首页 > 最新文献

Perspectives on Psychological Science最新文献

英文 中文
The Evolution of Developmental Theories Since Piaget: A Metaview. 皮亚杰以来发展理论的演变:元视角。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231186611
Philippe Rochat

History counts and cannot be overlooked. As a case in point, the origins of major theoretical tensions in the field of developmental psychology are traced back to Piaget (1896-1980), who paved the way to major discoveries regarding the origins and development of cognition. His theory framed much of the new ideas on early cognitive development that emerged in the 1970s, in the footsteps of the 1960s' cognitive revolution. Here, I retrace major conceptual changes since Piaget and provide a metaview on empirical findings that may have triggered the call for such changes. Nine theoretical views and intuitions are identified, all in strong reaction to some or all of the four cornerstone assumptions of Piaget's developmental account (i.e., action realism, domain generality, stages, and late representation). As a result, new and more extreme stances are now taken in the nature-versus-nurture debate. These stances rest on profoundly different, often clashing theoretical intuitions that keep shaping developmental research since Piaget.

历史是不容忽视的。以皮亚杰(1896-1980 年)为例,他为认知的起源和发展方面的重大发现铺平了道路。继 20 世纪 60 年代的认知革命之后,皮亚杰的理论为 20 世纪 70 年代出现的有关早期认知发展的新观点奠定了基础。在此,我将回溯自皮亚杰以来的主要概念变革,并对可能引发这些变革的经验性发现提供一个元视角。我指出了九种理论观点和直觉,它们都是对皮亚杰发展论的四个基石假设(即行动现实主义、领域普遍性、阶段性和晚期表征)的部分或全部的强烈反应。因此,在自然与养育的争论中出现了新的、更加极端的立场。这些立场的基础是自皮亚杰以来一直影响着发展研究的截然不同、往往相互冲突的理论直觉。
{"title":"The Evolution of Developmental Theories Since Piaget: A Metaview.","authors":"Philippe Rochat","doi":"10.1177/17456916231186611","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231186611","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>History counts and cannot be overlooked. As a case in point, the origins of major theoretical tensions in the field of developmental psychology are traced back to Piaget (1896-1980), who paved the way to major discoveries regarding the origins and development of cognition. His theory framed much of the new ideas on early cognitive development that emerged in the 1970s, in the footsteps of the 1960s' cognitive revolution. Here, I retrace major conceptual changes since Piaget and provide a metaview on empirical findings that may have triggered the call for such changes. Nine theoretical views and intuitions are identified, all in strong reaction to some or all of the four cornerstone assumptions of Piaget's developmental account (i.e., action realism, domain generality, stages, and late representation). As a result, new and more extreme stances are now taken in the nature-versus-nurture debate. These stances rest on profoundly different, often clashing theoretical intuitions that keep shaping developmental research since Piaget.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10011226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Active-Inference Approach to Second-Person Neuroscience. 第二人称神经科学的主动推理方法
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-11 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231188000
Konrad Lehmann, Dimitris Bolis, Karl J Friston, Leonhard Schilbach, Maxwell J D Ramstead, Philipp Kanske

Social neuroscience has often been criticized for approaching the investigation of the neural processes that enable social interaction and cognition from a passive, detached, third-person perspective, without involving any real-time social interaction. With the emergence of second-person neuroscience, investigators have uncovered the unique complexity of neural-activation patterns in actual, real-time interaction. Social cognition that occurs during social interaction is fundamentally different from that unfolding during social observation. However, it remains unclear how the neural correlates of social interaction are to be interpreted. Here, we leverage the active-inference framework to shed light on the mechanisms at play during social interaction in second-person neuroscience studies. Specifically, we show how counterfactually rich mutual predictions, real-time bodily adaptation, and policy selection explain activation in components of the default mode, salience, and frontoparietal networks of the brain, as well as in the basal ganglia. We further argue that these processes constitute the crucial neural processes that underwrite bona fide social interaction. By placing the experimental approach of second-person neuroscience on the theoretical foundation of the active-inference framework, we inform the field of social neuroscience about the mechanisms of real-life interactions. We thereby contribute to the theoretical foundations of empirical second-person neuroscience.

