首页 > 最新文献

Journal of the Optical Society of America最新文献

英文 中文
Nonmotor component of fusional response to vertical disparity: a second look using an afterimage method. 对垂直视差的融合反应的非运动成分:使用后像方法的第二次观察。
Pub Date : 1982-07-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000871
A L Duwaer

An afterimage method has been used to investigate the relative magnitudes of the nonmotor and motor components of the fusional response to vertical disparity in a complex visual stimulus of diameter 57 degrees consisting of 50 horizontal lines and a square of side 2.5 degrees in the middle. The largest vertical disparity that evoked a stable fusional response was found to be in the range 3-6 degrees, of which the nonmotor component amounted only to 8-15', i.e., 2-10% of the total. At these fusional amplitudes, binocular single vision was already disrupted in the foveola. When the 50 horizontal lines were omitted from the stimulus so that only the central square of side 2.5 degrees remained, the fusional amplitudes decreased by only 25% while the absolute level of the nonmotor components remained the same. The nonmotor components found here are much smaller than those (amounting to about 2 degrees, or 25-40% of the total response) reported recently in the literature.

本文采用后像法研究了由50条水平线和中间边长为2.5度的正方形组成的直径为57度的复杂视觉刺激对垂直视差的融合反应的非运动和运动分量的相对大小。引起稳定融合反应的最大垂直差异在3-6度范围内,其中非运动成分仅占8-15',即占总数的2-10%。在这些融合振幅处,双眼单一视觉在中央凹已经被破坏。当从刺激中去掉50条水平线,只留下2.5度边的中央正方形时,融合振幅仅下降25%,而非运动成分的绝对水平保持不变。这里发现的非运动成分比最近文献中报道的要小得多(约2度,或占总反应的25-40%)。
{"title":"Nonmotor component of fusional response to vertical disparity: a second look using an afterimage method.","authors":"A L Duwaer","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000871","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000871","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An afterimage method has been used to investigate the relative magnitudes of the nonmotor and motor components of the fusional response to vertical disparity in a complex visual stimulus of diameter 57 degrees consisting of 50 horizontal lines and a square of side 2.5 degrees in the middle. The largest vertical disparity that evoked a stable fusional response was found to be in the range 3-6 degrees, of which the nonmotor component amounted only to 8-15', i.e., 2-10% of the total. At these fusional amplitudes, binocular single vision was already disrupted in the foveola. When the 50 horizontal lines were omitted from the stimulus so that only the central square of side 2.5 degrees remained, the fusional amplitudes decreased by only 25% while the absolute level of the nonmotor components remained the same. The nonmotor components found here are much smaller than those (amounting to about 2 degrees, or 25-40% of the total response) reported recently in the literature.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000871","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40720711","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Localized effects of spatial frequency adaptation. 空间频率自适应的局部效应。
Pub Date : 1982-07-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000878
D W Williams, H R Wilson, J D Cowan

The spatially localized threshold-elevation aftereffect of spatial-frequency adaptation was measured by using localized, aperiodic test patterns that have bandpass Fourier transforms. At a given retinal location, the threshold-elevation curves are consistent with the fatigue of size-turned mechanisms with center-surround sensitivity profile. Only a few different sizes of such mechanisms were required to fit the local results. The local aftereffect was also measured as a function of eccentricity near the fovea. The results indicate that the threshold-elevation aftereffect of spatial-frequency adaptation is not spatially homogeneous.

利用具有带通傅里叶变换的局域化非周期测试图测量了空间频率自适应的空间局域化阈值-提升后效应。在给定的视网膜位置,阈值-仰角曲线与具有中心-环绕灵敏度剖面的尺寸旋转机构的疲劳一致。只需要几个不同尺寸的这种机构就可以满足局部结果。局部后效也被测量为中央凹附近偏心的函数。结果表明,空间频率自适应的阈值-高程后效应在空间上不均匀。
{"title":"Localized effects of spatial frequency adaptation.","authors":"D W Williams,&nbsp;H R Wilson,&nbsp;J D Cowan","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000878","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000878","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The spatially localized threshold-elevation aftereffect of spatial-frequency adaptation was measured by using localized, aperiodic test patterns that have bandpass Fourier transforms. At a given retinal location, the threshold-elevation curves are consistent with the fatigue of size-turned mechanisms with center-surround sensitivity profile. Only a few different sizes of such mechanisms were required to fit the local results. The local aftereffect was also measured as a function of eccentricity near the fovea. The results indicate that the threshold-elevation aftereffect of spatial-frequency adaptation is not spatially homogeneous.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000878","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40720712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 33
Inferring local surface orientation from motion fields. 从运动场推断局部表面方向。
Pub Date : 1982-07-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000888
D D Hoffman

The problem of inferring local surface orientation from changing images is studied computationally by deriving conditions under which the motion information is sufficient for an information processing system, biological or otherwise, to infer unique descriptions of the local surface orientation. The analysis is based on shape-from-motion proposition, which states, that given the first spatial derivatives of the orthographically projected velocity and acceleration fields of a rigidly rotating regular surface, then the angular velocity and the surface normal at each visible point on that surface are uniquely determined up to a reflection. The proof proceeds in two steps. First it is shown that surface tilt and one component of the angular velocity are uniquely determined by the first spatial derivatives of the velocity field. Then it is shown that surface slant and the remaining two components of the angular velocity are uniquely determined if the first spatial derivatives of the acceleration field are also available.

