首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Numerical Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
A capturing math language use during block play: Creation of the spatial and quantitative mathematical language coding system 在积木游戏中捕捉数学语言的使用:创建空间和数量数学语言编码系统
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-07-16 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.11589
Lindsey M. Bryant, Lauren Westerberg, Brianna L. Devlin, Tanya M. Paes, Elyssa A Geer, Anisha Katyayan, Kathleen M. Morse, Grace O’Brien, David J. Purpura, S. Schmitt
The goals of the current study were: 1) to modify and expand an existing spatial mathematical language coding system to include quantitative mathematical language terms and 2) to examine the extent to which preschool-aged children used spatial and quantitative mathematical language during a block play intervention. Participants included 24 preschool-aged children (Age M = 57.35 months) who were assigned to a block play intervention. Children participated in up to 14 sessions of 15-to-20-minute block play across seven weeks. Results demonstrated that spatial mathematical language terms were used with a higher raw frequency than quantitative mathematical language terms during the intervention sessions. However, once weighted frequencies were calculated to account for the number of codes in each category, spatial language was only used slightly more than quantitative language during block play. Similar patterns emerged between domains within the spatial and quantitative language categories. These findings suggest that both quantitative and spatial mathematical language usage should be evaluated when considering whether child activities can improve mathematical learning and spatial performance. Further, accounting for the number of codes within categories provided a more representative presentation of how mathematical language was used versus solely utilizing raw word counts. Implications for future research are discussed.
本研究的目标是1)修改和扩展现有的空间数学语言编码系统,以纳入定量数学语言术语;2)研究学龄前儿童在积木游戏干预中使用空间和定量数学语言的程度。参与者包括 24 名学龄前儿童(平均年龄为 57.35 个月),他们被分配到积木游戏干预中。儿童在七周内参加了多达 14 次 15-20 分钟的积木游戏。结果表明,在干预过程中,空间数学语言词汇的原始使用频率高于数量数学语言词汇。然而,一旦计算出每个类别中代码的加权频率,在积木游戏中,空间数学语言的使用频率仅略高于数量数学语言。在空间语言和定量语言类别中,不同领域之间也出现了类似的模式。这些发现表明,在考虑儿童活动是否能提高数学学习和空间表现时,应同时评估定量和空间数学语言的使用情况。此外,与仅使用原始字数相比,考虑类别内的代码数量更能反映数学语言的使用情况。本文还讨论了未来研究的意义。
{"title":"A capturing math language use during block play: Creation of the spatial and quantitative mathematical language coding system","authors":"Lindsey M. Bryant, Lauren Westerberg, Brianna L. Devlin, Tanya M. Paes, Elyssa A Geer, Anisha Katyayan, Kathleen M. Morse, Grace O’Brien, David J. Purpura, S. Schmitt","doi":"10.5964/jnc.11589","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.11589","url":null,"abstract":"The goals of the current study were: 1) to modify and expand an existing spatial mathematical language coding system to include quantitative mathematical language terms and 2) to examine the extent to which preschool-aged children used spatial and quantitative mathematical language during a block play intervention. Participants included 24 preschool-aged children (Age M = 57.35 months) who were assigned to a block play intervention. Children participated in up to 14 sessions of 15-to-20-minute block play across seven weeks. Results demonstrated that spatial mathematical language terms were used with a higher raw frequency than quantitative mathematical language terms during the intervention sessions. However, once weighted frequencies were calculated to account for the number of codes in each category, spatial language was only used slightly more than quantitative language during block play. Similar patterns emerged between domains within the spatial and quantitative language categories. These findings suggest that both quantitative and spatial mathematical language usage should be evaluated when considering whether child activities can improve mathematical learning and spatial performance. Further, accounting for the number of codes within categories provided a more representative presentation of how mathematical language was used versus solely utilizing raw word counts. Implications for future research are discussed.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141642676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ninth-grade students’ conceptual understanding of the number line 九年级学生对数线概念的理解
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.12501
Zehra E Ünal, A. M. Ala, Gamze Kartal, Serkan Özel, David C. Geary
Sixty (35 girls and 25 boys) 9th-grade students’ conceptual understanding of the number line was qualitatively assessed through verbal explanations and visual representations. The assessment included an open-ended question focused on students’ number line descriptions and the explanations coalesced around six features: sequential ordering (i.e., numbers are sequentially represented), positivity-negativity of numbers (i.e., the number line contains positive and negative numbers), non-centrality (i.e., zero does not have to be in the center), infinity, increment flexibility (i.e., number line increments can vary), and continuity (i.e., numbers can be placed anywhere between minus infinity and plus infinity without breaks). The students’ explanations show that these six features emerge in five successive stages in the conceptual understanding of the number line. These stages are (1) no knowledge, (2) sequential ordering and positivity-negativity, (3) infinity and non-centrality, (4) incremental flexibility, and (5) continuity. The last two stages were not found in most descriptions. The results suggest that students’ understanding of the number line is incomplete and may be overestimated by commonly used quantitative assessments of number line knowledge.
