首页 > 最新文献

Musicae Scientiae最新文献

英文 中文
Book Review: Aaron Williamon, Jane Ginsborg, Rosie Perkins, and George Waddell. Performing music research: Methods in music education, psychology, and performance science 书评:亚伦·威廉,简·金斯伯格,罗茜·珀金斯,乔治·瓦德尔。表演音乐研究:音乐教育、心理学和表演科学的方法
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-11-28 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221129749
R. Timmers
{"title":"Book Review: Aaron Williamon, Jane Ginsborg, Rosie Perkins, and George Waddell. Performing music research: Methods in music education, psychology, and performance science","authors":"R. Timmers","doi":"10.1177/10298649221129749","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221129749","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"115 1","pages":"510 - 513"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86203968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hearing functional harmony in jazz: A perceptual study of music-theoretical accounts of extended tonality 爵士乐中功能性和声的听觉:扩展调性的音乐理论描述的感性研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-11-05 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221122245
G. Cecchetti, S. Herff, Christoph Finkensiep, Daniel Harasim, M. Rohrmeier
Functional harmony is an integral part of many repertoires in the Western musical practices, including both diatonic and extended tonality. In the latter context, music-theoretical accounts suggest that the three octatonic equivalence classes (OECs) consisting of pitch-classes related by stacked minor-third intervals may be associated with tonic (T), dominant (D), and subdominant (S) functions. Whether this theoretical description of music is also relevant to the perception of music has not yet been tested empirically. In this study, 100 participants familiar with Western repertoires were presented with jazz chord progressions containing chord substitutions. When each stimulus had been played, participants predicted how many more chords they would have expected to hear before the progression could reach a plausible conclusion. We computed the similarity of responses for pairs of stimuli containing different harmonic substitutions and modeled such similarity values based on different measures of harmonic relatedness between substitutions. Data show that the OEC membership of substitutions strongly predicts the similarity of participants’ completion ratings. Bayesian mixed-effects modeling of similarity values further showed a categorical distinction between D and S as functional categories, on one hand, and T, on the other hand. The data also appear to reflect the prevalent influence of rock and pop repertoires on the participants, encouraging further research into the influence of stylistic diversity and musical expertise. Overall, results contribute to the characterization of listeners’ implicit knowledge of the principles of harmonic structure in extended tonality and support the relevance of OECs not only as descriptors of extended-tonal compositional practices but also parsimonious predictors of perceived functionality.
在西方音乐实践中,功能和声是许多曲目中不可或缺的一部分,包括全音阶和扩展调性。在后一种情况下,音乐理论表明,由堆叠的小三度音程相关的音阶组成的三个八音等价类(oec)可能与主音(T)、属音(D)和次属音(S)功能有关。这种对音乐的理论描述是否也与对音乐的感知有关,还没有经过实证检验。在本研究中,100名熟悉西方曲目的被试被呈现了包含和弦替换的爵士和弦进行。当每个刺激都被播放后,参与者预测他们还会听到多少和弦才能得出一个合理的结论。我们计算了包含不同谐波替换的刺激对的响应相似性,并基于替换之间谐波相关性的不同度量来模拟这种相似性值。数据表明,替代的OEC成员资格强烈地预测了参与者完成评级的相似性。相似值的Bayesian混合效应模型进一步显示了D和S作为功能类别与T作为功能类别之间的分类区别。这些数据似乎也反映了摇滚和流行曲目对参与者的普遍影响,这鼓励了对风格多样性和音乐专业知识影响的进一步研究。总体而言,这些结果有助于描述听者对扩展调性和声结构原理的内隐知识,并支持oec不仅作为扩展调性作曲实践的描述符,而且也是感知功能的简约预测因子的相关性。
{"title":"Hearing functional harmony in jazz: A perceptual study of music-theoretical accounts of extended tonality","authors":"G. Cecchetti, S. Herff, Christoph Finkensiep, Daniel Harasim, M. Rohrmeier","doi":"10.1177/10298649221122245","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221122245","url":null,"abstract":"Functional harmony is an integral part of many repertoires in the Western musical practices, including both diatonic and extended tonality. In the latter context, music-theoretical accounts suggest that the three octatonic equivalence classes (OECs) consisting of pitch-classes related by stacked minor-third intervals may be associated with tonic (T), dominant (D), and subdominant (S) functions. Whether this theoretical description of music is also relevant to the perception of music has not yet been tested empirically. In this study, 100 participants familiar with Western repertoires were presented with jazz chord progressions containing chord substitutions. When each stimulus had been played, participants predicted how many more chords they would have expected to hear before the progression could reach a plausible conclusion. We computed the similarity of responses for pairs of stimuli containing different harmonic substitutions and modeled such similarity values based on different measures of harmonic relatedness between substitutions. Data show that