首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Development of the Basel Version of the Awareness of Social Inference Test – Theory of Mind (BASIT-ToM) in healthy adults 健康成人社会推理意识测试-心理理论(BASIT-ToM)巴塞尔版本的开发
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-09-21 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12290
Marianne Jarsch, Olivier Piguet, Manfred Berres, Constantin Sluka, Anna Semenkova, Reto W. Kressig, Andreas U. Monsch, Skye McDonald, Marc Sollberger

Impairments of Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities occur in a wide range of brain disorders. Therefore, reliable and ecologically valid examination of these abilities is a crucial part of any comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. An established and ecologically valid, English-language test identifying deficits in ToM abilities is “The Awareness of Social Inference Test – Social Inference Minimal (TASIT-SIM)”. However, no comparable German-language ToM test currently exists. In this study, we aimed to develop the first German-language adaption of TASIT-SIM in healthy adults. We selected 13 scenes [four scenes per message type (i.e., honesty, simple sarcasm, paradoxical sarcasm) and one practice scene] out of the 30 TASIT-SIM scenes. In collaboration with a film institute, we filmed each scene at three different intensities. These intensity version scenes were then administered to 240 healthy adults, equally distributed in sex and age, ranging from 35 to 92 years. By applying Rasch analysis, we selected intensity versions that showed neither floor nor ceiling effects in the majority of ToM questions in participants whose ToM abilities were in the medium range. In conclusion, we have developed the first German-language adaption of TASIT-SIM, i.e., the “Basel Version of the Awareness of Social Inference Test – Theory of Mind (BASIT-ToM)”. The BASIT-ToM incorporates the strengths of TASIT-SIM, while overcoming its limitations such as inconsistencies in cinematic realization and ceiling effects in healthy participants. Next, the BASIT-ToM needs to be validated in healthy people and clinical populations.

心理理论(ToM)能力的损害发生在广泛的大脑疾病中。因此,对这些能力进行可靠和生态有效的检查是任何综合神经心理学评估的关键部分。“社会推理意识测试-最小社会推理(TASIT-SIM)”是一种确定ToM能力缺陷的已建立且生态有效的英语测试。然而,目前没有类似的德语ToM测试。在这项研究中,我们的目的是在健康成人中开发第一个德语适应的TASIT-SIM。我们从30个TASIT-SIM场景中选择了13个场景[每个消息类型(即诚实,简单讽刺,矛盾讽刺)四个场景和一个练习场景]。我们与一家电影学院合作,用三种不同的强度拍摄每个场景。然后对240名健康成年人进行了这些强度版本的场景,这些成年人按性别和年龄平均分布,年龄从35岁到92岁不等。通过应用Rasch分析,我们选择了在大多数ToM能力处于中等范围的参与者的问题中既没有地板效应也没有天花板效应的强度版本。综上所述,我们开发了第一个TASIT-SIM的德语版本,即“社会推理意识测试-心理理论(BASIT-ToM)的巴塞尔版本”。BASIT-ToM结合了TASIT-SIM的优点,同时克服了TASIT-SIM的局限性,例如在电影实现和健康参与者的天花板效果方面的不一致性。下一步,BASIT-ToM需要在健康人群和临床人群中进行验证。
{"title":"Development of the Basel Version of the Awareness of Social Inference Test – Theory of Mind (BASIT-ToM) in healthy adults","authors":"Marianne Jarsch,&nbsp;Olivier Piguet,&nbsp;Manfred Berres,&nbsp;Constantin Sluka,&nbsp;Anna Semenkova,&nbsp;Reto W. Kressig,&nbsp;Andreas U. Monsch,&nbsp;Skye McDonald,&nbsp;Marc Sollberger","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12290","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12290","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Impairments of Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities occur in a wide range of brain disorders. Therefore, reliable and ecologically valid examination of these abilities is a crucial part of any comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. An established and ecologically valid, English-language test identifying deficits in ToM abilities is “The Awareness of Social Inference Test – Social Inference Minimal (TASIT-SIM)”. However, no comparable German-language ToM test currently exists. In this study, we aimed to develop the first German-language adaption of TASIT-SIM in healthy adults. We selected 13 scenes [four scenes per message type (i.e<i>.</i>, honesty, simple sarcasm, paradoxical sarcasm) and one practice scene] out of the 30 TASIT-SIM scenes. In collaboration with a film institute, we filmed each scene at three different intensities. These intensity version scenes were then administered to 240 healthy adults, equally distributed in sex and age, ranging from 35 to 92 years. By applying Rasch analysis, we selected intensity versions that showed neither floor nor ceiling effects in the majority of ToM questions in participants whose ToM abilities were in the medium range. In conclusion, we have developed the first German-language adaption of TASIT-SIM, i.e<i>.</i>, the “Basel Version of the Awareness of Social Inference Test – Theory of Mind (BASIT-ToM)”. The BASIT-ToM incorporates the strengths of TASIT-SIM, while overcoming its limitations such as inconsistencies in cinematic realization and ceiling effects in healthy participants. Next, the BASIT-ToM needs to be validated in healthy people and clinical populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"125-145"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12290","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5797485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of conventional versus robust norming on cognitive characterization and clinical classification of MCI and dementia 传统与稳健规范对轻度认知损伤和痴呆的认知特征和临床分类的影响
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-09-19 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12289
Alyssa N. Kaser, David M. Kaplan, William Goette, Andrew M. Kiselica