从变化的图像中推断局部表面方向的问题,通过推导运动信息足以使信息处理系统(生物或其他)推断局部表面方向的唯一描述的条件,进行了计算研究。该分析基于运动形状命题,该命题指出,给定刚性旋转规则表面的正交投影速度和加速度场的第一个空间导数,那么该表面上每个可见点的角速度和表面法线是唯一确定的,直到反射。证明分两步进行。首先证明了表面倾斜和角速度的一个分量是由速度场的一阶空间导数唯一决定的。然后证明,如果加速度场的一阶空间导数也可用,则表面斜率和角速度的其余两个分量是唯一确定的。
{"title":"Inferring local surface orientation from motion fields.","authors":"D D Hoffman","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000888","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000888","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The problem of inferring local surface orientation from changing images is studied computationally by deriving conditions under which the motion information is sufficient for an information processing system, biological or otherwise, to infer unique descriptions of the local surface orientation. The analysis is based on shape-from-motion proposition, which states, that given the first spatial derivatives of the orthographically projected velocity and acceleration fields of a rigidly rotating regular surface, then the angular velocity and the surface normal at each visible point on that surface are uniquely determined up to a reflection. The proof proceeds in two steps. First it is shown that surface tilt and one component of the angular velocity are uniquely determined by the first spatial derivatives of the velocity field. Then it is shown that surface slant and the remaining two components of the angular velocity are uniquely determined if the first spatial derivatives of the acceleration field are also available.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000888","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40720713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 60
Dichroism of the macular pigment and Haidinger's brushes. 黄斑色素和海丁格画笔的二色性。
Pub Date : 1982-06-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000734
R P Hemenger

It seems quite certain that the visual entoptic phenomenon called Haidinger's brushes is a consequence of dichroism of the macular pigment; this dichroism is usually ascribed to orientation of the pigment molecules. An alternative explanation of the dichroism, an explanation that requires no orientation of pigment molecules, investigated and shown to be feasible. Specifically, it is shown that form dichroism that is due to the structure of the Henle fiber layer can account for Haidinger's brushes if the macular pigment is almost entirely confined to the Henle layer, if individual fibers have an index about 5% higher than that of the interfiber medium, and if the fibers form a tightly packed array.

似乎可以肯定的是,被称为海丁格画笔的视觉全视现象是黄斑色素二色性的结果;这种二色性通常归因于色素分子的取向。对二色性的另一种解释,一种不需要色素分子取向的解释,进行了研究,并证明是可行的。具体来说,研究表明,如果黄斑色素几乎完全局限于Henle层,如果单个纤维的指数比纤维间介质的指数高约5%,并且纤维形成紧密排列,则由Henle纤维层结构引起的形式二色性可以解释Haidinger刷。
{"title":"Dichroism of the macular pigment and Haidinger's brushes.","authors":"R P Hemenger","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000734","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000734","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It seems quite certain that the visual entoptic phenomenon called Haidinger's brushes is a consequence of dichroism of the macular pigment; this dichroism is usually ascribed to orientation of the pigment molecules. An alternative explanation of the dichroism, an explanation that requires no orientation of pigment molecules, investigated and shown to be feasible. Specifically, it is shown that form dichroism that is due to the structure of the Henle fiber layer can account for Haidinger's brushes if the macular pigment is almost entirely confined to the Henle layer, if individual fibers have an index about 5% higher than that of the interfiber medium, and if the fibers form a tightly packed array.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000734","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35268257","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 31
Temporal aspects of color discrimination. 颜色辨别的时间方面。
Pub Date : 1982-05-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000578
E Hita, J Romero, L Jiménez del Barco, R Martínez
{"title":"Temporal aspects of color discrimination.","authors":"E Hita,&nbsp;J Romero,&nbsp;L Jiménez del Barco,&nbsp;R Martínez","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000578","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000578","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000578","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35268255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 30
Exchange thresholds for long-wavelength incremental flashes. 长波长增量闪光的交换阈值。
Pub Date : 1982-05-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000565
A Reeves

Thresholds of 1-deg, 200-msec, 641-nm foveal test flashes rise after an exchange of II5-equated 536- and 626-nm fields, taking about 30 sec to recover. Silent substitution (no rise of threshold after the exchange) occurs, however, if these fields are alternated during adaptation. Thresholds for a 1-deg, 20-msec test rise similarly after an exchange but recover in only 0.5 sec and are not influenced by alternation of the 536- and 626-nm fields. These results can be accounted for if the 641-nm tests are detected not through pathways controlled by long-wavelength cones alone but through nonopponent (20-msc) and opponent (200-msec) pathways whose sensitivities may be reduced by transient inputs from other cones.