the OEC membership of substitutions strongly predicts the similarity of participants’ completion ratings. Bayesian mixed-effects modeling of similarity values further showed a categorical distinction between D and S as functional categories, on one hand, and T, on the other hand. The data also appear to reflect the prevalent influence of rock and pop repertoires on the participants, encouraging further research into the influence of stylistic diversity and musical expertise. Overall, results contribute to the characterization of listeners’ implicit knowledge of the principles of harmonic structure in extended tonality and support the relevance of OECs not only as descriptors of extended-tonal compositional practices but also parsimonious predictors of perceived functionality.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"16 1","pages":"672 - 697"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87071809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The effect of dichotic music presentation on ratings of emotional facial expressions 二元音乐呈现对情绪性面部表情评分的影响
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-11-04 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221126904
Erika Y Hae, Bianca DM Hatin
Music has a cross-modal influence on the emotional appraisal of pictures, probably due to a misattribution of emotion from music to the visually perceived images. Moreover, dichotic listening studies have demonstrated a left-ear (right-hemisphere) advantage for processing emotional and/or musical stimuli. The present study investigated the role of laterality in cross-modal affect misattribution: that is, whether visual judgments of emotion would be affected differently depending on which ear was presented with music via the dichotic listening task. Participants rated the emotionality of happy, sad, and neutral faces while listening to happy or sad music in one ear and white noise in the other. Baseline ratings without music were used as a comparison to see whether and how emotionality judgments shifted in the music conditions. As predicted, the results showed that happy music played to the left ear had a stronger cross-modal influence on ratings of sad faces than the same music played to the right ear. Furthermore, sad music affected the ratings of all faces regardless of whether it was played to the left or right ear. These results do not fully align with any given lateralized model of emotion processing, suggesting that other factors such as negativity bias may play a role.
音乐对图片的情感评价有跨模态的影响,可能是由于情感从音乐到视觉感知图像的错误归因。此外,二分法听力研究表明,左耳(右半球)在处理情绪和/或音乐刺激方面具有优势。本研究探讨了偏侧性在跨模态情感错误归因中的作用:即在二元听音任务中,当音乐呈现在不同的耳朵时,情感的视觉判断是否会受到不同的影响。参与者在一只耳朵听快乐或悲伤的音乐,另一只耳朵听白噪音时,对快乐、悲伤和中性面孔的情绪进行评级。在没有音乐的情况下,基线评分被用作对比,以观察情绪判断是否以及如何在音乐条件下发生变化。正如预测的那样,结果表明,左耳播放的快乐音乐比右耳播放的相同音乐对悲伤面孔的评分有更强的跨模态影响。此外,悲伤的音乐影响了所有面孔的评分,无论它是在左耳还是右耳播放。这些结果并不完全符合任何给定的情绪处理的偏侧模型,这表明其他因素,如消极偏见可能起作用。
{"title":"The effect of dichotic music presentation on ratings of emotional facial expressions","authors":"Erika Y Hae, Bianca DM Hatin","doi":"10.1177/10298649221126904","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221126904","url":null,"abstract":"Music has a cross-modal influence on the emotional appraisal of pictures, probably due to a misattribution of emotion from music to the visually perceived images. Moreover, dichotic listening studies have demonstrated a left-ear (right-hemisphere) advantage for processing emotional and/or musical stimuli. The present study investigated the role of laterality in cross-modal affect misattribution: that is, whether visual judgments of emotion would be affected differently depending on which ear was presented with music via the dichotic listening task. Participants rated the emotionality of happy, sad, and neutral faces while listening to happy or sad music in one ear and white noise in the other. Baseline ratings without music were used as a comparison to see whether and how emotionality judgments shifted in the music conditions. As predicted, the results showed that happy music played to the left ear had a stronger cross-modal influence on ratings of sad faces than the same music played to the right ear. Furthermore, sad music affected the ratings of all faces regardless of whether it was played to the left or right ear. These results do not fully align with any given lateralized model of emotion processing, suggesting that other factors such as negativity bias may play a role.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"16 1","pages":"741 - 756"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77068637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tell me what you see: An exploratory investigation of visual mental imagery evoked by music 告诉我你看到了什么:音乐引发的视觉心理意象的探索性调查
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-11-03 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221124862
S. Dahl, Antonio Stella, Thomas Bjørner
