析了以前发表的对广泛发展科学领域的纵向数据进行元分析的数据,以确定滞后对稳定性系数(kmeta-analyses = 6,kstudies = 157)和预测性系数(kmeta-analyses = 15,kstudies = 270)的影响。此外,我们还分析了参与者平均年龄与滞后期的交互作用,以检验滞后期的影响在整个生命周期中会发生怎样的变化。研究结果表明,常规滞后期(如 6 个月、12 个月)的使用率极高:超过 75% 的滞后期是根据惯例选择的。线性和非线性模型表明,滞后期对稳定性和预测性关联有调节作用,尽管这种影响的显著性、程度和方向因调查现象的不同而有所变化。在某些情况下,受试者的平均年龄与滞后期相互影响,这为更具时间特异性的发展理论提供了可能。然而,由于我们的样本中常规滞后期的数量较多,可能限制了滞后期的可变性和滞后期的长度,因此这些结果不应被认为是结论性的。未来的纵向研究应测量不同滞后期的现象,未来的元分析者在综合纵向研究时应考虑滞后期和参与者的平均年龄。这两种做法都将使发展科学能够确定现象发生的时间间隔,并促进发展理论的进步。
{"title":"The impact of ignoring lags on developmental science: A re-analysis of meta-analyses using lag as moderator","authors":"Rachel M. Taylor, Noel A. Card","doi":"10.1177/01650254241247155","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241247155","url":null,"abstract":"Longitudinal studies provide developmental science with invaluable information about how variables and the associations between variables change across time, but typically give limited attention to the length of time over which that change occurs. The present study re-analyzed data from previously published meta-analyses of longitudinal data across a broad range of developmental science to ascertain how lag may have impacted coefficients of stability ( k<jats:sub>meta-analyses</jats:sub> = 6, k<jats:sub>studies</jats:sub> = 157) and prediction ( k<jats:sub>meta-analyses</jats:sub> = 15, k<jats:sub>studies</jats:sub> = 270). We additionally analyzed how average participant age interacts with lag to test how the impact of lag might change across the lifespan. Findings indicate that conventional lags (e.g., 6 months, 12 months) were used at extremely high rates: More than 75% of lags were selected based on convention. Linear and nonlinear models indicated that lag moderated stability and predictive associations, although the significance, magnitude, and direction of this impact changed depending on the phenomenon under investigation. Average participant age interacted with lag in certain cases, providing a possibility for more time-specific developmental theory. However, these results should not be considered conclusive due to the high number of conventional lags in our sample, which likely restricted both variability in lags and the length of those lags. Future longitudinal studies should measure phenomena at varying lags, and future meta-analysts should consider both lag and average participant age when synthesizing longitudinal research. Both practices would enable developmental science to determine the interval over which a phenomenon occurs and facilitate advancements in developmental theory.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140637265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Family obligation moderates longitudinal associations between parental psychological control and adjustment of urban adolescents 家庭义务调节父母心理控制与城市青少年适应之间的纵向关系
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241233532
Leyah Christine T. Dizon, Liane Peña Alampay
This study investigated child-reported family obligation values (FOVs) in early adolescence as a moderator for associations between mother-, father-, and child-reported parental psychological control (PC) in early adolescence and child-reported internalizing and externalizing symptoms in middle and late adolescents in the Philippines. Data were drawn from three waves of a larger longitudinal study, when the Filipino youth were in late elementary grades (age M = 12.04, SD = 0.58; N = 91), in junior high school (age M = 15.03, SD = 0.59; N = 80), and in senior high school (age M = 17.00, SD = 0.59, N = 75). Results revealed that high levels of FOV buffered the positive associations between mother-reported PC and internalizing symptoms in late adolescence, and between child-reported PC and internalizing symptoms in middle and late adolescence, as well as externalizing symptoms in late adolescence. Conversely, low levels of FOV exacerbated the associations between mother- and child-reported PC on externalizing symptoms in late adolescence. Findings suggest that FOV may shape the meaning and influence of PC for children and adolescents in contexts where familial obligations are normative and important.