社会神经科学常常被批评为是从被动、超脱、第三人称的角度来研究促成社会交往和认知的神经过程,而不涉及任何实时的社会交往。随着第二人称神经科学的出现,研究人员发现了实际实时互动中神经激活模式的独特复杂性。在社会互动过程中发生的社会认知与在社会观察过程中发生的社会认知有着本质的区别。然而,如何解释社交互动的神经相关性仍不清楚。在这里,我们利用主动推理框架来揭示第二人称神经科学研究中社交互动过程中的作用机制。具体来说,我们展示了丰富的反事实相互预测、实时身体适应和策略选择是如何解释大脑默认模式、显著性和前顶叶网络以及基底节的激活的。我们进一步认为,这些过程构成了支撑真正社会互动的关键神经过程。通过将第二人称神经科学的实验方法置于主动推理框架的理论基础之上,我们向社会神经科学领域介绍了现实生活中的互动机制。因此,我们为实证第二人称神经科学的理论基础做出了贡献。
{"title":"An Active-Inference Approach to Second-Person Neuroscience.","authors":"Konrad Lehmann, Dimitris Bolis, Karl J Friston, Leonhard Schilbach, Maxwell J D Ramstead, Philipp Kanske","doi":"10.1177/17456916231188000","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231188000","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social neuroscience has often been criticized for approaching the investigation of the neural processes that enable social interaction and cognition from a passive, detached, third-person perspective, without involving any real-time social interaction. With the emergence of <i>second-person neuroscience</i>, investigators have uncovered the unique complexity of neural-activation patterns in actual, real-time interaction. Social cognition that occurs during social interaction is fundamentally different from that unfolding during social observation. However, it remains unclear how the neural correlates of social interaction are to be interpreted. Here, we leverage the active-inference framework to shed light on the mechanisms at play during social interaction in second-person neuroscience studies. Specifically, we show how counterfactually rich mutual predictions, real-time bodily adaptation, and policy selection explain activation in components of the default mode, salience, and frontoparietal networks of the brain, as well as in the basal ganglia. We further argue that these processes constitute the crucial neural processes that underwrite bona fide social interaction. By placing the experimental approach of second-person neuroscience on the theoretical foundation of the active-inference framework, we inform the field of social neuroscience about the mechanisms of real-life interactions. We thereby contribute to the theoretical foundations of empirical second-person neuroscience.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11539477/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10346343","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Too Anecdotal to Be True? Mechanical Turk Is Not All Bots and Bad Data: Response to Webb and Tangney (2022). 轶事太假?Mechanical Turk并非都是机器人和坏数据:对 Webb 和 Tangney(2022 年)的回应》。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-07 DOI: 10.1177/17456916241234328
Melissa G Keith, Alexander S McKay

In response to Webb and Tangney (2022) we call into question the conclusion that data collected on Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) was "at best-only 2.6% valid" (p. 1). We suggest that Webb and Tangney made certain choices during the study-design and data-collection process that adversely affected the quality of the data collected. As a result, the anecdotal experience of these authors provides weak evidence that MTurk provides low-quality data as implied. In our commentary we highlight best practice recommendations and make suggestions for more effectively collecting and screening online panel data.