The link between musical structure and evoked visual mental imagery (VMI), that is, seeing in the absence of a corresponding sensory stimulus, has yet to be thoroughly investigated. We explored this link by manipulating the characteristics of four pieces of music for synthesizer, guitars, and percussion (songs). Two original songs were selected on the basis of a pilot study, and two were new, specially composed to combine the musical and acoustical characteristics of the originals. A total of 135 participants were randomly assigned to one of the four groups who listened to one song each; 73% of participants reported experiencing VMI. There were similarities between participants’ descriptions of the mental imagery evoked by each song and clear differences between them. A combination of coding and content analysis produced 10 categories: Nature, Places and settings, Objects, Time, Movements and events, Color(s), Humans, Affects, Literal sound, and Film. Regardless of whether or not they had reported experiencing VMI, participants then carried out a card-sorting task in which they selected the terms they thought best described a scene or setting appropriate to the music they had heard and rated emotional dimensions. The results confirmed those of the content analysis. Taken together, participants’ ratings, descriptions of VMI, and selection of terms in the card-sorting task confirmed that new songs combining the characteristics of original songs evoke the elements of VMI associated with the latter. The findings are important for the understanding of the musical and acoustical characteristics that may influence our experiences of music, including VMI.
音乐结构和唤起的视觉心理意象(VMI)之间的联系,即在没有相应感官刺激的情况下观看,还有待深入研究。我们通过操纵合成器、吉他和打击乐(歌曲)的四段音乐的特征来探索这种联系。在初步研究的基础上选择了两首原创歌曲,其中两首是特别创作的,结合了原作的音乐和声学特征。共有135名参与者被随机分配到四组,每组听一首歌;73%的参与者报告有VMI。参与者对每首歌所唤起的心理意象的描述有相似之处,也有明显的差异。编码和内容分析的结合产生了10个类别:自然、地点和设置、对象、时间、运动和事件、颜色、人类、影响、文字声音和电影。不管他们是否报告经历过VMI,参与者随后进行了一项卡片分类任务,他们选择他们认为最能描述他们所听音乐的场景或背景的术语,并对情感维度进行评级。结果证实了含量分析的结果。总的来说,参与者的评分、对VMI的描述以及在卡片分类任务中的术语选择证实,结合了原创歌曲特征的新歌唤起了与后者相关的VMI元素。这些发现对于理解可能影响我们音乐体验(包括VMI)的音乐和声学特征非常重要。
{"title":"Tell me what you see: An exploratory investigation of visual mental imagery evoked by music","authors":"S. Dahl, Antonio Stella, Thomas Bjørner","doi":"10.1177/10298649221124862","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221124862","url":null,"abstract":"The link between musical structure and evoked visual mental imagery (VMI), that is, seeing in the absence of a corresponding sensory stimulus, has yet to be thoroughly investigated. We explored this link by manipulating the characteristics of four pieces of music for synthesizer, guitars, and percussion (songs). Two original songs were selected on the basis of a pilot study, and two were new, specially composed to combine the musical and acoustical characteristics of the originals. A total of 135 participants were randomly assigned to one of the four groups who listened to one song each; 73% of participants reported experiencing VMI. There were similarities between participants’ descriptions of the mental imagery evoked by each song and clear differences between them. A combination of coding and content analysis produced 10 categories: Nature, Places and settings, Objects, Time, Movements and events, Color(s), Humans, Affects, Literal sound, and Film. Regardless of whether or not they had reported experiencing VMI, participants then carried out a card-sorting task in which they selected the terms they thought best described a scene or setting appropriate to the music they had heard and rated emotional dimensions. The results confirmed those of the content analysis. Taken together, participants’ ratings, descriptions of VMI, and selection of terms in the card-sorting task confirmed that new songs combining the characteristics of original songs evoke the elements of VMI associated with the latter. The findings are important for the understanding of the musical and acoustical characteristics that may influence our experiences of music, including VMI.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"1 1","pages":"717 - 740"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77067450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Expressive goals for performing musicians: The case of clarinetists 表演音乐家的表达目标:以单簧管演奏家为例
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-20 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221122155
A. Almeida, Weicong Li, Emery Schubert, John Smith, J. Wolfe