通过口头解释和视觉呈现,对 60 名(35 名女生和 25 名男生)九年级学生对数列概念的理解进行了定性评估。评估包括一个开放式问题,重点是学生对数线的描述,解释围绕六个特征展开:顺序排序(即数字是按顺序表示的)、数字的正负性(即数线包含正数和负数)、非中心性(即数线包含正数和负数)、数字的正负性(即数线包含正数和负数)、非中心性(即数线包含正数和负数)、数线包含正数和负数)、非中心性(即 0 不一定在中心)、无穷大、增量灵活性(即数线的增量可 以变化)和连续性(即数字可以放在负无穷大和正无穷大之间的任何地方而不会中断)。从学生的解释中可以看出,在对数线的概念理解过程中,这六个特征依次出现在五个 阶段。这五个阶段是:(1) 一无所知;(2) 顺序排序和正负性;(3) 无穷大和非中心性;(4) 递增灵活性;(5) 连续性。后两个阶段在大多数描述中都没有出现。结果表明,学生对数线的理解是不完整的,常用的数线知识定量评估可能会高估学生的理解。
{"title":"Ninth-grade students’ conceptual understanding of the number line","authors":"Zehra E Ünal, A. M. Ala, Gamze Kartal, Serkan Özel, David C. Geary","doi":"10.5964/jnc.12501","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.12501","url":null,"abstract":"Sixty (35 girls and 25 boys) 9th-grade students’ conceptual understanding of the number line was qualitatively assessed through verbal explanations and visual representations. The assessment included an open-ended question focused on students’ number line descriptions and the explanations coalesced around six features: sequential ordering (i.e., numbers are sequentially represented), positivity-negativity of numbers (i.e., the number line contains positive and negative numbers), non-centrality (i.e., zero does not have to be in the center), infinity, increment flexibility (i.e., number line increments can vary), and continuity (i.e., numbers can be placed anywhere between minus infinity and plus infinity without breaks). The students’ explanations show that these six features emerge in five successive stages in the conceptual understanding of the number line. These stages are (1) no knowledge, (2) sequential ordering and positivity-negativity, (3) infinity and non-centrality, (4) incremental flexibility, and (5) continuity. The last two stages were not found in most descriptions. The results suggest that students’ understanding of the number line is incomplete and may be overestimated by commonly used quantitative assessments of number line knowledge.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141673370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the role of working memory and phonological memory in mathematics and response to intervention for emergent bilingual kindergartners 了解工作记忆和语音记忆在数学中的作用以及对初学双语的幼儿园儿童的干预反应
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.11635
Madison A. Cook, K. Smolkowski, Lina Shanley, Joanna Hermida, Sylvia Linan-Thompson, Christian T. Doabler, Ben Clarke
This study explores how kindergarten students from a multilingual sample (n = 131) representing 23 different languages differ in response to intervention, based on their skill in mathematics and domain general cognitive skills. Analyses for this study indicate significant correlations between initial math skill, phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. There was no statistically significant relationship demonstrated between gains in mathematics and phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. No moderation effect was found between domain general skills and response to math intervention. Implications of this work will inform development and delivery of math interventions for multilingual students in kindergarten.