作为对 Webb 和 Tangney(2022 年)的回应,我们对亚马逊的 Mechanical Turk (MTurk) 上收集的数据 "最多只有 2.6% 有效"(第 1 页)这一结论提出质疑。我们认为,Webb 和 Tangney 在研究设计和数据收集过程中做出的某些选择对所收集数据的质量产生了不利影响。因此,这些作者的亲身经历无法证明 MTurk 所提供的数据质量低下。在我们的评论中,我们强调了最佳实践建议,并提出了更有效地收集和筛选在线小组数据的建议。
{"title":"Too Anecdotal to Be True? Mechanical Turk Is Not All Bots and Bad Data: Response to Webb and Tangney (2022).","authors":"Melissa G Keith, Alexander S McKay","doi":"10.1177/17456916241234328","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916241234328","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In response to Webb and Tangney (2022) we call into question the conclusion that data collected on Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) was \"at best-only 2.6% valid\" (p. 1). We suggest that Webb and Tangney made certain choices during the study-design and data-collection process that adversely affected the quality of the data collected. As a result, the anecdotal experience of these authors provides weak evidence that MTurk provides low-quality data as implied. In our commentary we highlight best practice recommendations and make suggestions for more effectively collecting and screening online panel data.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140050024","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shifting the Level of Selection in Science. 改变科学的选择水平。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231182568
Leo Tiokhin, Karthik Panchanathan, Paul E Smaldino, Daniël Lakens

Criteria for recognizing and rewarding scientists primarily focus on individual contributions. This creates a conflict between what is best for scientists' careers and what is best for science. In this article, we show how the theory of multilevel selection provides conceptual tools for modifying incentives to better align individual and collective interests. A core principle is the need to account for indirect effects by shifting the level at which selection operates from individuals to the groups in which individuals are embedded. This principle is used in several fields to improve collective outcomes, including animal husbandry, team sports, and professional organizations. Shifting the level of selection has the potential to ameliorate several problems in contemporary science, including accounting for scientists' diverse contributions to knowledge generation, reducing individual-level competition, and promoting specialization and team science. We discuss the difficulties associated with shifting the level of selection and outline directions for future development in this domain.

表彰和奖励科学家的标准主要侧重于个人贡献。这就造成了对科学家职业生涯最有利的因素与对科学最有利的因素之间的冲突。在本文中,我们将展示多层次选择理论如何为修改激励机制提供概念工具,从而更好地协调个人利益和集体利益。一个核心原则是需要考虑间接效应,将选择发挥作用的层面从个人转移到个人所在的群体。这一原则在多个领域被用于改善集体成果,包括畜牧业、团队运动和职业组织。转变选择层次有可能改善当代科学中的一些问题,包括考虑科学家对知识创造的不同贡献、减少个人层面的竞争以及促进专业化和团队科学。我们讨论了与改变选择水平相关的困难,并概述了这一领域未来的发展方向。
{"title":"Shifting the Level of Selection in Science.","authors":"Leo Tiokhin, Karthik Panchanathan, Paul E Smaldino, Daniël Lakens","doi":"10.1177/17456916231182568","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231182568","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Criteria for recognizing and rewarding scientists primarily focus on individual contributions. This creates a conflict between what is best for scientists' careers and what is best for science. In this article, we show how the theory of multilevel selection provides conceptual tools for modifying incentives to better align individual and collective interests. A core principle is the need to account for indirect effects by shifting the level at which selection operates from individuals to the groups in which individuals are embedded. This principle is used in several fields to improve collective outcomes, including animal husbandry, team sports, and professional organizations. Shifting the level of selection has the potential to ameliorate several problems in contemporary science, including accounting for scientists' diverse contributions to knowledge generation, reducing individual-level competition, and promoting specialization and team science. We discuss the difficulties associated with shifting the level of selection and outline directions for future development in this domain.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11539478/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9911809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Incomparability and Incommensurability in Choice: No Common Currency of Value? 选择中的不可比性和不可比性:没有共同的价值货币?
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-29 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231192828
Lukasz Walasek, Gordon D A Brown