Music can convey emotions. Even in the performance of written rather than improvised music, the performer can modify the way they play particular elements of the music to convey specific emotions. Considerable research attention has been paid to the ways in which performers convey a small set of so-called basic emotions. In the current work, we investigated how musicians think they can convey a larger set of emotions in their performances by modifying musical elements and varying playing parameters. We presented 12 clarinetists trained in Western classical music with 55 pairs of expressive goals (EGs) derived from a list of 11 (e.g., to make the music sound happy, sad, or expressive), and asked them to provide similarity judgments for each pair representing how well they thought they would be able to distinguish between them when performing four excerpts of music, and how they would do it. The similarity judgments produced dissimilarity scores that were grouped using cluster analysis to form six independent, coherent clusters of EGs. Four clusters consisted of, or included, EGs representing four basic emotions: happy/humorous; sad; angry/mad/ugly; and fearful. The other two clusters consisted of the EGs expressive/overly expressive/beautiful and deadpan. We therefore suggest that this set of six clusters could be used in future research on emotional and musical expression in music. Participants reported that to achieve specific EGs, they would vary the way they played a wider range of musical elements than in previous studies, albeit including many of the same elements. Among the playing parameters they reported, reed-bite force was mentioned most often, with consistent associations between more force and ugly or angry, and between less force and fearful and expressive EGs.
音乐可以传达情感。即使在表演书面音乐而不是即兴音乐时,表演者也可以修改他们演奏音乐中特定元素的方式来传达特定的情感。相当多的研究关注于表演者传达一小部分所谓的基本情感的方式。在目前的工作中,我们调查了音乐家如何认为他们可以通过修改音乐元素和改变演奏参数来传达更多的情感。我们向12名接受过西方古典音乐训练的单簧管演奏家提供了55对表达目标(eg),这些目标来自11个目标(例如,让音乐听起来快乐、悲伤或富有表现力),并要求他们为每对提供相似性判断,代表他们认为在演奏四段音乐片段时能够区分它们的程度,以及他们将如何做到这一点。相似性判断产生不同的分数,使用聚类分析将其分组,形成6个独立的、连贯的EGs聚类。四组由代表四种基本情绪的eg组成或包含:快乐/幽默;悲伤的;愤怒/生气/丑陋;和恐惧。另外两组是表情型/过度表情型/美丽型和面无表情型。因此,我们建议这六个集群可以用于未来音乐中情感和音乐表达的研究。参与者报告说,为了达到特定的EGs,他们会改变演奏音乐元素的方式,比以前的研究更广泛,尽管包括许多相同的元素。在他们报告的游戏参数中,芦苇咬合力被提及最多,更大的力量与丑陋或愤怒之间存在一致的联系,而更小的力量与恐惧和表达性情绪之间存在一致的联系。
{"title":"Expressive goals for performing musicians: The case of clarinetists","authors":"A. Almeida, Weicong Li, Emery Schubert, John Smith, J. Wolfe","doi":"10.1177/10298649221122155","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221122155","url":null,"abstract":"Music can convey emotions. Even in the performance of written rather than improvised music, the performer can modify the way they play particular elements of the music to convey specific emotions. Considerable research attention has been paid to the ways in which performers convey a small set of so-called basic emotions. In the current work, we investigated how musicians think they can convey a larger set of emotions in their performances by modifying musical elements and varying playing parameters. We presented 12 clarinetists trained in Western classical music with 55 pairs of expressive goals (EGs) derived from a list of 11 (e.g., to make the music sound happy, sad, or expressive), and asked them to provide similarity judgments for each pair representing how well they thought they would be able to distinguish between them when performing four excerpts of music, and how they would do it. The similarity judgments produced dissimilarity scores that were grouped using cluster analysis to form six independent, coherent clusters of EGs. Four clusters consisted of, or included, EGs representing four basic emotions: happy/humorous; sad; angry/mad/ugly; and fearful. The other two clusters consisted of the EGs expressive/overly expressive/beautiful and deadpan. We therefore suggest that this set of six clusters could be used in future research on emotional and musical expression in music. Participants reported that to achieve specific EGs, they would vary the way they played a wider range of musical elements than in previous studies, albeit including many of the same elements. Among the playing parameters they reported, reed-bite force was mentioned most often, with consistent associations between more force and ugly or angry, and between less force and fearful and expressive EGs.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"75 1","pages":"655 - 671"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83378914","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Auditory imagery in congenital amusia 先天性失音症的听觉意象
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-13 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221122870
Ariadne Loutrari, Kathryn Ansell, C. Philip Beaman, Cunmei Jiang, Fang Liu