本研究探讨了代表 23 种不同语言的多语言样本(n = 131)中的幼儿园学生,在数学技能和领域一般认知技能的基础上,对干预措施的反应有何不同。本研究的分析表明,初始数学技能、语音记忆、工作记忆和语言能力之间存在显著相关性。数学成绩的提高与语音记忆、工作记忆和语言能力之间没有明显的统计学关系。在领域一般技能和对数学干预的反应之间没有发现调节效应。这项研究的意义在于为幼儿园中的多语言学生制定和实施数学干预措施。
{"title":"Understanding the role of working memory and phonological memory in mathematics and response to intervention for emergent bilingual kindergartners","authors":"Madison A. Cook, K. Smolkowski, Lina Shanley, Joanna Hermida, Sylvia Linan-Thompson, Christian T. Doabler, Ben Clarke","doi":"10.5964/jnc.11635","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.11635","url":null,"abstract":"This study explores how kindergarten students from a multilingual sample (n = 131) representing 23 different languages differ in response to intervention, based on their skill in mathematics and domain general cognitive skills. Analyses for this study indicate significant correlations between initial math skill, phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. There was no statistically significant relationship demonstrated between gains in mathematics and phonological memory, working memory, and language proficiency. No moderation effect was found between domain general skills and response to math intervention. Implications of this work will inform development and delivery of math interventions for multilingual students in kindergarten.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140692287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessment of computation competence and non-count strategy use in addition and subtraction in Grade 1 评估一年级加减法的计算能力和非计算策略的使用情况
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.12633
Delia Leuenberger, Elisabeth Moser Opitz, Noemi Gloor
Computation competence (CC) in simple addition and subtraction using non-counting (NC) strategies is an important learning objective in Grade 1 mathematics but many children, especially low achievers in mathematics, struggle to acquire these skills. To provide these students with the support they need, it is important to have valid and reliable tools for assessing progress in CC and NC strategy use. Developing an assessment instrument for use in Grade 1, when some children start the year unable to solve any problems, is challenging, as is ensuring measurement invariance over a school year when children generally make large achievement gains. This paper presents a new assessment tool for CC and NC strategy use in Grade 1 that was tested in a longitudinal study with N = 1,017 children. Analyses using the Rasch model revealed acceptable mean square scores (MNSQ 0.83 – 1.20). Warm’s Weighted Likelihood Estimate (WLE) reliability scores were acceptable (pre-test .77; post-test .87). Measurement invariance over time was given. The instrument is promising for assessing CC and NC strategy use efficiently and accurately in Grade 1.
使用非计算(NC)策略进行简单加减法的计算能力(CC)是一年级数学的一个重要学习目标,但许多儿童,尤其是数学成绩较差的儿童,在掌握这些技能方面却步履维艰。要为这些学生提供他们所需的支持,就必须有有效可靠的工具来评估他们在使用 CC 和 NC 策略方面所取得的进展。有些孩子在一年级开始时无法解决任何问题,因此,开发一种在一年级使用的评估工具具有挑战性,同时,在孩子们成绩普遍大幅提高的一学年中,确保测量的不变性也具有挑战性。本文介绍了一种针对一年级学生使用 CC 和 NC 策略的新评估工具,该工具在一项纵向研究中进行了测试,测试对象为 1,017 名儿童。使用 Rasch 模型进行的分析表明,平均平方得分(MNSQ 0.83 - 1.20)是可以接受的。Warm加权似然估计(WLE)信度得分也可以接受(测试前为0.77;测试后为0.87)。该工具具有随时间变化的测量不变性。该工具有望在一年级有效、准确地评估 CC 和 NC 策略的使用情况。
{"title":"Assessment of computation competence and non-count strategy use in addition and subtraction in Grade 1","authors":"Delia Leuenberger, Elisabeth Moser Opitz, Noemi Gloor","doi":"10.5964/jnc.12633","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.12633","url":null,"abstract":"Computation competence (CC) in simple addition and subtraction using non-counting (NC) strategies is an important learning objective in Grade 1 mathematics but many children, especially low achievers in mathematics, struggle to acquire these skills. To provide these students with the support they need, it is important to have valid and reliable tools for assessing progress in CC and NC strategy use. Developing an assessment instrument for use in Grade 1, when some children start the year unable to solve any problems, is challenging, as is ensuring measurement invariance over a school year when children generally make large achievement gains. This paper presents a new assessment tool for CC and NC strategy use in Grade 1 that was tested in a longitudinal study with N = 1,017 children. Analyses using the Rasch model revealed acceptable mean square scores (MNSQ 0.83 – 1.20). Warm’s Weighted Likelihood Estimate (WLE) reliability scores were acceptable (pre-test .77; post-test .87). Measurement invariance over time was given. The instrument is promising for assessing CC and NC strategy use efficiently and accurately in Grade 1.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140690713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A brief, multiple-choice assessment of mature number sense is strongly correlated with more resource-intensive measures 对成熟的数感进行简短的多项选择评估与资源密集型的评估结果密切相关
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.12679
Patrick K. Kirkland, Claire Guang, Chineme Otuonye, Nicole M. McNeil