We examined the impact of conventional versus robust normative approaches on cognitive characterization and clinical classification of MCI versus dementia. The sample included participants from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set. Separate demographically adjusted z-scores for cognitive tests were derived from conventional (n = 4273) and robust (n = 602) normative groups. To assess the impact of deriving scores from a conventional versus robust normative group on cognitive characterization, we examined likelihood of having a low score on each neuropsychological test. Next, we created receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves for the ability of normed scores derived from each normative group to differentiate between MCI (n = 3570) and dementia (n = 1564). We examined the impact of choice of normative group on classification accuracy by comparing sensitivity and specificity values and areas under the curves (AUC). Compared with using a conventional normative group, using a robust normative group resulted in a higher likelihood of low cognitive scores for individuals classified with MCI and dementia. Comparison of the classification accuracy for distinguishing MCI from dementia did not suggest a statistically significant advantage for either normative approach (Z = −0.29, p = .77; AUC = 0.86 for conventional and AUC = 0.86 for robust). In summary, these results indicate that using a robust normative group increases the likelihood of characterizing cognitive performance as low. However, there is not a clear advantage of using a robust over a conventional normative group when differentiating between MCI and dementia.

我们研究了传统与稳健的规范方法对认知特征和MCI与痴呆的临床分类的影响。样本包括来自国家阿尔茨海默病协调中心统一数据集的参与者。分别从常规(n = 4273)和稳健(n = 602)规范组中得出经人口统计学调整的认知测试z分数。为了评估从常规组和稳健的规范组中获得分数对认知特征的影响,我们检查了在每个神经心理测试中获得低分的可能性。接下来,我们创建了受试者工作特征(ROC)曲线,用于区分每个规范组的标准化评分(n = 3570)和痴呆(n = 1564)的能力。我们通过比较敏感性和特异性值以及曲线下面积(AUC)来检验标准组选择对分类准确性的影响。与使用传统的规范组相比,使用稳健的规范组导致MCI和痴呆个体认知得分低的可能性更高。比较区分轻度认知障碍和痴呆的分类准确度,两种标准方法都没有统计学上的显著优势(Z = - 0.29, p = .77;常规AUC = 0.86,稳健AUC = 0.86)。综上所述,这些结果表明,使用稳健的规范组增加了将认知表现定性为低的可能性。然而,在区分MCI和痴呆时,使用鲁棒组并没有明显的优势。
{"title":"The impact of conventional versus robust norming on cognitive characterization and clinical classification of MCI and dementia","authors":"Alyssa N. Kaser,&nbsp;David M. Kaplan,&nbsp;William Goette,&nbsp;Andrew M. Kiselica","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12289","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12289","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We examined the impact of conventional versus robust normative approaches on cognitive characterization and clinical classification of MCI versus dementia. The sample included participants from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set. Separate demographically adjusted <i>z</i>-scores for cognitive tests were derived from conventional (<i>n</i> = 4273) and robust (<i>n</i> = 602) normative groups. To assess the impact of deriving scores from a conventional versus robust normative group on cognitive characterization, we examined likelihood of having a low score on each neuropsychological test. Next, we created receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves for the ability of normed scores derived from each normative group to differentiate between MCI (<i>n</i> = 3570) and dementia (<i>n</i> = 1564). We examined the impact of choice of normative group on classification accuracy by comparing sensitivity and specificity values and areas under the curves (AUC). Compared with using a conventional normative group, using a robust normative group resulted in a higher likelihood of low cognitive scores for individuals classified with MCI and dementia. Comparison of the classification accuracy for distinguishing MCI from dementia did not suggest a statistically significant advantage for either normative approach (<i>Z</i> = −0.29, <i>p</i> = .77; AUC = 0.86 for conventional and AUC = 0.86 for robust). In summary, these results indicate that using a robust normative group increases the likelihood of characterizing cognitive performance as low. However, there is not a clear advantage of using a robust over a conventional normative group when differentiating between MCI and dementia.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"108-124"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12289","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5735827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Autobiographical memory impairment in chronic schizophrenia: Significance and clinical correlates 慢性精神分裂症的自传式记忆障碍:意义和临床相关性
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-09-05 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12288
Christina J. Herold, Marc M. L?sser, Johannes Schr?der

Previous studies of autobiographical memory (AM) in schizophrenia yielded a reduction of specificity, richness of details and conscious recollection, which indicate both, quantitative and qualitative AM changes. However, their associations with psychopathological symptoms and neuropsychological deficits were not resolved. Therefore, we sought to investigate AM with respect to psychopathology and neuropsychology in patients with chronic schizophrenia to rule out the influence of different courses of the disease. AM of four lifetime periods was examined in 75 patients and 50 healthy controls by using a semi-structured interview. The recalled episodes were rated for memory specificity. Subsequently, one single event of each period of life was rated for details and experiential aspects of reliving (originality, vividness/visual imagery, emotional re-experiencing and emotional valence). When contrasted with healthy controls, patients recalled a significantly reduced number of episodes and personal semantic facts; moreover, memory specificity of AM was significantly lower in patients than controls. While the richness of details calculated for single events showed only minor, non-significant group differences, vividness and emotional re-experiencing were significantly less pronounced in the patient group. Along with this, AM performance correlated significantly with negative symptoms including apathy as well as verbal memory and executive functions. Our results underline the significance of overgenerality as a key feature of AM in schizophrenia as well as a dissociation between intact number of details of single events and reduced vividness and emotional re-experiencing. The extent of negative symptoms including apathy and impairments of verbal memory/executive functions may explain AM deficits in chronic schizophrenia.