Models of decision-making typically assume the existence of some common currency of value, such as utility, happiness, or inclusive fitness. This common currency is taken to allow comparison of options and to underpin everyday choice. Here we suggest instead that there is no universal value scale, that incommensurable values pervade everyday choice, and hence that most existing models of decision-making in both economics and psychology are fundamentally limited. We propose that choice objects can be compared only with reference to specific but nonuniversal "covering values." These covering values may reflect decision-makers' goals, motivations, or current states. A complete model of choice must accommodate the range of possible covering values. We show that abandoning the common-currency assumption in models of judgment and decision-making necessitates rank-based and "simple heuristics" models that contrast radically with conventional utility-based approaches. We note that if there is no universal value scale, then Arrow's impossibility theorem places severe bounds on the rationality of individual decision-making and hence that there is a deep link between the incommensurability of value, inconsistencies in human decision-making, and rank-based coding of value. More generally, incommensurability raises the question of whether it will ever be possible to develop single-quantity-maximizing models of decision-making.

决策模型通常假定存在某种共同的价值货币,如效用、幸福感或包容性健康。这种共同货币被认为可以对各种选择进行比较,并成为日常选择的基础。而在这里,我们认为并不存在通用的价值尺度,不可比拟的价值充斥着日常选择,因此经济学和心理学中现有的大多数决策模型都存在根本性的局限。我们提出,只有参照特定但非普遍的 "覆盖价值",才能对选择对象进行比较。这些覆盖值可能反映了决策者的目标、动机或当前状态。一个完整的选择模型必须包含一系列可能的覆盖值。我们表明,在判断和决策模型中放弃通用货币假设,就必须采用基于等级的模型和 "简单启发式 "模型,这与传统的基于效用的方法截然不同。我们注意到,如果不存在通用的价值尺度,那么阿罗的不可能性定理就会对个人决策的合理性施加严格的限制,因此,价值的不可通约性、人类决策的不一致性以及基于等级的价值编码之间存在着深刻的联系。更广义地说,不可通约性提出了一个问题,即是否有可能建立单一数量最大化的决策模型。
{"title":"Incomparability and Incommensurability in Choice: No Common Currency of Value?","authors":"Lukasz Walasek, Gordon D A Brown","doi":"10.1177/17456916231192828","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231192828","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Models of decision-making typically assume the existence of some common currency of value, such as utility, happiness, or inclusive fitness. This common currency is taken to allow comparison of options and to underpin everyday choice. Here we suggest instead that there is no universal value scale, that incommensurable values pervade everyday choice, and hence that most existing models of decision-making in both economics and psychology are fundamentally limited. We propose that choice objects can be compared only with reference to specific but nonuniversal \"covering values.\" These covering values may reflect decision-makers' goals, motivations, or current states. A complete model of choice must accommodate the range of possible covering values. We show that abandoning the common-currency assumption in models of judgment and decision-making necessitates rank-based and \"simple heuristics\" models that contrast radically with conventional utility-based approaches. We note that if there is no universal value scale, then Arrow's impossibility theorem places severe bounds on the rationality of individual decision-making and hence that there is a deep link between the incommensurability of value, inconsistencies in human decision-making, and rank-based coding of value. More generally, incommensurability raises the question of whether it will ever be possible to develop single-quantity-maximizing models of decision-making.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11539466/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10112260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Critical Perspective on Neural Mechanisms in Cognitive Neuroscience: Towards Unification. 认知神经科学中神经机制的批判性视角:走向统一。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-29 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231191744
Sander van Bree

A central pursuit of cognitive neuroscience is to find neural mechanisms of cognition, with research programs favoring different strategies to look for them. But what is a neural mechanism, and how do we know we have captured them? Here I answer these questions through a framework that integrates Marr's levels with philosophical work on mechanism. From this, the following goal emerges: What needs to be explained are the computations of cognition, with explanation itself given by mechanism-composed of algorithms and parts of the brain that realize them. This reveals a delineation within cognitive neuroscience research. In the premechanism stage, the computations of cognition are linked to phenomena in the brain, narrowing down where and when mechanisms are situated in space and time. In the mechanism stage, it is established how computation emerges from organized interactions between parts-filling the premechanistic mold. I explain why a shift toward mechanistic modeling helps us meet our aims while outlining a road map for doing so. Finally, I argue that the explanatory scope of neural mechanisms can be approximated by effect sizes collected across studies, not just conceptual analysis. Together, these points synthesize a mechanistic agenda that allows subfields to connect at the level of theory.