Students who exhibit mature number sense make sense of numbers and operations, use reasoning to notice patterns, and flexibly choose effective problem-solving strategies (McIntosh et al., 1997, https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworks/6819). Due to its dispositional nature, mature number sense is typically measured through in-depth interviews or tests of strategy usage. Yet, the lack of an efficient, rigorously developed measure has made it difficult to collect systematic, replicable evidence on students’ mature number sense. To address this, we developed a brief assessment of mature number sense. The present study provides additional convergent evidence of validity for this measure with US students in grades 3-8 (8–14 years old). We compared middle school (N = 40) and upper elementary school (N = 41) scores from the brief assessment with an established, time-intensive measure (Yang, 2019, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9874-8) and an in-depth interview of student strategy usage (Markovits & Sowder, 1994, https://doi.org/10.2307/749290). We found strong correlations (r > 0.7) across all three measures, and this held even when controlling for students’ arithmetic scores (pr > 0.6). Researchers and educators can now use the brief assessment to investigate students’ mathematical thinking and advance knowledge of a key aspect of mathematical cognition.
表现出成熟数感的学生能够理解数字和运算,运用推理发现规律,并灵活选择有效的解题策略(McIntosh et al., 1997, https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworks/6819)。由于成熟的数感具有倾向性,通常通过深入访谈或策略使用测试来衡量。然而,由于缺乏有效、严格的测量方法,因此很难收集到有关学生成熟数感的系统性、可重复的证据。为了解决这个问题,我们开发了一种简短的成熟数感评估方法。本研究针对美国 3-8 年级(8-14 岁)的学生,为这一测评方法的有效性提供了更多收敛性证据。我们将初中(40 人)和小学高年级(41 人)的简短评估得分与一项成熟的、时间密集型测量(Yang, 2019, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9874-8)和学生策略使用情况深度访谈(Markovits & Sowder, 1994, https://doi.org/10.2307/749290)进行了比较。我们发现,所有三种测量方法之间都存在很强的相关性(r > 0.7),即使在控制学生算术分数(pr > 0.6)的情况下也是如此。研究人员和教育工作者现在可以利用简短的评估来研究学生的数学思维,并增进对数学认知的一个关键方面的了解。
{"title":"A brief, multiple-choice assessment of mature number sense is strongly correlated with more resource-intensive measures","authors":"Patrick K. Kirkland, Claire Guang, Chineme Otuonye, Nicole M. McNeil","doi":"10.5964/jnc.12679","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.12679","url":null,"abstract":"Students who exhibit mature number sense make sense of numbers and operations, use reasoning to notice patterns, and flexibly choose effective problem-solving strategies (McIntosh et al., 1997, https://ro.ecu.edu.au/ecuworks/6819). Due to its dispositional nature, mature number sense is typically measured through in-depth interviews or tests of strategy usage. Yet, the lack of an efficient, rigorously developed measure has made it difficult to collect systematic, replicable evidence on students’ mature number sense. To address this, we developed a brief assessment of mature number sense. The present study provides additional convergent evidence of validity for this measure with US students in grades 3-8 (8–14 years old). We compared middle school (N = 40) and upper elementary school (N = 41) scores from the brief assessment with an established, time-intensive measure (Yang, 2019, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-018-9874-8) and an in-depth interview of student strategy usage (Markovits & Sowder, 1994, https://doi.org/10.2307/749290). We found strong correlations (r > 0.7) across all three measures, and this held even when controlling for students’ arithmetic scores (pr > 0.6). Researchers and educators can now use the brief assessment to investigate students’ mathematical thinking and advance knowledge of a key aspect of mathematical cognition.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140240650","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The ecological validity of picture SFON tasks 图片 SFON 任务的生态有效性
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.11055
Sophie Batchelor, C. Gilmore, Jayne Spiller, Matthew Inglis
Research has identified that children differ in the extent to which they spontaneously focus on numerical aspects of the environment (Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, SFON) and that this correlates with their mathematics achievement. It is assumed that the mechanism underpinning this relationship is that children who spontaneously focus on numerical features of their environment will experience more self-initiated practice with number concepts. We explored this mechanism by investigating whether 4- to 5-year-old children’s verbal SFON scores on a picture description task related to their spontaneous focusing on number while engaged in play activities with their parent. We found that the scores derived from a picture description task were strongly correlated with the scores derived from the play sessions, rₛ = .638, 95% CI [.433, .781], providing evidence for this mechanism. We further investigated the role that verbal abilities may play in children’s performance on the picture description task, finding that general verbal abilities were not associated with verbal SFON scores. These results contribute to our understanding of the role played by verbal SFON tendencies in explaining differences in numerical development, and demonstrate the ecological validity of SFON picture tasks.
