Pub Date : 2022-09-01DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09243-5
Alexander Wimmer
{"title":"zhi-{yao, you} ‘only-{need, have}’: on two conditional connectives in Mandarin","authors":"Alexander Wimmer","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09243-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09243-5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"401-438"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48027689","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-08-08DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09240-8
A. Arano
{"title":"Reconsidering multiple scrambling in Japanese","authors":"A. Arano","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09240-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09240-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"265 - 303"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42683543","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-06-07DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09239-1
Qiongpeng Luo
This article motivates and develops a compositional account for bare noun incorporation (BNI) constructions in Mandarin Chinese. Mandarin BNI constructions, taking the form of V-O compounds, exhibit a constellation of properties (e.g., obligatory narrow scope, institutionalized meaning, reduced discourse capacity, restricted modification of incorporated nominals, etc.) which are typically associated with (pseudo-)incorporated structures in other languages. However, unlike other attested (pseudo-)incorporated structures, which are mostly verbal in nature, BNI constructions can be freely used as arguments, akin to nominalized expressions. Integrating the analytical insights from both the advances in the theories of kinds (Chierchia in Nat Lang Semant 6: 339–405, 1998; Gehrke in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 33: 897–938, 2015) and in the theories of incorporation (Dayal in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 29: 123–167, 2011; Schwarzs in Weak referentiality, John Benjamins, 2014), the article proposes an event kind-based analysis by treating BNI constructions as expressions of Chierchia-style kinds in the domain of events, where the (proto-typical) theme arguments instantiating the bare noun complements form part of the event kinds rather than function as independent semantic arguments to the verbs. Extending the notion of kinds from the domain of individuals to the domain of events has not only provided a motivated account of the paradoxical properties of BNI constructions which would otherwise defy formal treatment, but also bridged two lines of research previously thought to be independent of each other, viz. the semantics of kinds which are mostly confined to the domain of individuals and the semantics of events which are mostly confined to canonical verbal expressions.
{"title":"Bare nouns, incorporation, and event kinds in Mandarin Chinese","authors":"Qiongpeng Luo","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09239-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09239-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This article motivates and develops a compositional account for bare noun incorporation (BNI) constructions in Mandarin Chinese. Mandarin BNI constructions, taking the form of V-O compounds, exhibit a constellation of properties (e.g., obligatory narrow scope, institutionalized meaning, reduced discourse capacity, restricted modification of incorporated nominals, etc.) which are typically associated with (pseudo-)incorporated structures in other languages. However, unlike other attested (pseudo-)incorporated structures, which are mostly verbal in nature, BNI constructions can be freely used as arguments, akin to nominalized expressions. Integrating the analytical insights from both the advances in the theories of kinds (Chierchia in Nat Lang Semant 6: 339–405, 1998; Gehrke in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 33: 897–938, 2015) and in the theories of incorporation (Dayal in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 29: 123–167, 2011; Schwarzs in <i>Weak referentiality</i>, John Benjamins, 2014), the article proposes an event kind-based analysis by treating BNI constructions as expressions of Chierchia-style kinds in the domain of events, where the (proto-typical) theme arguments instantiating the bare noun complements form part of the event kinds rather than function as independent semantic arguments to the verbs. Extending the notion of kinds from the domain of individuals to the domain of events has not only provided a motivated account of the paradoxical properties of BNI constructions which would otherwise defy formal treatment, but also bridged two lines of research previously thought to be independent of each other, viz. the semantics of kinds which are mostly confined to the domain of individuals and the semantics of events which are mostly confined to canonical verbal expressions.</p>","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"58 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138520250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-05-31DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09238-2
C. Liu
{"title":"The possessive morphosyntactic strategy of gradable predication in Taiwanese Southern Min and the measure function","authors":"C. Liu","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09238-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09238-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"179 - 220"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49304853","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-05-09DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09237-3
So-young Park
{"title":"Two types of plurals and numeral classifiers in classifier languages: the case of Korean","authors":"So-young Park","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09237-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09237-3","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"139 - 177"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47510611","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-04-02DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09236-4
Zhiguo Xie
{"title":"Epistemic modality and comparison in Mandarin Chinese","authors":"Zhiguo Xie","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09236-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09236-4","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"99 - 137"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42894974","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-03-25DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09235-5
Satoshi Tomioka
Unlike typical wh-questions, why-questions are known to be focus-sensitive, but the linguistic realization of their focus sensitivity shows an unexpected pattern in Japanese. The phrase that immediately follows a causal wh-phrase can be considered as the focus associate without any focal prominence. This prosodic pattern contradicts the generally accepted view that a focused phrase invariably receives focal prominence (pitch boost) in Japanese. The paper presents an analysis based on focus movement for this surprising prosodic pattern. We characterize the focus sensitivity of a why-question as an association-with-focus effect with the silent focus exhaustivity operator. The adjacency of a causal wh-phrase and the focus associate is a result of the focus movement to the operator position, which mimics the focus movement proposed by some of the advocates of focus association by movement (Krifka in The Architecture of Focus 82:105, 2006; Wagner in Natural Language Semantics 14(4):297-324, 2006; as reported by Erlewine (Movement out of focus, 2014)). We argue that the adjacency strategy, which places a focus associate immediately after why, is a syntactic manifestation of association with focus, and that this structural disambiguation makes prosodic marking unnecessary. The proposal brings a functional perspective to the syntax–semantics–prosody correspondence in such a way that a focus-marked phrase does not automatically lead to prosodic prominence and the phonological interpretation of focus is influenced by the consideration of usefulness.