认知神经科学的核心追求是找到认知的神经机制,不同的研究计划倾向于采用不同的策略来寻找这些机制。但什么是神经机制,我们又如何知道我们已经捕捉到了它们?在这里,我将通过一个框架来回答这些问题,这个框架将马尔的层次与关于机制的哲学工作结合在一起。由此,我提出了以下目标:需要解释的是认知的计算,而解释本身则由机制给出--机制由算法和实现算法的大脑部分组成。这揭示了认知神经科学研究中的一种划分。在前机制阶段,认知的计算与大脑中的现象相联系,缩小了机制在空间和时间上的位置和时间。在机制阶段,计算是如何从各部分之间有组织的相互作用中产生的,这就完成了前机制阶段的模型。我将解释为什么转向机制建模有助于我们实现目标,同时勾勒出实现目标的路线图。最后,我认为神经机制的解释范围可以通过跨研究收集的效应大小来近似,而不仅仅是概念分析。这些观点共同构成了一个机制议程,使各子领域能够在理论层面相互连接。
{"title":"A Critical Perspective on Neural Mechanisms in Cognitive Neuroscience: Towards Unification.","authors":"Sander van Bree","doi":"10.1177/17456916231191744","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231191744","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A central pursuit of cognitive neuroscience is to find neural mechanisms of cognition, with research programs favoring different strategies to look for them. But what is a neural mechanism, and how do we know we have captured them? Here I answer these questions through a framework that integrates Marr's levels with philosophical work on mechanism. From this, the following goal emerges: What needs to be explained are the computations of cognition, with explanation itself given by mechanism-composed of algorithms and parts of the brain that realize them. This reveals a delineation within cognitive neuroscience research. In the <i>premechanism stage</i>, the computations of cognition are linked to phenomena in the brain, narrowing down where and when mechanisms are situated in space and time. In the <i>mechanism stage</i>, it is established how computation emerges from organized interactions between parts-filling the premechanistic mold. I explain why a shift toward mechanistic modeling helps us meet our aims while outlining a road map for doing so. Finally, I argue that the explanatory scope of neural mechanisms can be approximated by effect sizes collected across studies, not just conceptual analysis. Together, these points synthesize a mechanistic agenda that allows subfields to connect at the level of theory.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11539489/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10114217","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Talking About the Absent and the Abstract: Referential Communication in Language and Gesture. 谈论缺席者和摘要:语言和手势中的参照交流。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-21 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231180589
Elena Luchkina, Sandra Waxman

Human language permits us to call to mind objects, events, and ideas that we cannot witness directly, either because they are absent or because they have no physical form (e.g., people we have not met, concepts like justice). What enables language to transmit such knowledge? We propose that a referential link between words, referents, and mental representations of those referents is key. This link enables us to form, access, and modify mental representations even when the referents themselves are absent ("absent reference"). In this review we consider the developmental and evolutionary origins of absent reference, integrating previously disparate literatures on absent reference in language and gesture in very young humans and gesture in nonhuman primates. We first evaluate when and how infants acquire absent reference during the process of language acquisition. With this as a foundation, we consider the evidence for absent reference in gesture in infants and in nonhuman primates. Finally, having woven these literatures together, we highlight new lines of research that promise to sharpen our understanding of the development of reference and its role in learning about the absent and the abstract.