Congenital amusia is a neurogenetic disorder affecting various aspects of music and speech processing. Although perception and auditory imagery in the general population may share mechanisms, it is not known whether previously documented perceptual impairments in amusia are coupled with difficulties in imaging auditory objects. We employed the Bucknell Auditory Imagery Scale (BAIS) to assess participants’ self-perceived voluntary imagery and a short earworm questionnaire to gauge their subjective experience of involuntary musical imagery. A total of 32 participants with amusia and 34 matched controls, recruited based on their performance on the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA), filled out the questionnaires in their own time. The earworm scores of amusic participants were not statistically significantly different from those of controls. By contrast, their scores on vividness and control of auditory imagery were significantly lower relative to controls. Overall, results suggest that the presence of amusia may not have an adverse effect on generating involuntary musical imagery—at the level of self-report—but still significantly reduces the individual’s self-rated voluntary imagery of musical, vocal, and environmental sounds. We discuss the findings in the light of previous research on explicit musical judgments and implicit engagement with music, while also touching on some statistical power considerations.
先天性失音症是一种影响音乐和语言处理各个方面的神经遗传性疾病。虽然一般人群的感知和听觉意象可能有共同的机制,但尚不清楚先前记录的失音症患者的感知障碍是否与想象听觉物体的困难相结合。本研究采用巴克内尔听觉意象量表(BAIS)评估被试自我感知的自愿意象,并采用短耳虫问卷评估被试对非自愿音乐意象的主观体验。共有32名患有失音症的参与者和34名匹配的对照组,根据他们在蒙特利尔失音症评估电池(MBEA)中的表现招募,他们在自己的时间填写了问卷。音乐组的耳虫得分与对照组的差异无统计学意义。相比之下,他们在生动度和听觉意象控制方面的得分明显低于对照组。总的来说,研究结果表明,失音症的存在可能不会对自发的音乐图像产生不利影响——在自我报告的水平上——但仍然显著降低了个体对音乐、声乐和环境声音的自我评价的自发图像。我们根据之前关于显性音乐判断和隐性音乐参与的研究讨论了这些发现,同时也涉及了一些统计能力方面的考虑。
{"title":"Auditory imagery in congenital amusia","authors":"Ariadne Loutrari, Kathryn Ansell, C. Philip Beaman, Cunmei Jiang, Fang Liu","doi":"10.1177/10298649221122870","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221122870","url":null,"abstract":"Congenital amusia is a neurogenetic disorder affecting various aspects of music and speech processing. Although perception and auditory imagery in the general population may share mechanisms, it is not known whether previously documented perceptual impairments in amusia are coupled with difficulties in imaging auditory objects. We employed the Bucknell Auditory Imagery Scale (BAIS) to assess participants’ self-perceived voluntary imagery and a short earworm questionnaire to gauge their subjective experience of involuntary musical imagery. A total of 32 participants with amusia and 34 matched controls, recruited based on their performance on the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA), filled out the questionnaires in their own time. The earworm scores of amusic participants were not statistically significantly different from those of controls. By contrast, their scores on vividness and control of auditory imagery were significantly lower relative to controls. Overall, results suggest that the presence of amusia may not have an adverse effect on generating involuntary musical imagery—at the level of self-report—but still significantly reduces the individual’s self-rated voluntary imagery of musical, vocal, and environmental sounds. We discuss the findings in the light of previous research on explicit musical judgments and implicit engagement with music, while also touching on some statistical power considerations.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"68 1","pages":"698 - 716"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87412601","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Tracing change during music therapy for depression: Toward a markers-based understanding of communicative behaviors 追踪抑郁症音乐治疗过程中的变化:对交流行为的基于标记的理解
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-08-15 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221116024
S. Knight, N. Spiro
This article focuses on behavioral markers—changes in communicative behaviors that reliably indicate the presence and severity of mental health conditions. We explore the potential of behavioral markers to provide new insights and approaches to diagnosis, assessment, and monitoring, with a particular focus on music therapy for depression. We propose a framework for understanding these markers that encompasses three broad functional categories fulfilled by communicative behaviors: semantic, pragmatic, and phatic. The disordered interactions observed in those with depression reflect changes in many types of communicative behavior, but much research has focused on pragmatic behaviors. However, changes in phatic behaviors also seem likely to be important, given their crucial role in facilitating interpersonal relationships. Given the strong phatic element of music-making, music represents a fertile context in which to explore these behaviors. We argue here that the uniquely multimodal and profoundly interactive environment of music therapy in particular allows for the identification of changes in pragmatic and phatic communicative behaviors that reliably indicate depression presence/severity. By identifying these behavioral markers, we open the door to new ways of assessing depression, and improving diagnosis and monitoring. Furthermore, this markers-based approach has broad implications, being applicable beyond depression and beyond music therapy.