以往对精神分裂症患者自传体记忆(AM)的研究发现,其特异性、细节丰富性和有意识的回忆都有所减少,这表明AM在定量和定性上都发生了变化。然而,它们与精神病理症状和神经心理缺陷的关系尚未得到解决。因此,我们试图调查慢性精神分裂症患者的精神病理学和神经心理学方面的AM,以排除不同病程的影响。采用半结构化访谈法,对75例患者和50例健康对照者进行了四个生命周期的AM检测。这些回忆的片段被根据记忆特异性进行评分。随后,对生活中每个时期的一个事件进行细节和体验方面的评分(原创性、生动性/视觉意象、情感再体验和情感效价)。与健康对照组相比,患者回忆的事件次数和个人语义事实显著减少;此外,AM的记忆特异性明显低于对照组。虽然对单个事件计算的细节丰富性只显示出轻微的、不显著的组间差异,但在患者组中,生动度和情绪再体验明显不那么明显。与此同时,AM表现与消极症状(包括冷漠、言语记忆和执行功能)显著相关。我们的研究结果强调了过度概括作为精神分裂症AM的一个关键特征的重要性,以及单个事件的完整细节数量与减少的生动性和情感再体验之间的分离。包括冷漠和言语记忆/执行功能损伤在内的阴性症状的程度可以解释慢性精神分裂症的AM缺陷。
{"title":"Autobiographical memory impairment in chronic schizophrenia: Significance and clinical correlates","authors":"Christina J. Herold,&nbsp;Marc M. L?sser,&nbsp;Johannes Schr?der","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12288","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12288","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous studies of autobiographical memory (AM) in schizophrenia yielded a reduction of specificity, richness of details and conscious recollection, which indicate both, quantitative and qualitative AM changes. However, their associations with psychopathological symptoms and neuropsychological deficits were not resolved. Therefore, we sought to investigate AM with respect to psychopathology and neuropsychology in patients with chronic schizophrenia to rule out the influence of different courses of the disease. AM of four lifetime periods was examined in 75 patients and 50 healthy controls by using a semi-structured interview. The recalled episodes were rated for memory specificity. Subsequently, one single event of each period of life was rated for details and experiential aspects of reliving (originality, vividness/visual imagery, emotional re-experiencing and emotional valence). When contrasted with healthy controls, patients recalled a significantly reduced number of episodes and personal semantic facts; moreover, memory specificity of AM was significantly lower in patients than controls. While the richness of details calculated for single events showed only minor, non-significant group differences, vividness and emotional re-experiencing were significantly less pronounced in the patient group. Along with this, AM performance correlated significantly with negative symptoms including apathy as well as verbal memory and executive functions. Our results underline the significance of overgenerality as a key feature of AM in schizophrenia as well as a dissociation between intact number of details of single events and reduced vividness and emotional re-experiencing. The extent of negative symptoms including apathy and impairments of verbal memory/executive functions may explain AM deficits in chronic schizophrenia.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"89-107"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12288","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"6109540","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Are the inhibitory and faciliatory effects during retrieval of semantically related items present in amnestic mild cognitive impairment? 遗忘性轻度认知障碍患者在检索语义相关项目时是否存在抑制和促进效应?
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-08-15 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12286
Laura Serra, Giulia Bechi Gabrielli, Carlotta Di Domenico, Chiara Del Bono, Camillo Marra, Leonardo Lopiano, Carlo Caltagirone, Marco Bozzali

Prefrontal functions subserve inhibition control for retrieval of semantically related items inducing forgetting 19 a-MCI patients and 29 controls underwent neuropsychological evaluation and retrieval-practice paradigm (RPP) to estimate baseline remember (BR), retrieval-induced facilitation (FAC) and retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF). A-MCI patients underwent also 3 T-MRI to assess relationship between regional grey matter (rGM) volumes and RPP indexes Behaviourally, RIF and FAC were both observed controls, while RIF only was observed in a-MCI patients. In patients but not in controls, RIF was associated with cognitive efficiency and FAC with memory performance. Patients showed also associations between BR and rGM volumes in the precuneus, no association was found between rGM volumes and RIF and FAC. A-MCI patients did not benefit from repeated practice during retrieval of studied items, which is likely due to their memory disorder. In contrast, patient cognitive efficiency would drive retrieval suppression of interfering stimuli.

对19例a-MCI患者和29例对照组进行了神经心理学评价和检索-实践范式(RPP),以评估基线记忆(BR)、检索诱发促进(FAC)和检索诱发遗忘(RIF)。A-MCI患者还进行了3次T-MRI检查,以评估区域灰质(rGM)体积与RPP指数之间的关系。行为上,RIF和FAC均为观察对照组,而RIF仅在A-MCI患者中观察到。在患者中,RIF与认知效率有关,FAC与记忆表现有关。患者也显示出楔前叶BR和rGM体积之间的相关性,rGM体积与RIF和FAC之间没有相关性。A-MCI患者在检索研究项目时没有从重复练习中获益,这可能是由于他们的记忆障碍。相反,患者的认知效率会驱动干扰刺激的检索抑制。
{"title":"Are the inhibitory and faciliatory effects during retrieval of semantically related items present in amnestic mild cognitive impairment?","authors":"Laura Serra,&nbsp;Giulia Bechi Gabrielli,&nbsp;Carlotta Di Domenico,&nbsp;Chiara Del Bono,&nbsp;Camillo Marra,&nbsp;Leonardo Lopiano,&nbsp;Carlo Caltagirone,&nbsp;Marco Bozzali","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12286","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12286","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Prefrontal functions subserve inhibition control for retrieval of semantically related items inducing forgetting 19 a-MCI patients and 29 controls underwent neuropsychological evaluation and retrieval-practice paradigm (RPP) to estimate baseline remember (BR), retrieval-induced facilitation (FAC) and retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF). A-MCI patients underwent also 3 T-MRI to assess relationship between regional grey matter (rGM) volumes and RPP indexes Behaviourally, RIF and FAC were both observed controls, while RIF only was observed in a-MCI patients. In patients but not in controls, RIF was associated with cognitive efficiency and FAC with memory performance. Patients showed also associations between BR and rGM volumes in the precuneus, no association was found between rGM volumes and RIF and FAC. A-MCI patients did not benefit from repeated practice during retrieval of studied items, which is likely due to their memory disorder. In contrast, patient cognitive efficiency would drive retrieval suppression of interfering stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"63-80"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12286","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5696095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stroke damages attentional maintenance in working memory 中风损害工作记忆中的注意力维持
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-08-08 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12287
Ga?n Plancher, Bernadette Na?gelé, Victoria Guinet, Sophie Portrat, Pascale Colliot