与典型的“为什么”问句不同,“为什么”问句具有焦点敏感性,但其焦点敏感性的语言实现在日语中呈现出一种意想不到的模式。紧跟着因果性的“wh”短语的短语可以被认为是没有任何焦点突出的焦点联想。这种韵律模式与普遍接受的观点相矛盾,即在日语中,重点短语总是得到重点突出(音调提高)。本文对这一惊人的韵律模式进行了基于焦点运动的分析。我们将“为什么”问题的焦点敏感性描述为与沉默焦点穷竭运算符的焦点关联效应。因果h-短语和焦点关联的邻接是焦点移动到操作者位置的结果,这模仿了一些主张通过运动进行焦点关联的人提出的焦点移动(Krifka in The Architecture of focus 82:105, 2006;瓦格纳在自然语言语义14(4):297- 324,2006;正如Erlewine(运动失焦,2014)所报道的那样。我们认为,邻接策略(将焦点关联紧接在“为什么”之后)是与焦点关联的句法表现,这种结构上的消歧使韵律标记变得不必要。该建议从功能的角度来看待语法-语义-韵律的对应关系,这样一个焦点标记短语不会自动导致韵律突出,焦点的语音解释受到有用性考虑的影响。
{"title":"Focus without pitch boost: focus sensitivity in Japanese why-questions and its theoretical implications","authors":"Satoshi Tomioka","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09235-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09235-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Unlike typical wh-questions, <i>why</i>-questions are known to be focus-sensitive, but the linguistic realization of their focus sensitivity shows an unexpected pattern in Japanese. The phrase that immediately follows a causal wh-phrase can be considered as the focus associate without any focal prominence. This prosodic pattern contradicts the generally accepted view that a focused phrase invariably receives focal prominence (pitch boost) in Japanese. The paper presents an analysis based on focus movement for this surprising prosodic pattern. We characterize the focus sensitivity of a <i>why</i>-question as an association-with-focus effect with the silent focus exhaustivity operator. The adjacency of a causal wh-phrase and the focus associate is a result of the focus movement to the operator position, which mimics the focus movement proposed by some of the advocates of focus association by movement (Krifka in The Architecture of Focus 82:105, 2006; Wagner in Natural Language Semantics 14(4):297-324, 2006; as reported by Erlewine (Movement out of focus, 2014)). We argue that the adjacency strategy, which places a focus associate immediately after <i>why</i>, is a syntactic manifestation of association with focus, and that this structural disambiguation makes prosodic marking unnecessary. The proposal brings a functional perspective to the syntax–semantics–prosody correspondence in such a way that a focus-marked phrase does not automatically lead to prosodic prominence and the phonological interpretation of focus is influenced by the consideration of usefulness.\u0000</p>","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"95 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138520225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-03-01DOI: 10.1007/s10831-021-09233-z
H. Kishimoto, Kazushige Moriyama
{"title":"Adverbial particle modification and argument ellipsis in Japanese","authors":"H. Kishimoto, Kazushige Moriyama","doi":"10.1007/s10831-021-09233-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-021-09233-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"31 1","pages":"1 - 43"},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49513919","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-02-25DOI: 10.1007/s10831-022-09234-6
Hana Jee, Monica Tamariz, Richard Shillcock
Studies of word-level meaning-sound systematicity in English and four other European languages have shown that words that sound similar tend to have similar meanings. The term ‘systematicity’ in this research tradition is defined as statistically non-arbitrary relations between sub-domains of language, in contrast to the traditionally assumed Saussurian arbitrariness. We explore such systematicity in a typologically distinct language, Korean. We find a relatively high level of systematicity, which we attribute to the method of analysis where we applied Latent Semantic Analysis based on eo-jeols—sequences of syllable-blocks bounded by spaces in an internet corpus of written Korean. Eo-jeols embody a psychologically realistic spectrum of linguistic structure and influence, compared with previous purely lexically based studies of systematicity. Systematicity was pervasive in our sample of the Korean lexicon—partitioned by word frequency, etymological origin, syllabic constituents (onset, vowel, coda, rhyme), syntactic categories, homonyms, onomatopoeia, and loanwords—suggesting a fundamental basis for systematicity. We explain meaning-sound systematicity in terms of related degrees of cognitive effort in speaking and listening.