这篇文章的重点是行为标记-交际行为的变化,可靠地表明心理健康状况的存在和严重程度。我们探索行为标记的潜力,为诊断、评估和监测提供新的见解和方法,特别关注抑郁症的音乐治疗。我们提出了一个理解这些标记的框架,其中包括交际行为所实现的三大类功能:语义、语用和语言。在抑郁症患者中观察到的无序互动反映了许多类型的交际行为的变化,但许多研究都集中在语用行为上。然而,饮食行为的改变似乎也很重要,因为它们在促进人际关系中起着至关重要的作用。考虑到音乐创作中强烈的情感元素,音乐为探索这些行为提供了一个肥沃的环境。我们认为,音乐治疗独特的多模态和深刻的互动环境特别允许识别语用和语言交际行为的变化,这些变化可靠地表明抑郁症的存在/严重程度。通过识别这些行为标记,我们为评估抑郁症的新方法打开了大门,并改善了诊断和监测。此外,这种基于标记的方法具有广泛的含义,适用于抑郁症和音乐治疗之外。
{"title":"Tracing change during music therapy for depression: Toward a markers-based understanding of communicative behaviors","authors":"S. Knight, N. Spiro","doi":"10.1177/10298649221116024","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221116024","url":null,"abstract":"This article focuses on behavioral markers—changes in communicative behaviors that reliably indicate the presence and severity of mental health conditions. We explore the potential of behavioral markers to provide new insights and approaches to diagnosis, assessment, and monitoring, with a particular focus on music therapy for depression. We propose a framework for understanding these markers that encompasses three broad functional categories fulfilled by communicative behaviors: semantic, pragmatic, and phatic. The disordered interactions observed in those with depression reflect changes in many types of communicative behavior, but much research has focused on pragmatic behaviors. However, changes in phatic behaviors also seem likely to be important, given their crucial role in facilitating interpersonal relationships. Given the strong phatic element of music-making, music represents a fertile context in which to explore these behaviors. We argue here that the uniquely multimodal and profoundly interactive environment of music therapy in particular allows for the identification of changes in pragmatic and phatic communicative behaviors that reliably indicate depression presence/severity. By identifying these behavioral markers, we open the door to new ways of assessing depression, and improving diagnosis and monitoring. Furthermore, this markers-based approach has broad implications, being applicable beyond depression and beyond music therapy.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"1 1","pages":"637 - 654"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89317821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Book Review: Raymond A. R. MacDonald and Graeme B. Wilson. The art of becoming: How group improvisation works 书评:雷蒙德·a·r·麦克唐纳和格雷姆·b·威尔逊。成为的艺术:团队的即兴创作
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-08-04 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221114568
J. Walduck
thinking about knowledge and research methods and uncertainties about “getting it right.” That is not to say that the book lacks reflective scrutiny. By contrast, in discussing advantages and disadvantages of approaches, and highlighting important developments such as the replication crisis and open access publication, it provides a fruitful basis for these discussions to be promoted in the teaching contexts where this book is used. In conclusion, I am convinced that this book will support the teaching of empirical research techniques at conservatoires and in music programs across the world, and as such will help to further this flourishing area of research from its foundations upward.