研究发现,儿童自发关注环境中数字特征的程度不同(自发关注数字特征,SFON),这与他们的数学成绩相关。据推测,这种关系的基本机制是,自发关注环境中数字特征的儿童会经历更多的自我数字概念练习。我们通过研究 4 至 5 岁儿童在图片描述任务中的口头 SFON 分数是否与他们在与父母进行游戏活动时自发关注数字有关,来探索这一机制。我们发现,图片描述任务的得分与游戏环节的得分密切相关(rₛ = .638,95% CI [.433, .781]),为这一机制提供了证据。我们进一步研究了言语能力在儿童图片描述任务中可能扮演的角色,发现一般言语能力与言语 SFON 分数无关。这些结果有助于我们理解言语SFON倾向在解释数字发展差异中的作用,并证明了SFON图片任务的生态学有效性。
{"title":"The ecological validity of picture SFON tasks","authors":"Sophie Batchelor, C. Gilmore, Jayne Spiller, Matthew Inglis","doi":"10.5964/jnc.11055","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.11055","url":null,"abstract":"Research has identified that children differ in the extent to which they spontaneously focus on numerical aspects of the environment (Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, SFON) and that this correlates with their mathematics achievement. It is assumed that the mechanism underpinning this relationship is that children who spontaneously focus on numerical features of their environment will experience more self-initiated practice with number concepts. We explored this mechanism by investigating whether 4- to 5-year-old children’s verbal SFON scores on a picture description task related to their spontaneous focusing on number while engaged in play activities with their parent. We found that the scores derived from a picture description task were strongly correlated with the scores derived from the play sessions, rₛ = .638, 95% CI [.433, .781], providing evidence for this mechanism. We further investigated the role that verbal abilities may play in children’s performance on the picture description task, finding that general verbal abilities were not associated with verbal SFON scores. These results contribute to our understanding of the role played by verbal SFON tendencies in explaining differences in numerical development, and demonstrate the ecological validity of SFON picture tasks.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140240380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Embodied magnitude processing: On the relation between the SNARC effect and perceived reachability 嵌入式幅度处理:SNARC效应与感知可达性之间的关系
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.10885
Nadine Koch, J. Lohmann, Martin V. Butz, H. Nuerk
Magnitude information, for instance, regarding weight, distance, or velocity, is crucial for planning goal-directed interactions. Accordingly, magnitude information, including numerical magnitude, can affect actions: Responses to small numbers are faster with the left hand than the right and vice versa (hand-based SNARC effect). Previous experiments found an influence of effector placements on the SNARC effect but also an influence of the mere distance between effectors and numbers. This indicates a sensorimotor grounding of space-number processing. In the current study, we investigated this grounding by probing the SNARC effect close to and far from the hands. We used a magnitude comparison task with a fixed standard of 5 (smaller numbers 1, 2, 3, 4; larger numbers 6, 7, 8, 9) and a sagittal response arrangement to measure hand-based and sagittal SNARC effects for digits presented at different sagittal distances to the hands, i.e., in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. A significant sagittal SNARC effect was found, with the largest effect size in extrapersonal space. Meanwhile, the hand-based SNARC effect appeared only descriptively, with the largest effect size between the hands, i.e., in peripersonal space. Additionally, a purely spatial congruency effect surfaced, prioritizing responses with the hand closer to the number. Together, these results emphasize that responses in simple decision-making tasks can be influenced interactively by a multitude of task-relevant axes and relative spatial locations, including effector placement and stimulus placement, as well as number magnitude.