{"title":"Exploring meaning-sound systematicity in Korean","authors":"Hana Jee, Monica Tamariz, Richard Shillcock","doi":"10.1007/s10831-022-09234-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-022-09234-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies of word-level meaning-sound systematicity in English and four other European languages have shown that words that sound similar tend to have similar meanings. The term ‘systematicity’ in this research tradition is defined as statistically non-arbitrary relations between sub-domains of language, in contrast to the traditionally assumed Saussurian arbitrariness. We explore such systematicity in a typologically distinct language, Korean. We find a relatively high level of systematicity, which we attribute to the method of analysis where we applied Latent Semantic Analysis based on <i>eo-jeols</i>—sequences of syllable-blocks bounded by spaces in an internet corpus of written Korean. Eo-jeols embody a psychologically realistic spectrum of linguistic structure and influence, compared with previous purely lexically based studies of systematicity. Systematicity was pervasive in our sample of the Korean lexicon—partitioned by word frequency, etymological origin, syllabic constituents (onset, vowel, coda, rhyme), syntactic categories, homonyms, onomatopoeia, and loanwords—suggesting a fundamental basis for systematicity. We explain meaning-sound systematicity in terms of related degrees of cognitive effort in speaking and listening.</p>","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"15 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-02-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138520232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2022-02-12DOI: 10.1007/s10831-021-09229-9
Isaac Gould
This paper presents the first detailed study of pronouncing multiple wh-pronouns within the same dependency in Mon (Mon-Khmer). I argue the data involve movement, and thus a wh-copying construction: multiple wh-copies can be pronounced, either in full pronoun form or in a reduced pronoun form—and I propose reduction occurs via m-merger (Harizanov in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 32:1033–1088, 2014). This supports the view (McCloskey in Everaert, van Riemsdijk (eds) The Blackwell companion to syntax, Blackwell, Oxford 94–117, 2006) that resumptive pronouns can be the pronunciation of structurally reduced copies. Interestingly, the distribution of full and reduced copies is highly free, although there is a puzzling restriction on where reduced copies can appear, which is analyzed with a context-sensitive constraint that is subject to Richards’s (Linguist Inq 29:599–629, 1998) Principle of Minimal Compliance. This relatively free, though constrained, distribution is novel, and is challenging for prominent approaches to copy-chain realization. For example, the linearization-based approach of Nunes (Linearization of chains and sideward movement, MIT Press, Cambridge, 2004) struggles to account for this restriction, and though I follow the economy-based approach of Van Urk (Nat Lang Linguist Theory 36:937–990, 2018) in having the syntax specify which copies end up pronounced, I show that economy does not drive copy reduction in the data here.
本文首次详细研究了在孟高棉语中同一依赖语中多个wh-代词的发音。我认为数据涉及移动,因此是一个“wh-copy”结构:多个“wh-copy”可以以完整的代词形式或略读的代词形式发音,我认为略读是通过“m-merger”发生的(Harizanov在《Nat Lang语言学家理论》32:33 - 1088,2014)。这支持了McCloskey in Everaert, van Riemsdijk(主编)the Blackwell companion to syntax, Blackwell, Oxford 94-117, 2006)的观点,即恢复代词可以是结构简化副本的发音。有趣的是,完整副本和简化副本的分发是高度自由的,尽管在简化副本可以出现的地方有一个令人困惑的限制,这是根据Richards (Linguist Inq 29:59 - 629, 1998)最小遵从原则的上下文敏感约束来分析的。这种相对自由(尽管受到限制)的分布是新颖的,并且对实现复制链的主要方法具有挑战性。例如,Nunes的基于线性化的方法(链和横向运动的线性化,麻省理工学院出版社,剑桥,2004年)努力解释这一限制,尽管我遵循Van Urk的基于经济的方法(Nat Lang语言学家理论36:937-990,2018),让语法指定哪些副本最终发音,但我表明经济并没有推动数据中的副本减少。
{"title":"On wh-copying in Mon","authors":"Isaac Gould","doi":"10.1007/s10831-021-09229-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10831-021-09229-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This paper presents the first detailed study of pronouncing multiple <i>wh</i>-pronouns within the same dependency in Mon (Mon-Khmer). I argue the data involve movement, and thus a <i>wh</i>-copying construction: multiple <i>wh</i>-copies can be pronounced, either in full pronoun form or in a reduced pronoun form—and I propose reduction occurs via <i>m-merger</i> (Harizanov in Nat Lang Linguist Theory 32:1033–1088, 2014). This supports the view (McCloskey in Everaert, van Riemsdijk (eds) The Blackwell companion to syntax, Blackwell, Oxford 94–117, 2006) that resumptive pronouns can be the pronunciation of structurally reduced copies. Interestingly, the distribution of full and reduced copies is highly free, although there is a puzzling restriction on where reduced copies can appear, which is analyzed with a context-sensitive constraint that is subject to Richards’s (Linguist Inq 29:599–629, 1998) Principle of Minimal Compliance. This relatively free, though constrained, distribution is novel, and is challenging for prominent approaches to copy-chain realization. For example, the linearization-based approach of Nunes (Linearization of chains and sideward movement, MIT Press, Cambridge, 2004) struggles to account for this restriction, and though I follow the economy-based approach of Van Urk (Nat Lang Linguist Theory 36:937–990, 2018) in having the syntax specify which copies end up pronounced, I show that economy does not drive copy reduction in the data here.</p>","PeriodicalId":45331,"journal":{"name":"Journal of East Asian Linguistics","volume":"47 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.5,"publicationDate":"2022-02-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138520249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}