思考知识和研究方法以及“正确”的不确定性。这并不是说这本书缺乏深思熟虑。相比之下,在讨论方法的优缺点,并强调重要的发展,如复制危机和开放获取出版,它提供了一个富有成效的基础,这些讨论将在使用本书的教学环境中得到促进。总之,我相信这本书将支持在世界各地的音乐学院和音乐节目的实证研究技术的教学,因此将有助于进一步从它的基础向上研究这一蓬勃发展的领域。
{"title":"Book Review: Raymond A. R. MacDonald and Graeme B. Wilson. The art of becoming: How group improvisation works","authors":"J. Walduck","doi":"10.1177/10298649221114568","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221114568","url":null,"abstract":"thinking about knowledge and research methods and uncertainties about “getting it right.” That is not to say that the book lacks reflective scrutiny. By contrast, in discussing advantages and disadvantages of approaches, and highlighting important developments such as the replication crisis and open access publication, it provides a fruitful basis for these discussions to be promoted in the teaching contexts where this book is used. In conclusion, I am convinced that this book will support the teaching of empirical research techniques at conservatoires and in music programs across the world, and as such will help to further this flourishing area of research from its foundations upward.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"24 4","pages":"513 - 517"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72476941","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Mind-wandering during contemporary live music: An exploratory study 当代现场音乐中的走神:一项探索性研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221103210
Juliane Deil, Nina Markert, Philip Normand, Philipp Kammen, M. Küssner, Liila Taruffi
During a live concert, the mind can wander to unrelated thoughts such as personal concerns or past memories or to vivid images that are inspired by the music. This is an omnipresent phenomenon commonly referred to as mind-wandering. Psychological research on mind-wandering has explored its main characteristics, such as frequency, phenomenology, and impact on mood, both in the laboratory and in daily life contexts. This study aimed to harness the ecological setting of a live music concert to examine the occurrence and content of mind-wandering, as well as visual mental imagery as a mode through which mind-wandering occurs, and its relationship with the concertgoers’ moods before and after the music event. A self-report questionnaire (n = 43) was used to collect data at two concerts of ambient music given as part of the CTM Festival. Findings suggest that mind-wandering occurs extensively in a concert environment. While mind-wandering episodes feature negative themes and moods—in the form of dark content of the visual mental imagery associated with the program’s musical tone—the concert environment still contributes to participants feeling more inspired afterward. Overall, this study points to the potential of live music contexts to stimulate a beneficial style of mind-wandering (i.e., one that leads to a positive impact on mood and imagery), and its findings are in line with those of previous research showing that live concerts lead to increased well-being of concertgoers. Implications for well-being and a call for more systematic research on this subject are discussed.
在现场音乐会期间,思维可以游离到不相关的想法,如个人关注或过去的记忆,或由音乐激发的生动形象。这是一种普遍存在的现象,通常被称为走神。在实验室和日常生活中,关于走神的心理学研究已经探索了走神的主要特征,如频率、现象学和对情绪的影响。本研究旨在利用现场音乐会的生态环境,考察走神的发生和内容,以及作为走神发生模式的视觉心理意象,以及走神与音乐会前后观众情绪的关系。采用自我报告问卷(n = 43)收集作为CTM音乐节一部分的两场环境音乐音乐会的数据。研究结果表明,在音乐会环境中,走神现象很普遍。虽然走神的情节以消极的主题和情绪为特征——以与节目音乐音调相关的视觉心理意象的黑暗内容的形式——但音乐会的环境仍然有助于参与者在之后感受到更多的灵感。总的来说,这项研究指出,现场音乐环境有可能刺激一种有益的走神方式(即,对情绪和意象产生积极影响的方式),其发现与之前的研究一致,即现场音乐会会增加音乐会观众的幸福感。对福祉的影响和对这一主题进行更系统研究的呼吁进行了讨论。
{"title":"Mind-wandering during contemporary live music: An exploratory study","authors":"Juliane Deil, Nina Markert, Philip Normand, Philipp Kammen, M. Küssner, Liila Taruffi","doi":"10.1177/10298649221103210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221103210","url":null,"abstract":"During a live concert, the mind can wander to unrelated thoughts such as personal concerns or past memories or to vivid images that are inspired by the music. This is an omnipresent phenomenon commonly referred to as mind-wandering. Psychological research on mind-wandering has explored its main characteristics, such as frequency, phenomenology, and impact on mood, both in the laboratory and in daily life contexts. This study aimed to harness the ecological setting of a live music concert to examine the