人类语言使我们能够唤起那些我们无法直接目睹的对象、事件和观念,或者因为它们不存在,或者因为它们没有物理形式(例如,我们没有见过的人、像正义这样的概念)。是什么让语言能够传递这些知识呢?我们认为,词语、参照物和这些参照物的心理表征之间的参照联系是关键所在。这种联系使我们能够形成、访问和修改心理表征,即使参照物本身并不存在("缺失参照")。在这篇综述中,我们考虑了缺失参照的发展和进化起源,整合了以前关于幼年人类语言和手势中的缺失参照以及非人灵长类动物手势中的缺失参照的不同文献。我们首先评估了婴儿在语言习得过程中何时以及如何获得缺失参照。在此基础上,我们考虑了婴儿和非人灵长类动物手势中缺失参照的证据。最后,在将这些文献交织在一起后,我们强调了新的研究方向,这些方向有望加深我们对参照的发展及其在学习无参照和抽象概念中的作用的理解。
{"title":"Talking About the Absent and the Abstract: Referential Communication in Language and Gesture.","authors":"Elena Luchkina, Sandra Waxman","doi":"10.1177/17456916231180589","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231180589","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human language permits us to call to mind objects, events, and ideas that we cannot witness directly, either because they are absent or because they have no physical form (e.g., people we have not met, concepts like justice). What enables language to transmit such knowledge? We propose that a referential link between words, referents, and mental representations of those referents is key. This link enables us to form, access, and modify mental representations even when the referents themselves are absent (\"absent reference\"). In this review we consider the developmental and evolutionary origins of absent reference, integrating previously disparate literatures on absent reference in language and gesture in very young humans and gesture in nonhuman primates. We first evaluate when and how infants acquire absent reference during the process of language acquisition. With this as a foundation, we consider the evidence for absent reference in gesture in infants and in nonhuman primates. Finally, having woven these literatures together, we highlight new lines of research that promise to sharpen our understanding of the development of reference and its role in learning about the absent and the abstract.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10879458/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10032511","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How the Complexity of Psychological Processes Reframes the Issue of Reproducibility in Psychological Science. 心理过程的复杂性如何重塑心理科学的可重复性问题》(How the Complexity of Psychological Processes Reframes the Issue of Reproducibility in Psychological Science)。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-14 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231187324
Christophe Gernigon, Ruud J R Den Hartigh, Robin R Vallacher, Paul L C van Geert

In the past decade, various recommendations have been published to enhance the methodological rigor and publication standards in psychological science. However, adhering to these recommendations may have limited impact on the reproducibility of causal effects as long as psychological phenomena continue to be viewed as decomposable into separate and additive statistical structures of causal relationships. In this article, we show that (a) psychological phenomena are patterns emerging from nondecomposable and nonisolable complex processes that obey idiosyncratic nonlinear dynamics, (b) these processual features jeopardize the chances of standard reproducibility of statistical results, and (c) these features call on researchers to reconsider what can and should be reproduced, that is, the psychological processes per se, and the signatures of their complexity and dynamics. Accordingly, we argue for a greater consideration of process causality of psychological phenomena reflected by key properties of complex dynamical systems (CDSs). This implies developing and testing formal models of psychological dynamics, which can be implemented by computer simulation. The scope of the CDS paradigm and its convergences with other paradigms are discussed regarding the reproducibility issue. Ironically, the CDS approach could account for both reproducibility and nonreproducibility of the statistical effects usually sought in mainstream psychological science.