1度、200毫秒、641纳米的中央凹测试闪光阈值在i5相当于536和626纳米的电场交换后上升,大约需要30秒才能恢复。但是,如果这些字段在适应期间交替,则会发生无声替代(交换后阈值不会上升)。1度,20毫秒测试的阈值在交换后同样上升,但仅在0.5秒内恢复,并且不受536和626纳米电场交替的影响。如果641纳米测试不是通过单独由长波长锥体控制的途径检测,而是通过非对手(20毫秒)和对手(200毫秒)途径检测,则可以解释这些结果,这些途径的灵敏度可能会因其他锥体的瞬态输入而降低。
{"title":"Exchange thresholds for long-wavelength incremental flashes.","authors":"A Reeves","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000565","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000565","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Thresholds of 1-deg, 200-msec, 641-nm foveal test flashes rise after an exchange of II5-equated 536- and 626-nm fields, taking about 30 sec to recover. Silent substitution (no rise of threshold after the exchange) occurs, however, if these fields are alternated during adaptation. Thresholds for a 1-deg, 20-msec test rise similarly after an exchange but recover in only 0.5 sec and are not influenced by alternation of the 536- and 626-nm fields. These results can be accounted for if the 641-nm tests are detected not through pathways controlled by long-wavelength cones alone but through nonopponent (20-msc) and opponent (200-msec) pathways whose sensitivities may be reduced by transient inputs from other cones.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000565","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35268254","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Refractive index of the fly rhabdomere. 苍蝇横纹肌的折射率。
Pub Date : 1982-05-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000583
D G Beersma, B J Hoenders, A M Huiser, P van Toorn

The refractive index and the diameter of the fly rhabdomere were determined by comparing the experimental results derived from interference microscopy with the results of theoretical study on the scattering of plane waves by a homogeneous, isotropic cylindrical dielectric rod. It was found that the refractive index of the isolated rhabdomere of Calliphora erythrocephala is 1.363 +/- 0.003 in an area of the rhabdomere where its diameter is calculated to be 1.32 +/- 0.04 micrometers.

通过干涉显微镜的实验结果与均匀各向同性圆柱形介质杆对平面波散射的理论研究结果的比较,确定了蝇横纹膜的折射率和直径。在横纹肌直径为1.32 +/- 0.04微米的区域内,发现赤头驼鹿横纹肌离体的折射率为1.363 +/- 0.003。
{"title":"Refractive index of the fly rhabdomere.","authors":"D G Beersma,&nbsp;B J Hoenders,&nbsp;A M Huiser,&nbsp;P van Toorn","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000583","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000583","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The refractive index and the diameter of the fly rhabdomere were determined by comparing the experimental results derived from interference microscopy with the results of theoretical study on the scattering of plane waves by a homogeneous, isotropic cylindrical dielectric rod. It was found that the refractive index of the isolated rhabdomere of Calliphora erythrocephala is 1.363 +/- 0.003 in an area of the rhabdomere where its diameter is calculated to be 1.32 +/- 0.04 micrometers.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000583","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35268256","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Homogeneity of large-field color matches in congenital red-green color deficients. 先天性红绿色缺陷大视场配色的均匀性。
Pub Date : 1982-05-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000571
A L Nagy

Rayleigh matches obtained from red-green color deficients with conventional methods show large individual differences within diagnostic categories. Similar matches obtained from the same observers with a large-field substitution method show much less variability and suggest that the differences observed among simple anomals, extreme anomals, and dichromats with conventional methods are probably not solely due to the visual pigments contained in the cones. A theory that attributes these differences to the relative number of abnormal cones present in the observer's retina is described.