Stroke is the main cause of acquired disability in adults, and specific deficits in working memory (WM) are among the most common cognitive consequences. In neuropsychological routine, WM is most of the time investigated in the framework of the multicomponent model (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974, The psychology of learning and motivation, 47). Using a more recent theoretical WM model, the time-based resource-sharing (TBRS) model (Barrouillet et al., 2011, Psychol. Rev., 118, 175), the aim of the present study was to investigate in young post-stroke patients to which extent attentional maintenance is impaired in WM. To address this question, we discarded other factors known to directly influence WM performance, that is processing speed and short-term memory span. We proposed to 53 post-stroke patients and to 63 healthy controls a complex span paradigm in which participants were asked to alternate between the memorization of a series of images and a concurrent parity judgement task of a series of digits. To investigate the attentional maintenance processes, we manipulated the cognitive load (CL) of the concurrent task. CL effect is typically interpreted as the involvement of attentional maintenance processes. The task was adapted to each participant according to their processing speed and memory span. As expected, the results showed higher recall performance in healthy controls compared with post-stroke patients. Consistent with the literature, we also observed higher performance when the CL was low compared with high. However, the improvement in recall at low CL was smaller for post-stroke patients compared with controls, suggesting that post-stroke WM deficit could be in part due to a deficit of the attentional maintenance processes.

中风是成人获得性残疾的主要原因,工作记忆(WM)的特异性缺陷是最常见的认知后果之一。在神经心理学常规中,WM大部分时间是在多成分模型的框架下进行研究的(Baddeley &Hitch, 1974, The psychology of learning and motivation, 47)。使用较新的理论WM模型,即基于时间的资源共享(TBRS)模型(Barrouillet et al., 2011;Rev., 118,175),本研究的目的是调查年轻脑卒中后患者的注意力维持在何种程度上受损。为了解决这个问题,我们抛弃了已知的直接影响WM性能的其他因素,即处理速度和短期记忆广度。我们对53名中风后患者和63名健康对照者提出了一个复杂的跨距范式,要求参与者在一系列图像的记忆和一系列数字的并行奇偶判断任务之间交替进行。为了研究注意维持过程,我们对并发任务的认知负荷进行了控制。CL效应通常被解释为注意力维持过程的参与。该任务根据每个参与者的处理速度和记忆广度进行调整。正如预期的那样,结果显示,与中风后患者相比,健康对照组的回忆表现更高。与文献一致,我们还观察到,当CL较低时,性能优于CL较高时。然而,与对照组相比,卒中后患者在低CL下回忆的改善较小,这表明卒中后WM缺陷可能部分是由于注意维持过程的缺陷。
{"title":"Stroke damages attentional maintenance in working memory","authors":"Ga?n Plancher,&nbsp;Bernadette Na?gelé,&nbsp;Victoria Guinet,&nbsp;Sophie Portrat,&nbsp;Pascale Colliot","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12287","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12287","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Stroke is the main cause of acquired disability in adults, and specific deficits in working memory (WM) are among the most common cognitive consequences. In neuropsychological routine, WM is most of the time investigated in the framework of the multicomponent model (Baddeley &amp; Hitch, 1974, <i>The psychology of learning and motivation</i>, 47). Using a more recent theoretical WM model, the time-based resource-sharing (TBRS) model (Barrouillet et al., 2011, <i>Psychol</i>. <i>Rev</i>., 118, 175), the aim of the present study was to investigate in young post-stroke patients to which extent attentional maintenance is impaired in WM. To address this question, we discarded other factors known to directly influence WM performance, <i>that is</i> processing speed and short-term memory span. We proposed to 53 post-stroke patients and to 63 healthy controls a complex span paradigm in which participants were asked to alternate between the memorization of a series of images and a concurrent parity judgement task of a series of digits. To investigate the attentional maintenance processes, we manipulated the cognitive load (CL) of the concurrent task. CL effect is typically interpreted as the involvement of attentional maintenance processes. The task was adapted to each participant according to their processing speed and memory span. As expected, the results showed higher recall performance in healthy controls compared with post-stroke patients. Consistent with the literature, we also observed higher performance when the CL was low compared with high. However, the improvement in recall at low CL was smaller for post-stroke patients compared with controls, suggesting that post-stroke WM deficit could be in part due to a deficit of the attentional maintenance processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"81-88"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"6135367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word finding, prosody and social cognition in multiple sclerosis 多发性硬化症的词汇发现、韵律与社会认知
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-07-12 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12285
Siew Mei Yap, Laura Davenport, Clodagh Cogley, Fiona Craddock, Alex Kennedy, Maria Gaughan, Hugh Kearney, Niall Tubridy, Céline De Looze, Fiadhnait O’Keeffe, Richard B. Reilly, Christopher McGuigan

Background

Impairments in speech and social cognition have been reported in people with multiple sclerosis (pwMS), although their relationships with neuropsychological outcomes and their clinical utility in MS are unclear.