在过去的十年中,为了提高心理科学方法的严谨性和出版标准,已经发布了各种建议。然而,只要心理现象继续被视为可分解为独立的、可相加的因果关系统计结构,那么遵守这些建议对因果效应的可重复性可能影响有限。在本文中,我们将说明:(a) 心理现象是由不可分解和不可隔离的复杂过程所产生的模式,这些过程服从于特异的非线性动力学;(b) 这些过程性特征会危及统计结果的标准可重复性;(c) 这些特征要求研究人员重新考虑什么是可以且应该被重现的,即心理过程本身及其复杂性和动力学特征。因此,我们主张更多地考虑复杂动力系统(CDS)的关键特性所反映的心理现象的过程因果关系。这意味着要开发和测试可通过计算机模拟实现的心理动力学正规模型。在可重复性问题上,讨论了 CDS 范式的范围及其与其他范式的趋同性。具有讽刺意味的是,CDS 方法可以解释主流心理科学通常寻求的统计效应的可再现性和不可再现性。
{"title":"How the Complexity of Psychological Processes Reframes the Issue of Reproducibility in Psychological Science.","authors":"Christophe Gernigon, Ruud J R Den Hartigh, Robin R Vallacher, Paul L C van Geert","doi":"10.1177/17456916231187324","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231187324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the past decade, various recommendations have been published to enhance the methodological rigor and publication standards in psychological science. However, adhering to these recommendations may have limited impact on the reproducibility of causal effects as long as psychological phenomena continue to be viewed as decomposable into separate and additive statistical structures of causal relationships. In this article, we show that (a) psychological phenomena are patterns emerging from nondecomposable and nonisolable complex processes that obey idiosyncratic nonlinear dynamics, (b) these processual features jeopardize the chances of standard reproducibility of statistical results, and (c) these features call on researchers to reconsider what can and should be reproduced, that is, the psychological processes per se, and the signatures of their complexity and dynamics. Accordingly, we argue for a greater consideration of <i>process causality</i> of psychological phenomena reflected by key properties of complex dynamical systems (CDSs). This implies developing and testing formal models of psychological dynamics, which can be implemented by computer simulation. The scope of the CDS paradigm and its convergences with other paradigms are discussed regarding the reproducibility issue. Ironically, the CDS approach could account for <i>both</i> reproducibility and nonreproducibility of the statistical effects usually sought in mainstream psychological science.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9993774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality Science in the Digital Age: The Promises and Challenges of Psychological Targeting for Personalized Behavior-Change Interventions at Scale. 数字时代的人格科学:大规模个性化行为改变干预的心理定位承诺与挑战》(Personality Science in the Digital Age: The Promises and Challenges of Psychological Targeting for Personalized Behavior Change Interventions at Scale)。
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-29 DOI: 10.1177/17456916231191774
Sandra C Matz, Emorie D Beck, Olivia E Atherton, Mike White, John F Rauthmann, Dan K Mroczek, Minhee Kim, Tim Bogg

With the rapidly growing availability of scalable psychological assessments, personality science holds great promise for the scientific study and applied use of customized behavior-change interventions. To facilitate this development, we propose a classification system that divides psychological targeting into two approaches that differ in the process by which interventions are designed: audience-to-content matching or content-to-audience matching. This system is both integrative and generative: It allows us to (a) integrate existing research on personalized interventions from different psychological subdisciplines (e.g., political, educational, organizational, consumer, and clinical and health psychology) and to (b) articulate open questions that generate promising new avenues for future research. Our objective is to infuse personality science into intervention research and encourage cross-disciplinary collaborations within and outside of psychology. To ensure the development of personality-customized interventions aligns with the broader interests of individuals (and society at large), we also address important ethical considerations for the use of psychological targeting (e.g., privacy, self-determination, and equity) and offer concrete guidelines for researchers and practitioners.