本研究调查了菲律宾青少年在青春期早期由儿童报告的家庭义务价值观(FOVs)作为母亲、父亲和儿童报告的青春期早期父母心理控制(PC)与儿童报告的青春期中后期内化和外化症状之间关系的调节因子的情况。数据来自一项大型纵向研究的三个波次,当时菲律宾青少年分别处于小学低年级(年龄中位数 = 12.04,标准差 = 0.58;人数 = 91)、初中(年龄中位数 = 15.03,标准差 = 0.59;人数 = 80)和高中(年龄中位数 = 17.00,标准差 = 0.59,人数 = 75)。结果显示,高水平的 FOV 可以缓冲母亲报告的 PC 与青少年晚期内化症状之间的正相关、儿童报告的 PC 与青少年中期和晚期内化症状之间的正相关以及青少年晚期外化症状之间的正相关。相反,FOV 水平低会加剧母亲和儿童报告的 PC 与青少年晚期外化症状之间的关联。研究结果表明,在家庭义务具有规范性和重要性的情况下,家庭责任感可能会影响个人防护对儿童和青少年的意义和影响。
{"title":"Family obligation moderates longitudinal associations between parental psychological control and adjustment of urban adolescents","authors":"Leyah Christine T. Dizon, Liane Peña Alampay","doi":"10.1177/01650254241233532","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241233532","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigated child-reported family obligation values (FOVs) in early adolescence as a moderator for associations between mother-, father-, and child-reported parental psychological control (PC) in early adolescence and child-reported internalizing and externalizing symptoms in middle and late adolescents in the Philippines. Data were drawn from three waves of a larger longitudinal study, when the Filipino youth were in late elementary grades (age M = 12.04, SD = 0.58; N = 91), in junior high school (age M = 15.03, SD = 0.59; N = 80), and in senior high school (age M = 17.00, SD = 0.59, N = 75). Results revealed that high levels of FOV buffered the positive associations between mother-reported PC and internalizing symptoms in late adolescence, and between child-reported PC and internalizing symptoms in middle and late adolescence, as well as externalizing symptoms in late adolescence. Conversely, low levels of FOV exacerbated the associations between mother- and child-reported PC on externalizing symptoms in late adolescence. Findings suggest that FOV may shape the meaning and influence of PC for children and adolescents in contexts where familial obligations are normative and important.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140601175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A content analysis of cognitive, emotional, and social development in popular kid’s YouTube 流行儿童 YouTube 中的认知、情感和社交发展内容分析
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241239964
Yun Jung Choi, Changsook Kim
With the explosive growth in time spent on YouTube by babies and toddlers, it’s important to analyze what they’re watching on YouTube . Indexes that evaluate the contents of YouTube channels for infants and toddlers have been developed, but since those were evaluation-based indexes of educators and parents, it is difficult to find out what content children are watching. In this study, the YouTube content that infants mainly watch were content analyzed in three developmental areas: cognition, emotion, and socialization. Specifically, language destruction was analyzed for the cognitive field, verbal and physical violence for the emotional field, and emotional expression, understanding others’ emotions, emotional control, antisocial and prosocial behavior representation, and prosocial expression were analyzed for the socialization. As a result, the emotional index was the highest, and the physical violence index was very low. In general, emotional expression, understanding of other’s emotions, and prosocial behavior, which had a positive effect on early childhood development, were higher than linguistic destruction, verbal violence, and physical violence, which had a negative effect.