Objectives

To evaluate word finding, prosody and social cognition in pwMS relative to healthy controls (HC).

Methods

We recruited people with relapsing MS (RMS, n = 21), progressive MS (PMS, n = 24) and HC (n = 25) from an outpatient MS clinic. Participants completed a battery of word-finding, social cognitive, neuropsychological and clinical assessments and performed a speech task for prosodic analysis.

Results

Of 45 pwMS, mean (SD) age was 49.4 (9.4) years, and median (range) Expanded Disability Severity Scale score was 3.5 (1.0–6.5). Compared with HC, pwMS were older and had slower information processing speed (measured with the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, SDMT) and higher depression scores. Most speech and social cognitive measures were associated with information processing speed but not with depression. Unlike speech, social cognition consistently correlated with intelligence and memory. Visual naming test mean response time (VNT-MRT) demonstrated worse outcomes in MS versus HC (p = .034, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 65.0%), and in PMS versus RMS (p = .009, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 50.2%). Rapid automatised object naming demonstrated worse outcomes in MS versus HC (p = .014, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 49.1%). These word-finding measures showed larger effect sizes than that of the SDMT (MS vs. HC, p = .010, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 40.6%; PMS vs. RMS, p = .023, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 43.5%). Prosody and social cognition did not differ between MS and HC.

Conclusions

Word finding, prosody and social cognition in MS are associated with information processing speed and largely independent of mood. Impairment in visual object meaning perception is potentially a unique MS disease-related deficit that could be further explored and cautiously considered as an adjunct disability metric for MS.

语言和社会认知障碍在多发性硬化症(pwMS)患者中有报道,尽管它们与神经心理学结果的关系及其在多发性硬化症中的临床应用尚不清楚。目的比较pwMS患者与健康对照组的词汇发现、韵律和社会认知能力。方法我们从MS门诊招募复发性MS (RMS, n = 21)、进行性MS (PMS, n = 24)和HC (n = 25)患者。参与者完成了一系列词汇发现、社会认知、神经心理学和临床评估,并完成了韵律分析的语音任务。结果45例pwMS患者的平均(SD)年龄为49.4(9.4)岁,扩展残疾严重程度量表评分中位数(范围)为3.5(1.0-6.5)。与HC相比,pwMS年龄更大,信息处理速度更慢(用符号数字模态测试,SDMT测量),抑郁评分更高。大多数言语和社会认知测试与信息处理速度有关,但与抑郁无关。与语言不同,社会认知始终与智力和记忆相关。视觉命名测试平均反应时间(VNT-MRT)显示MS比HC的结果更差(p =。034, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 65.0%), PMS与RMS比较(p =。009, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 50.2%)。快速自动化对象命名在MS和HC中表现出更差的结果(p =。014, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 49.1%)。这些词查找测量比SDMT显示更大的效应量(MS vs. HC, p =)。010, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 40.6%;PMS vs. RMS, p =。023, Nagelkerke’s R2 = 43.5%)。韵律和社会认知在MS和HC之间没有差异。结论多发性硬化症患者的词语发现、韵律和社会认知与信息加工速度有关,且与情绪无关。视觉客体的损伤意味着感知可能是一种独特的多发性硬化症相关缺陷,可以进一步探索并谨慎考虑作为多发性硬化症的辅助残疾指标。
{"title":"Word finding, prosody and social cognition in multiple sclerosis","authors":"Siew Mei Yap,&nbsp;Laura Davenport,&nbsp;Clodagh Cogley,&nbsp;Fiona Craddock,&nbsp;Alex Kennedy,&nbsp;Maria Gaughan,&nbsp;Hugh Kearney,&nbsp;Niall Tubridy,&nbsp;Céline De Looze,&nbsp;Fiadhnait O’Keeffe,&nbsp;Richard B. Reilly,&nbsp;Christopher McGuigan","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12285","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12285","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Impairments in speech and social cognition have been reported in people with multiple sclerosis (pwMS), although their relationships with neuropsychological outcomes and their clinical utility in MS are unclear.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objectives</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>To evaluate word finding, prosody and social cognition in pwMS relative to healthy controls (HC).</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Methods</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We recruited people with relapsing MS (RMS, <i>n</i> = 21), progressive MS (PMS, <i>n</i> = 24) and HC (<i>n</i> = 25) from an outpatient MS clinic. Participants completed a battery of word-finding, social cognitive, neuropsychological and clinical assessments and performed a speech task for prosodic analysis.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Of 45 pwMS, mean (SD) age was 49.4 (9.4) years, and median (range) Expanded Disability Severity Scale score was 3.5 (1.0–6.5). Compared with HC, pwMS were older and had slower information processing speed (measured with the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, SDMT) and higher depression scores. Most speech and social cognitive measures were associated with information processing speed but not with depression. Unlike speech, social cognition consistently correlated with intelligence and memory. Visual naming test mean response time (VNT-MRT) demonstrated worse outcomes in MS versus HC (<i>p</i> = .034, Nagelkerke’s <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = 65.0%), and in PMS versus RMS (<i>p</i> = .009, Nagelkerke’s <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = 50.2%). Rapid automatised object naming demonstrated worse outcomes in MS versus HC (<i>p</i> = .014, Nagelkerke’s <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = 49.1%). These word-finding measures showed larger effect sizes than that of the SDMT (MS vs. HC, <i>p</i> = .010, Nagelkerke’s <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = 40.6%; PMS vs. RMS, <i>p</i> = .023, Nagelkerke’s <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = 43.5%). Prosody and social cognition did not differ between MS and HC.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Word finding, prosody and social cognition in MS are associated with information processing speed and largely independent of mood. Impairment in visual object meaning perception is potentially a unique MS disease-related deficit that could be further explored and cautiously considered as an adjunct disability metric for MS.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"32-62"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"6207871","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury: A systematic review 后天性脑损伤后的感觉敏感性:一项系统综述
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-06-30 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12284
Hella Thielen, Nora Tuts, Lies Welkenhuyzen, Irene M. C. Huenges Wajer, Christophe Lafosse, Céline R. Gillebert