用常规方法从红绿色缺陷中获得的瑞利匹配在诊断类别中显示出较大的个体差异。用大视场替代法从同一观察者处获得的相似匹配显示出更小的可变性,这表明用常规方法在简单异常、极端异常和二色视之间观察到的差异可能不仅仅是由于视锥细胞中含有的视觉色素。一种理论将这些差异归因于观察者视网膜中存在的异常视锥细胞的相对数量。
{"title":"Homogeneity of large-field color matches in congenital red-green color deficients.","authors":"A L Nagy","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000571","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000571","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rayleigh matches obtained from red-green color deficients with conventional methods show large individual differences within diagnostic categories. Similar matches obtained from the same observers with a large-field substitution method show much less variability and suggest that the differences observed among simple anomals, extreme anomals, and dichromats with conventional methods are probably not solely due to the visual pigments contained in the cones. A theory that attributes these differences to the relative number of abnormal cones present in the observer's retina is described.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000571","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"18029415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
Comparison of flicker-photometric and flicker-threshold spectral sensitivities while the eye is adapted to colored backgrounds. 眼睛适应有色背景时,闪烁光度和闪烁阈值光谱灵敏度的比较。
Pub Date : 1982-04-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000517
A Eisner
{"title":"Comparison of flicker-photometric and flicker-threshold spectral sensitivities while the eye is adapted to colored backgrounds.","authors":"A Eisner","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000517","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000517","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"18124002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Motion and vision. IV. Isotropic and anisotropic spatial responses. 运动和视觉。各向同性和各向异性空间响应。
Pub Date : 1982-04-01 DOI: 10.1364/josa.72.000432
D H Kelly

When the thresholds for periodic spatial patterns containing two or more differently oriented components (e.g., crossed gratings) are measured under normal, unstabilized conditions, each component seems to be detected almost independently of the others if their angular orientations are sufficiently different. This psychophysical behavior has been attributed to anisotropic or orientation-tuned units in the visual cortex. Here we report that when the image of such a multicomponent pattern is stabilized on the retina, the independent-detection behavior vanishes. Under stabilized-image conditions, the contrast sensitivity is governed by the maximum local contrast at the retina. The number and relative contrast of individual components, even orthogonal ones, behave almost additively in making up the threshold contrast. We confirmed this conclusion with a variety of patterns that give orientation-tuning effects in unstabilized viewing. Controlled image motion (resembling the effect of the natural drifts of the eye) restores the independent-detection behavior in every case, as do other forms of temporal modulation (e.g., flicker or flash presentations). We infer (1) that orientation-tuned units in man do not respond to unchanging stimuli--they cannot function unless the pattern on the retina is temporally modulated, and (2) in the absence of temporal modulation, spatial patterns are detected by isotropic units of relatively low sensitivity.

当在正常、不稳定的条件下测量包含两个或多个不同取向分量(例如交叉光栅)的周期性空间模式的阈值时,如果它们的角取向足够不同,则每个分量似乎几乎独立于其他分量被检测到。这种心理物理行为归因于视觉皮层的各向异性或定向调谐单元。在这里,我们报告说,当这种多组分模式的图像在视网膜上稳定时,独立检测行为消失。在稳定的图像条件下,对比灵敏度是由视网膜的最大局部对比度控制的。单个分量的数量和相对对比度,甚至是正交分量,在构成阈值对比度时几乎是相加的。我们证实了这一结论与各种模式,给予定向调谐效果在不稳定的观看。控制图像运动(类似于眼睛自然漂移的效果)在任何情况下都能恢复独立检测行为,就像其他形式的时间调制(例如,闪烁或闪光呈现)一样。我们推断(1)人类的定向调谐单元不会对不变的刺激做出反应——除非视网膜上的模式被时间调制,否则它们无法发挥作用;(2)在没有时间调制的情况下,空间模式被相对低灵敏度的各向同性单元检测到。
{"title":"Motion and vision. IV. Isotropic and anisotropic spatial responses.","authors":"D H Kelly","doi":"10.1364/josa.72.000432","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.72.000432","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When the thresholds for periodic spatial patterns containing two or more differently oriented components (e.g., crossed gratings) are measured under normal, unstabilized conditions, each component seems to be detected almost independently of the others if their angular orientations are sufficiently different. This psychophysical behavior has been attributed to anisotropic or orientation-tuned units in the visual cortex. Here we report that when the image of such a multicomponent pattern is stabilized on the retina, the independent-detection behavior vanishes. Under stabilized-image conditions, the contrast sensitivity is governed by the maximum local contrast at the retina. The number and relative contrast of individual components, even orthogonal ones, behave almost additively in making up the threshold contrast. We confirmed this conclusion with a variety of patterns that give orientation-tuning effects in unstabilized viewing. Controlled image motion (resembling the effect of the natural drifts of the eye) restores the independent-detection behavior in every case, as do other forms of temporal modulation (e.g., flicker or flash presentations). We infer (1) that orientation-tuned units in man do not respond to unchanging stimuli--they cannot function unless the pattern on the retina is temporally modulated, and (2) in the absence of temporal modulation, spatial patterns are detected by isotropic units of relatively low sensitivity.</p>","PeriodicalId":17413,"journal":{"name":"Journal of the Optical Society of America","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1982-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1364/josa.72.000432","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"18124000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 41
期刊
Journal of the Optical Society of America
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1