随着婴幼儿在 YouTube 上花费时间的爆炸性增长,分析他们在 YouTube 上观看的内容就显得尤为重要。目前已经开发出了评估婴幼儿 YouTube 频道内容的指标,但由于这些指标都是基于教育工作者和家长的评估,因此很难了解儿童正在观看哪些内容。本研究从认知、情感和社会化三个发展领域对婴幼儿主要观看的 YouTube 内容进行了内容分析。具体来说,认知领域分析了语言破坏,情感领域分析了语言和肢体暴力,社会化领域分析了情绪表达、理解他人情绪、情绪控制、反社会和亲社会行为表现以及亲社会表达。结果,情感指数最高,身体暴力指数很低。总体而言,对幼儿发展有积极影响的情绪表达、对他人情绪的理解和亲社会行为高于有消极影响的语言破坏、语言暴力和身体暴力。
{"title":"A content analysis of cognitive, emotional, and social development in popular kid’s YouTube","authors":"Yun Jung Choi, Changsook Kim","doi":"10.1177/01650254241239964","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241239964","url":null,"abstract":"With the explosive growth in time spent on YouTube by babies and toddlers, it’s important to analyze what they’re watching on YouTube . Indexes that evaluate the contents of YouTube channels for infants and toddlers have been developed, but since those were evaluation-based indexes of educators and parents, it is difficult to find out what content children are watching. In this study, the YouTube content that infants mainly watch were content analyzed in three developmental areas: cognition, emotion, and socialization. Specifically, language destruction was analyzed for the cognitive field, verbal and physical violence for the emotional field, and emotional expression, understanding others’ emotions, emotional control, antisocial and prosocial behavior representation, and prosocial expression were analyzed for the socialization. As a result, the emotional index was the highest, and the physical violence index was very low. In general, emotional expression, understanding of other’s emotions, and prosocial behavior, which had a positive effect on early childhood development, were higher than linguistic destruction, verbal violence, and physical violence, which had a negative effect.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140602089","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unveiling the tapestry of mother–child interactions through text mining and sentiment analysis 通过文本挖掘和情感分析揭开母子互动的面纱
IF 3.7 2区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-06 DOI: 10.1177/01650254241242662
Chao Liu, Kira Waltz
The interaction between a mother and child stands as one of the most profound and intricate human connections, weaving a rich tapestry of behavioral and emotional bonds during the formative years. Although mother–child interactions have received substantial attention in the developmental science literature, few studies have tapped into the extensive corpus of speech data available to uncover the nuances of these interactions across developmental stages. This study applied text mining and sentiment analysis on narratives extracted from mother–child conversations to identify the developmental trend of mother–child interactions from early to middle childhood. The results, based on three key areas of dyadic interactions, demonstrated a shift toward more balanced turn-taking dynamics and linguistic congruence as children age. Also, there was a significant interdependence of mother and child expressed emotions across time. Further investigation of the dyadic emotionality revealed a nonlinear effect of mother-expressed emotion on child-expressed emotion: mother-expressed negative emotions followed a cubic-like pattern, while positive emotions followed a mild quadratic trend. Taken together, the findings of this study present a picture of progressive augmentation of mother–child synchrony over time.
母亲与孩子之间的互动是人类最深刻、最复杂的联系之一,在孩子的成长过程中编织了丰富的行为和情感纽带。虽然母子之间的互动在发育科学文献中受到了广泛关注,但很少有研究利用现有的大量语音数据来揭示这些互动在不同发育阶段的细微差别。本研究对从母子对话中提取的叙述进行了文本挖掘和情感分析,以确定从幼儿期到中幼儿期母子互动的发展趋向。结果表明,随着儿童年龄的增长,母子互动的三个关键领域会向更平衡的轮流动态和语言一致性转变。此外,在不同时期,母亲和孩子表达的情绪之间存在着明显的相互依存关系。进一步研究发现,母亲表达的情绪对儿童表达的情绪有非线性影响:母亲表达的消极情绪呈三次方模式,而积极情绪呈轻微的二次方趋势。综合来看,本研究的结果呈现出母子同步性随着时间的推移逐渐增强的情况。
{"title":"Unveiling the tapestry of mother–child interactions through text mining and sentiment analysis","authors":"Chao Liu, Kira Waltz","doi":"10.1177/01650254241242662","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254241242662","url":null,"abstract":"The interaction between a mother and child stands as one of the most profound and intricate human connections, weaving a rich tapestry of behavioral and emotional bonds during the formative years. Although mother–child interactions have received substantial attention in the developmental science literature, few studies have tapped into the extensive corpus of speech data available to uncover the nuances of these interactions across developmental stages. This study applied text mining and sentiment analysis on narratives extracted from mother–child conversations to identify the developmental trend of mother–child interactions from early to middle childhood. The results, based on three key areas of dyadic interactions, demonstrated a shift toward more balanced turn-taking dynamics and linguistic congruence as children age. Also, there was a significant interdependence of mother and child expressed emotions across time. Further investigation of the dyadic emotionality revealed a nonlinear effect of mother-expressed emotion on child-expressed emotion: mother-expressed negative emotions followed a cubic-like pattern, while positive emotions followed a mild quadratic trend. Taken together, the findings of this study present a picture of progressive augmentation of mother–child synchrony over time.","PeriodicalId":13880,"journal":{"name":"International Journal of Behavioral Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140601065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
International Journal of Behavioral Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1