Patients with acquired brain injury frequently report experiencing sensory stimuli as abnormally under- (sensory hyposensitivity) or overwhelming (sensory hypersensitivity). Although they can negatively impact daily functioning, these symptoms are poorly understood. To provide an overview of the current evidence on atypical sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury, we conducted a systematic literature review. The primary aim of the review was to investigate the behavioural and neural mechanisms that are associated with self-reported sensory sensitivity. Studies were included when they studied sensory sensitivity in acquired brain injury populations, and excluded when they were not written in English, consisted of non-empirical research, did not study human subjects, studied pain, related sensory sensitivity to peripheral injury or studied patients with a neurodegenerative disorder, meningitis, encephalitis or a brain tumour. The Web of Science, PubMed and Scopus databases were searched for appropriate studies. A qualitative synthesis of the results of the 81 studies that were included suggests that abnormal sensory thresholds and a reduced information processing speed are candidate behavioural mechanisms of atypical subjective sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury. Furthermore, there was evidence for an association between subjective sensory sensitivity and structural grey or white matter abnormalities, and to functional abnormalities in sensory cortices. However, further research is needed to explore the causation of atypical sensory sensitivity. In addition, there is a need for the development of adequate diagnostic tools. This can significantly advance the quantity and quality of research on the prevalence, aetiology, prognosis and treatment of these symptoms.

获得性脑损伤患者经常报告感觉刺激异常不足(感觉低敏)或过度(感觉超敏)。尽管它们会对日常功能产生负面影响,但人们对这些症状知之甚少。为了提供关于获得性脑损伤后非典型感觉敏感性的现有证据的概述,我们进行了系统的文献综述。回顾的主要目的是调查与自我报告的感觉敏感性相关的行为和神经机制。当研究获得性脑损伤人群的感觉敏感性时,研究被纳入,当它们不是用英语写的,由非经验研究组成,没有研究人类受试者,研究疼痛,与周围损伤相关的感觉敏感性或研究患有神经退行性疾病,脑膜炎,脑炎或脑肿瘤的患者时,研究被排除。在Web of Science、PubMed和Scopus数据库中搜索合适的研究。对81项研究结果的定性综合表明,异常的感觉阈值和信息处理速度降低是获得性脑损伤后非典型主观感觉敏感性的候选行为机制。此外,有证据表明主观感觉敏感性与结构灰质或白质异常以及感觉皮质功能异常之间存在关联。然而,非典型感觉敏感性的病因尚需进一步研究。此外,还需要开发适当的诊断工具。这可以显著提高这些症状的患病率、病因、预后和治疗研究的数量和质量。
{"title":"Sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury: A systematic review","authors":"Hella Thielen,&nbsp;Nora Tuts,&nbsp;Lies Welkenhuyzen,&nbsp;Irene M. C. Huenges Wajer,&nbsp;Christophe Lafosse,&nbsp;Céline R. Gillebert","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12284","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12284","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Patients with acquired brain injury frequently report experiencing sensory stimuli as abnormally under- (sensory hyposensitivity) or overwhelming (sensory hypersensitivity). Although they can negatively impact daily functioning, these symptoms are poorly understood. To provide an overview of the current evidence on atypical sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury, we conducted a systematic literature review. The primary aim of the review was to investigate the behavioural and neural mechanisms that are associated with self-reported sensory sensitivity. Studies were included when they studied sensory sensitivity in acquired brain injury populations, and excluded when they were not written in English, consisted of non-empirical research, did not study human subjects, studied pain, related sensory sensitivity to peripheral injury or studied patients with a neurodegenerative disorder, meningitis, encephalitis or a brain tumour. The Web of Science, PubMed and Scopus databases were searched for appropriate studies. A qualitative synthesis of the results of the 81 studies that were included suggests that abnormal sensory thresholds and a reduced information processing speed are candidate behavioural mechanisms of atypical subjective sensory sensitivity after acquired brain injury. Furthermore, there was evidence for an association between subjective sensory sensitivity and structural grey or white matter abnormalities, and to functional abnormalities in sensory cortices. However, further research is needed to explore the causation of atypical sensory sensitivity. In addition, there is a need for the development of adequate diagnostic tools. This can significantly advance the quantity and quality of research on the prevalence, aetiology, prognosis and treatment of these symptoms.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"17 1","pages":"1-31"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5894367","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Investigating the association of mood and fatigue with objective and subjective cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis 调查多发性硬化症患者情绪和疲劳与客观和主观认知障碍的关系
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-06-28 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12283
Laura Davenport, Clodagh Cogley, Ruth Monaghan, Maria Gaughan, Mei Yap, Jessica Bramham, Niall Tubridy, Chris McGuigan, Fiadhnait O'Keeffe