例如,有关重量、距离或速度的大小信息对于规划目标导向的互动至关重要。因此,包括数字大小在内的大小信息会影响行动:左手对小数字的反应比右手快,反之亦然(基于手的 SNARC 效应)。之前的实验发现,效应器的位置对 SNARC 效应有影响,但效应器与数字之间的距离也有影响。这表明空间-数字处理是以感觉运动为基础的。在当前的研究中,我们通过探究近手和远手的SNARC效应来研究这种基础。我们使用了一个固定标准为 5(较小的数字为 1、2、3、4;较大的数字为 6、7、8、9)和矢状反应排列的大小比较任务,以测量在与手的不同矢状距离(即人周空间和人外空间)上呈现的数字的基于手的和矢状的 SNARC 效应。结果发现,矢状面 SNARC 效应非常明显,人外空间的效应大小最大。同时,以手为基础的 SNARC 效应仅出现在描述性方面,其最大效应大小出现在两手之间,即人周空间。此外,一种纯粹的空间一致性效应也浮出水面,即更靠近数字的那只手的反应优先。总之,这些结果强调,在简单的决策任务中,反应会受到许多任务相关轴和相对空间位置的交互影响,包括效应器位置、刺激物位置以及数字大小。
{"title":"Embodied magnitude processing: On the relation between the SNARC effect and perceived reachability","authors":"Nadine Koch, J. Lohmann, Martin V. Butz, H. Nuerk","doi":"10.5964/jnc.10885","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.10885","url":null,"abstract":"Magnitude information, for instance, regarding weight, distance, or velocity, is crucial for planning goal-directed interactions. Accordingly, magnitude information, including numerical magnitude, can affect actions: Responses to small numbers are faster with the left hand than the right and vice versa (hand-based SNARC effect). Previous experiments found an influence of effector placements on the SNARC effect but also an influence of the mere distance between effectors and numbers. This indicates a sensorimotor grounding of space-number processing. In the current study, we investigated this grounding by probing the SNARC effect close to and far from the hands. We used a magnitude comparison task with a fixed standard of 5 (smaller numbers 1, 2, 3, 4; larger numbers 6, 7, 8, 9) and a sagittal response arrangement to measure hand-based and sagittal SNARC effects for digits presented at different sagittal distances to the hands, i.e., in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. A significant sagittal SNARC effect was found, with the largest effect size in extrapersonal space. Meanwhile, the hand-based SNARC effect appeared only descriptively, with the largest effect size between the hands, i.e., in peripersonal space. Additionally, a purely spatial congruency effect surfaced, prioritizing responses with the hand closer to the number. Together, these results emphasize that responses in simple decision-making tasks can be influenced interactively by a multitude of task-relevant axes and relative spatial locations, including effector placement and stimulus placement, as well as number magnitude.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140239747","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Money talks! The role of parents’ discussion of money for preschoolers’ math knowledge 有钱能使鬼推磨父母讨论金钱对学龄前儿童数学知识的影响
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.11351
Shirley Duong, Leanne E. Elliott, Olivia Sidoti, Heather J. Bachman, Melissa E. Libertus, E. Votruba-Drzal
Children’s participation in cultural, everyday practices and social interactions involving math and money can contribute to the development of their knowledge and skills in these domains. Further work is needed to uncover what features of these activities, such as aspects of the conversations that may occur (e.g., number and money talk), facilitate and/or are shaped by children’s understanding of money concepts and skills. The present study examined the extent to which parents engaged in conversations about numbers and money with their four-year-old children during pretend grocery play and the relations to children’s math skills. We found that talk about price labeling and exchanging currency or goods occurred most frequently and that money and number talk were not significantly related to children’s broader math skills. However, parents’ money talk was positively associated with children’s money-related math skills, and this association was driven by the co-occurrence of talk about money and numbers. Our results suggest that parent-child conversations in familiar contexts such as grocery shopping provide rich opportunities to discuss culturally relevant practices surrounding money and practice math skills in the context of monetary exchanges. Thus, it is critical to consider how existing family practices and everyday contexts support children’s early math learning.