随着可扩展的心理评估技术的迅速发展,人格科学为定制行为改变干预措施的科学研究和应用带来了巨大希望。为了促进这一发展,我们提出了一个分类系统,将心理定位分为两种方法,这两种方法在设计干预措施的过程中有所不同:受众与内容匹配或内容与受众匹配。这一系统既具有整合性,又具有生成性:它使我们能够(a)整合来自不同心理学分支学科(如政治、教育、组织、消费者、临床和健康心理学)的有关个性化干预的现有研究,以及(b)阐明开放性问题,为未来研究开辟有前景的新途径。我们的目标是将人格科学渗透到干预研究中,并鼓励心理学内外的跨学科合作。为了确保个性定制干预的发展与个人(以及整个社会)的更广泛利益相一致,我们还讨论了使用心理定位的重要伦理考虑(如隐私、自我决定和公平),并为研究人员和从业人员提供了具体的指导方针。
{"title":"Personality Science in the Digital Age: The Promises and Challenges of Psychological Targeting for Personalized Behavior-Change Interventions at Scale.","authors":"Sandra C Matz, Emorie D Beck, Olivia E Atherton, Mike White, John F Rauthmann, Dan K Mroczek, Minhee Kim, Tim Bogg","doi":"10.1177/17456916231191774","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916231191774","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>With the rapidly growing availability of scalable psychological assessments, personality science holds great promise for the scientific study and applied use of customized behavior-change interventions. To facilitate this development, we propose a classification system that divides psychological targeting into two approaches that differ in the process by which interventions are designed: audience-to-content matching or content-to-audience matching. This system is both integrative and generative: It allows us to (a) integrate existing research on personalized interventions from different psychological subdisciplines (e.g., political, educational, organizational, consumer, and clinical and health psychology) and to (b) articulate open questions that generate promising new avenues for future research. Our objective is to infuse personality science into intervention research and encourage cross-disciplinary collaborations within and outside of psychology. To ensure the development of personality-customized interventions aligns with the broader interests of individuals (and society at large), we also address important ethical considerations for the use of psychological targeting (e.g., privacy, self-determination, and equity) and offer concrete guidelines for researchers and practitioners.</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10114215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Too Good to Be True: Bots and Bad Data From Mechanical Turk. 好得不真实:来自 Mechanical Turk 的机器人和不良数据》(Too Good to Be True: Bots and Bad Data from Mechanical Turk.
IF 10.5 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2022-11-07 DOI: 10.1177/17456916221120027
Margaret A Webb, June P Tangney

Psychology is moving increasingly toward digital sources of data, with Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) at the forefront of that charge. In 2015, up to an estimated 45% of articles published in the top behavioral and social science journals included at least one study conducted on MTurk. In this article, I summarize my own experience with MTurk and how I deduced that my sample was-at best-only 2.6% valid, by my estimate. I share these results as a warning and call for caution. Recently, I conducted an online study via Amazon's MTurk, eager and excited to collect my own data for the first time as a doctoral student. What resulted has prompted me to write this as a warning: it is indeed too good to be true. This is a summary of how I determined that, at best, I had gathered valid data from 14 human beings-2.6% of my participant sample (N = 529).

心理学正越来越多地转向数字数据源,亚马逊的 Mechanical Turk(MTurk)就是其中的佼佼者。2015 年,在顶级行为和社会科学期刊上发表的文章中,估计有高达 45% 的文章包含了至少一项在 MTurk 上进行的研究。在这篇文章中,我总结了自己在MTurk上的经验,以及我如何推断出我的样本--根据我的估计,最多只有2.6%是有效的。我分享这些结果是为了警示和呼吁大家谨慎行事。最近,我通过亚马逊的 MTurk 开展了一项在线研究,作为一名博士生,我第一次渴望并兴奋地收集自己的数据。结果促使我写下这篇文章以示警告:这的确好得不像真的。本文总结了我是如何确定我最多只收集到了 14 个人的有效数据--占我的参与者样本(N = 529)的 2.6%。
{"title":"Too Good to Be True: Bots and Bad Data From Mechanical Turk.","authors":"Margaret A Webb, June P Tangney","doi":"10.1177/17456916221120027","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17456916221120027","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Psychology is moving increasingly toward digital sources of data, with Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) at the forefront of that charge. In 2015, up to an estimated 45% of articles published in the top behavioral and social science journals included at least one study conducted on MTurk. In this article, I summarize my own experience with MTurk and how I deduced that my sample was-at best-only 2.6% valid, by my estimate. I share these results as a warning and call for caution. Recently, I conducted an online study via Amazon's MTurk, eager and excited to collect my own data for the first time as a doctoral student. What resulted has prompted me to write this as a warning: it is indeed too good to be true. This is a summary of how I determined that, at best, I had gathered valid data from 14 human beings-2.6% of my participant sample (<i>N</i> = 529).</p>","PeriodicalId":19757,"journal":{"name":"Perspectives on Psychological Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.5,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40452649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Perspectives on Psychological Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1