Discrepancies between subjective cognitive difficulties and objective measures of cognitive function in people with MS have been identified and may be related to mood and fatigue. The aim of the present study was to examine associations of depression and fatigue with discrepancies between subjective and objective cognitive functioning in pwMS. 177 participants with MS attending a University Hospital Department of Neurology MS Outpatient clinic completed the Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BICAMS), MS Neuropsychological Questionnaire (MSNQ), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) and Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS). To quantify the discrepancy between objective (BICAMS) and subjective (MSNQ) cognitive functioning, discrepancy scores were calculated by subtracting MSNQ z-score from composite BICAMS z-score. Based on their discrepancy score, participants were grouped as ‘Underestimated’, ‘Overestimated’ and ‘Non-discrepant’. 39% of the total sample demonstrated poorer subjective cognitive functioning than their objective cognitive performance suggested (‘Underestimated’). 23% of the total sample indicated lower objective scores than their subjective report suggests (‘Overestimated’). 38% participants indicated relatively no discrepancy between objective and subjective cognitive measures (‘Non-discrepant’). Significant differences were observed between the discrepancy groups in terms of depression and fatigue, with the ‘Underestimated’ group demonstrating greater levels of depression and fatigue (ps < .01). Regression analysis indicated that cognitive fatigue and depression significantly contributed to variance in subjective cognitive functioning. Our findings suggest that subjective reports of cognitive function may be influenced by depression and fatigue, emphasising the importance of cognitive, mood and fatigue screening as part of routine clinical care.

MS患者的主观认知困难和客观认知功能测量之间的差异已经被确定,可能与情绪和疲劳有关。本研究的目的是检查抑郁症和疲劳与主客观认知功能差异的关联。177名MS患者在某大学医院神经内科MS门诊完成了国际MS简短认知评估(BICAMS)、MS神经心理问卷(MSNQ)、医院焦虑抑郁量表(HADS)和修正疲劳影响量表(MFIS)。为了量化客观认知功能(BICAMS)和主观认知功能(MSNQ)之间的差异,用BICAMS z-score减去MSNQ z-score计算差异评分。根据他们的差异得分,参与者被分为“低估”、“高估”和“非差异”。总样本中有39%的人表现出较差的主观认知功能,而不是他们的客观认知表现(“低估”)。总样本中有23%的人的客观得分低于他们的主观报告(“高估”)。38%的参与者表示客观和主观认知测量之间相对没有差异(“无差异”)。在抑郁和疲劳方面,差异组之间观察到显著差异,“低估”组表现出更高水平的抑郁和疲劳(ps < 0.01)。回归分析表明,认知疲劳和抑郁对主观认知功能变异有显著影响。我们的研究结果表明,认知功能的主观报告可能受到抑郁和疲劳的影响,强调了认知、情绪和疲劳筛查作为常规临床护理一部分的重要性。
{"title":"Investigating the association of mood and fatigue with objective and subjective cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis","authors":"Laura Davenport,&nbsp;Clodagh Cogley,&nbsp;Ruth Monaghan,&nbsp;Maria Gaughan,&nbsp;Mei Yap,&nbsp;Jessica Bramham,&nbsp;Niall Tubridy,&nbsp;Chris McGuigan,&nbsp;Fiadhnait O'Keeffe","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12283","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12283","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Discrepancies between subjective cognitive difficulties and objective measures of cognitive function in people with MS have been identified and may be related to mood and fatigue. The aim of the present study was to examine associations of depression and fatigue with discrepancies between subjective and objective cognitive functioning in pwMS. 177 participants with MS attending a University Hospital Department of Neurology MS Outpatient clinic completed the Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BICAMS), MS Neuropsychological Questionnaire (MSNQ), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) and Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS). To quantify the discrepancy between objective (BICAMS) and subjective (MSNQ) cognitive functioning, discrepancy scores were calculated by subtracting MSNQ <i>z</i>-score from composite BICAMS <i>z</i>-score. Based on their discrepancy score, participants were grouped as ‘Underestimated’, ‘Overestimated’ and ‘Non-discrepant’. 39% of the total sample demonstrated poorer subjective cognitive functioning than their objective cognitive performance suggested (‘Underestimated’). 23% of the total sample indicated lower objective scores than their subjective report suggests (‘Overestimated’). 38% participants indicated relatively no discrepancy between objective and subjective cognitive measures (‘Non-discrepant’). Significant differences were observed between the discrepancy groups in terms of depression and fatigue, with the ‘Underestimated’ group demonstrating greater levels of depression and fatigue (<i>p</i>s &lt; .01). Regression analysis indicated that cognitive fatigue and depression significantly contributed to variance in subjective cognitive functioning. Our findings suggest that subjective reports of cognitive function may be influenced by depression and fatigue, emphasising the importance of cognitive, mood and fatigue screening as part of routine clinical care.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"16 3","pages":"537-554"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-06-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"6085116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
What determines cognitive estimation ability? Changing contributions of semantic and executive domains as a function of age 是什么决定了认知估计能力?语义和执行域随年龄变化的贡献
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-05-21 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12279
Paul Hoffman, Sarah E. MacPherson

The Cognitive Estimation Test (CET) is commonly used in neuropsychological assessment. It is typically assumed to load on executive functions, although research has shown that CET performance also depends on access to semantic knowledge. It is unknown whether these contributions vary with age. It is important to examine this question as these abilities have divergent life course trajectories: executive functions tend to decline as people age but semantic knowledge continues to accrue. In addition, previous research has not examined potential contributions to CET performance from semantic control abilities, that is cognitive control processes involved specifically in the retrieval and use of semantic information. To address these questions, we investigated cognitive predictors of CET performance in healthy young and older adults. We found that better executive function was associated with more accurate estimation in both age groups. However, the effect of semantic knowledge on CET performance was significantly larger in older people, having no predictive power in the younger group. The ability to detect weak semantic associations, which is thought to index controlled search and retrieval of semantic information, also had divergent effects on CET performance in the two age groups. Our results provide empirical support for the idea that older people are more reliant on semantic knowledge when estimating quantities, which may explain why age-related decline in CET scores is not typically found. We conclude that deficits on the CET may be indicative either of semantic or executive impairments, particularly in older age groups.