儿童参与涉及数学和金钱的文化、日常实践和社会交往,可促进其在这些领域的知识 和技能的发展。我们还需要进一步研究这些活动的特点,如可能出现的对话(如数字和金钱对话),这些特点会促进和/或影响儿童对金钱概念和技能的理解。本研究探讨了在假装买东西的游戏中,父母与四岁儿童进行数字和金钱对话的程度,以及这些对话与儿童数学技能的关系。我们发现,关于价格标签和货币或货物交换的对话出现得最频繁,而金钱和数字对话与儿童更广泛的数学技能并无显著关系。然而,父母的金钱谈话与儿童与金钱有关的数学技能呈正相关,而这种相关性是由共同出现的金钱和数字谈话所驱动的。我们的研究结果表明,在诸如买菜等熟悉的情境中,亲子对话为讨论与文化相关的金钱习俗和在金钱交换中练习数学技能提供了丰富的机会。因此,考虑现有的家庭实践和日常环境如何支持儿童的早期数学学习至关重要。
{"title":"Money talks! The role of parents’ discussion of money for preschoolers’ math knowledge","authors":"Shirley Duong, Leanne E. Elliott, Olivia Sidoti, Heather J. Bachman, Melissa E. Libertus, E. Votruba-Drzal","doi":"10.5964/jnc.11351","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.11351","url":null,"abstract":"Children’s participation in cultural, everyday practices and social interactions involving math and money can contribute to the development of their knowledge and skills in these domains. Further work is needed to uncover what features of these activities, such as aspects of the conversations that may occur (e.g., number and money talk), facilitate and/or are shaped by children’s understanding of money concepts and skills. The present study examined the extent to which parents engaged in conversations about numbers and money with their four-year-old children during pretend grocery play and the relations to children’s math skills. We found that talk about price labeling and exchanging currency or goods occurred most frequently and that money and number talk were not significantly related to children’s broader math skills. However, parents’ money talk was positively associated with children’s money-related math skills, and this association was driven by the co-occurrence of talk about money and numbers. Our results suggest that parent-child conversations in familiar contexts such as grocery shopping provide rich opportunities to discuss culturally relevant practices surrounding money and practice math skills in the context of monetary exchanges. Thus, it is critical to consider how existing family practices and everyday contexts support children’s early math learning.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140240047","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“We don’t have things for counting”: An exploration of early numeracy skills and home learning experiences of children growing up in poverty in South Africa “我们没有计算的东西”:探索南非贫困儿童的早期计算技能和家庭学习经验
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2023-07-31 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.8061
R. Merkley, Elizabeth Sernoskie, C. Cook, S. Howard, Hleliwe Makaula, Mbulelo Mshudulu, Nosibusiso Tshetu, C. Draper, G. Scerif
A child’s home environment has been shown to be related to the development of early numeracy skills in some countries. However, significant relationships between home learning environment and math achievement have not consistently been found, and likely vary across different cultural and socio-political contexts. Here we explored the home environment and early numeracy skills of 243 children (3-5 years), who were not attending preschool programmes in very low-income settings in Cape Town, South Africa. Caregivers completed a questionnaire including information regarding experiences of children in the home; children completed a number identification task, a counting task and the Give-N task. The amount of resources in the home learning environment (e.g. the number of books and toys), frequency of home learning activities caregivers did with their children, and caregiver levels of education and income were not associated with number knowledge. While the home learning environment has been shown to be important for developing early numeracy skills in previous research, this study suggests that factors other than the home learning environment may also be important targets to foster numeracy skills and school readiness in low-income settings in South Africa.
在一些国家,儿童的家庭环境已被证明与早期计算技能的发展有关。然而,家庭学习环境和数学成绩之间的显著关系并没有被一致地发现,而且可能在不同的文化和社会政治背景下有所不同。在这里,我们探索了243名儿童(3-5岁)的家庭环境和早期计算技能,他们没有参加南非开普敦非常低收入的学前教育项目。照顾者完成了一份调查问卷,包括关于儿童在家中经历的信息;孩子们完成了数字识别任务、计数任务和给予n任务。家庭学习环境中的资源数量(例如书籍和玩具的数量)、照顾者与孩子进行家庭学习活动的频率、照顾者的教育程度和收入水平与数字知识无关。虽然在之前的研究中,家庭学习环境已被证明对培养早期计算技能很重要,但本研究表明,除了家庭学习环境之外,其他因素也可能是南非低收入环境中培养计算技能和入学准备的重要目标。