认知估计测验(CET)是一种常用的神经心理评估方法。它通常被认为是对执行功能的负荷,尽管研究表明,CET的表现也取决于对语义知识的获取。目前尚不清楚这些贡献是否随年龄而变化。研究这个问题很重要,因为这些能力有不同的生命轨迹:随着人们年龄的增长,执行功能往往会下降,但语义知识会继续积累。此外,以前的研究并没有考察语义控制能力对CET成绩的潜在贡献,语义控制能力是指具体涉及语义信息检索和使用的认知控制过程。为了解决这些问题,我们调查了健康年轻人和老年人CET表现的认知预测因素。我们发现,在两个年龄组中,更好的执行功能与更准确的估计有关。然而,语义知识对CET成绩的影响在老年人中明显更大,在年轻人中没有预测能力。检测弱语义关联的能力(被认为是索引语义信息的受控搜索和检索)对两个年龄组的CET表现也有不同的影响。我们的研究结果为老年人在估计数量时更依赖语义知识的观点提供了实证支持,这可能解释了为什么没有发现与年龄相关的CET分数下降。我们的结论是,CET的缺陷可能表明语义或执行障碍,特别是在老年群体中。
{"title":"What determines cognitive estimation ability? Changing contributions of semantic and executive domains as a function of age","authors":"Paul Hoffman,&nbsp;Sarah E. MacPherson","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12279","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12279","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Cognitive Estimation Test (CET) is commonly used in neuropsychological assessment. It is typically assumed to load on executive functions, although research has shown that CET performance also depends on access to semantic knowledge. It is unknown whether these contributions vary with age. It is important to examine this question as these abilities have divergent life course trajectories: executive functions tend to decline as people age but semantic knowledge continues to accrue. In addition, previous research has not examined potential contributions to CET performance from semantic control abilities, that is cognitive control processes involved specifically in the retrieval and use of semantic information. To address these questions, we investigated cognitive predictors of CET performance in healthy young and older adults. We found that better executive function was associated with more accurate estimation in both age groups. However, the effect of semantic knowledge on CET performance was significantly larger in older people, having no predictive power in the younger group. The ability to detect weak semantic associations, which is thought to index controlled search and retrieval of semantic information, also had divergent effects on CET performance in the two age groups. Our results provide empirical support for the idea that older people are more reliant on semantic knowledge when estimating quantities, which may explain why age-related decline in CET scores is not typically found. We conclude that deficits on the CET may be indicative either of semantic or executive impairments, particularly in older age groups.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"16 3","pages":"481-497"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12279","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5973649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Endpoints and viewpoints on spatial neglect 空间忽视的终点和观点
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2022-05-04 DOI: 10.1111/jnp.12278
Robert D. McIntosh, Sumio Ishiai

In this issue of the Journal of Neuropsychology, Abe and Ishiai (2022) report an experiment designed to probe the subjective experience of line bisection in neglect. A re-analysis of their data can also offer insights into how best to characterise neglect performance for this and other tasks. We show that sensitive measures of neglect can be obtained by quantifying the difference in the influence (or ‘weighting’) that each endpoint has on the response. The right endpoint is dramatically more influential than the left in people with neglect performing line bisection and endpoint reproduction tasks. This supports the view that neglect may limit the ability to simultaneously represent two locations, so that the response is determined primarily with respect to the right endpoint. We also discuss Abe and Ishiai's conclusion that bisection responses in neglect are accompanied by the subjective experience of a complete line extending equally to either side of the chosen midpoint.

在这一期的《神经心理学杂志》上,Abe和Ishiai(2022)报告了一项实验,旨在探索忽视中平分线的主观体验。对他们的数据进行重新分析,也可以为如何最好地描述这项任务和其他任务的忽视表现提供见解。我们表明,可以通过量化每个端点对响应的影响(或“权重”)的差异来获得忽略的敏感度量。在执行线平分和端点再现任务的忽视患者中,右端点比左端点明显更有影响力。这支持这样一种观点,即忽略可能会限制同时表示两个位置的能力,因此响应主要是根据正确的端点确定的。我们还讨论了Abe和Ishiai的结论,即忽略的平分反应伴随着一条完整的线的主观体验,该线均匀地延伸到所选中点的两侧。
{"title":"Endpoints and viewpoints on spatial neglect","authors":"Robert D. McIntosh,&nbsp;Sumio Ishiai","doi":"10.1111/jnp.12278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12278","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this issue of the <i>Journal of Neuropsychology</i>, Abe and Ishiai (2022) report an experiment designed to probe the subjective experience of line bisection in neglect. A re-analysis of their data can also offer insights into how best to characterise neglect performance for this and other tasks. We show that sensitive measures of neglect can be obtained by quantifying the difference in the influence (or ‘weighting’) that each endpoint has on the response. The right endpoint is dramatically more influential than the left in people with neglect performing line bisection and endpoint reproduction tasks. This supports the view that neglect may limit the ability to simultaneously represent two locations, so that the response is determined primarily with respect to the right endpoint. We also discuss Abe and Ishiai's conclusion that bisection responses in neglect are accompanied by the subjective experience of a complete line extending equally to either side of the chosen midpoint.</p>","PeriodicalId":197,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Neuropsychology","volume":"16 2","pages":"299-305"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jnp.12278","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5971557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1