{"title":"“We don’t have things for counting”: An exploration of early numeracy skills and home learning experiences of children growing up in poverty in South Africa","authors":"R. Merkley, Elizabeth Sernoskie, C. Cook, S. Howard, Hleliwe Makaula, Mbulelo Mshudulu, Nosibusiso Tshetu, C. Draper, G. Scerif","doi":"10.5964/jnc.8061","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.8061","url":null,"abstract":"A child’s home environment has been shown to be related to the development of early numeracy skills in some countries. However, significant relationships between home learning environment and math achievement have not consistently been found, and likely vary across different cultural and socio-political contexts. Here we explored the home environment and early numeracy skills of 243 children (3-5 years), who were not attending preschool programmes in very low-income settings in Cape Town, South Africa. Caregivers completed a questionnaire including information regarding experiences of children in the home; children completed a number identification task, a counting task and the Give-N task. The amount of resources in the home learning environment (e.g. the number of books and toys), frequency of home learning activities caregivers did with their children, and caregiver levels of education and income were not associated with number knowledge. While the home learning environment has been shown to be important for developing early numeracy skills in previous research, this study suggests that factors other than the home learning environment may also be important targets to foster numeracy skills and school readiness in low-income settings in South Africa.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47361356","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Number line estimation patterns and their relationship with mathematical performance 数线估计模式及其与数学性能的关系
Q2 Mathematics Pub Date : 2023-07-31 DOI: 10.5964/jnc.10557
Carola Ruiz, Saskia Kohnen, R. Bull
There is ongoing debate regarding what performance on the number line estimation task represents and its role in mathematics learning. The patterns followed by children’s estimates on the number line task could provide insight into this. This study investigates children’s estimation patterns on the number line task and assesses whether mathematics achievement is associated with these estimation patterns. Singaporean children (n = 324, Age M = 6.2 years, Age SD = 0.3 years) in their second year of kindergarten were assessed on the number line task (0-100) and their mathematical performance (Numerical Operations and Mathematical Reasoning subtests from WIAT II). The results show that most children’s number line estimation patterns can be explained by at least one mathematical model (i.e., linear, logarithmic, unbounded power model, one-cycle power model, two-cycle power model). But the findings also highlight the high percentage of participants for which more than one model shows similar support. Children’s mathematical achievement differed based on the models that best explained children’s estimation patterns. Children whose estimation patterns corresponded to a more advanced model tended to show higher mathematical achievement. Limitations of drawing conclusions regarding what performance on the number line task represents based on models that best explain the estimation patterns are discussed.
关于数轴估计任务的表现及其在数学学习中的作用,一直存在争论。孩子们在数轴任务中所遵循的估计模式可以对此提供深入的了解。本研究调查了儿童在数轴任务上的估计模式,并评估了数学成绩是否与这些估计模式有关。对324名新加坡幼儿园二年级儿童(n = 324,年龄M = 6.2岁,年龄SD = 0.3岁)的数线任务(0-100)和数学表现(WIAT II的数值运算和数学推理子测试)进行了评估。结果表明,大多数儿童的数线估计模式可以用至少一种数学模型(即线性、对数、无界幂模型、单周期幂模型、双周期幂模型)来解释。但研究结果也强调了一个以上模型显示出类似支持的参与者比例很高。孩子们的数学成绩取决于最能解释孩子们的估计模式的模型。估计模式符合更高级模型的儿童往往表现出更高的数学成就。讨论了基于最能解释估计模式的模型得出关于数轴任务上的性能所代表的结论的局限性。
{"title":"Number line estimation patterns and their relationship with mathematical performance","authors":"Carola Ruiz, Saskia Kohnen, R. Bull","doi":"10.5964/jnc.10557","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.10557","url":null,"abstract":"There is ongoing debate regarding what performance on the number line estimation task represents and its role in mathematics learning. The patterns followed by children’s estimates on the number line task could provide insight into this. This study investigates children’s estimation patterns on the number line task and assesses whether mathematics achievement is associated with these estimation patterns. Singaporean children (n = 324, Age M = 6.2 years, Age SD = 0.3 years) in their second year of kindergarten were assessed on the number line task (0-100) and their mathematical performance (Numerical Operations and Mathematical Reasoning subtests from WIAT II). The results show that most children’s number line estimation patterns can be explained by at least one mathematical model (i.e., linear, logarithmic, unbounded power model, one-cycle power model, two-cycle power model). But the findings also highlight the high percentage of participants for which more than one model shows similar support. Children’s mathematical achievement differed based on the models that best explained children’s estimation patterns. Children whose estimation patterns corresponded to a more advanced model tended to show higher mathematical achievement. Limitations of drawing conclusions regarding what performance on the number line task represents based on models that best explain the estimation patterns are discussed.","PeriodicalId":36632,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Numerical Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45662667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Numerical Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1