occurrence and content of mind-wandering, as well as visual mental imagery as a mode through which mind-wandering occurs, and its relationship with the concertgoers’ moods before and after the music event. A self-report questionnaire (n = 43) was used to collect data at two concerts of ambient music given as part of the CTM Festival. Findings suggest that mind-wandering occurs extensively in a concert environment. While mind-wandering episodes feature negative themes and moods—in the form of dark content of the visual mental imagery associated with the program’s musical tone—the concert environment still contributes to participants feeling more inspired afterward. Overall, this study points to the potential of live music contexts to stimulate a beneficial style of mind-wandering (i.e., one that leads to a positive impact on mood and imagery), and its findings are in line with those of previous research showing that live concerts lead to increased well-being of concertgoers. Implications for well-being and a call for more systematic research on this subject are discussed.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"60 1","pages":"616 - 636"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83517996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The Japanese translation of the Gold-MSI: Adaptation and validation of the self-report questionnaire of musical sophistication Gold-MSI的日文翻译:音乐修养自我报告问卷的改编与验证
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/10298649221110089
Makiko Sadakata, Yasumasa Yamaguchi, Chie Ohsawa, Masaki Matsubara, Hiroko Terasawa, Andres von Schnehen, Daniel Müllensiefen, K. Sekiyama
This study presents a Japanese translation of the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI). The index consists of 38 self-report questions and provides a general sophistication score as well as subscale scores for Active Engagement, Perceptual Abilities, Musical Training, Singing Abilities, and Emotions. The validation of the translation with 689 native Japanese speakers indicated excellent internal consistency and test–retest reliability. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the bifactor model structure formulated by the original study of Gold-MSI is maintained reasonably in our data. The strengths of the Gold-MSI self-report inventory are (1) it offers a multifaceted view of musical sophistication, (2) a subset of five subscales can be used to measure different aspects of musical sophistication independently, and (3) the ease of administration as it is a self-report questionnaire. In view of the fact that this inventory and its translations increasingly contribute to research on musical expertise, skills, and abilities, having a Japanese translation may enhance future research in these areas even further.
本研究提出了金史密斯音乐成熟度指数(Gold-MSI)的日文翻译。该指数由38个自我报告问题组成,提供了一个总体的复杂程度分数,以及积极参与、感知能力、音乐训练、唱歌能力和情绪的子量表分数。对689名日语母语人士的翻译进行了验证,显示出极好的内部一致性和重测信度。验证性因子分析表明,我们的数据合理地保持了原始研究中Gold-MSI所制定的双因子模型结构。Gold-MSI自我报告清单的优势在于(1)它提供了音乐复杂性的多方面观点,(2)五个子量表的子集可用于独立测量音乐复杂性的不同方面,以及(3)易于管理,因为它是一份自我报告问卷。鉴于这一清单及其翻译对音乐专业知识、技能和能力的研究贡献越来越大,日文翻译可能会进一步加强这些领域的未来研究。
{"title":"The Japanese translation of the Gold-MSI: Adaptation and validation of the self-report questionnaire of musical sophistication","authors":"Makiko Sadakata, Yasumasa Yamaguchi, Chie Ohsawa, Masaki Matsubara, Hiroko Terasawa, Andres von Schnehen, Daniel Müllensiefen, K. Sekiyama","doi":"10.1177/10298649221110089","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649221110089","url":null,"abstract":"This study presents a Japanese translation of the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI). The index consists of 38 self-report questions and provides a general sophistication score as well as subscale scores for Active Engagement, Perceptual Abilities, Musical Training, Singing Abilities, and Emotions. The validation of the translation with 689 native Japanese speakers indicated excellent internal consistency and test–retest reliability. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the bifactor model structure formulated by the original study of Gold-MSI is maintained reasonably in our data. The strengths of the Gold-MSI self-report inventory are (1) it offers a multifaceted view of musical sophistication, (2) a subset of five subscales can be used to measure different aspects of musical sophistication independently, and (3) the ease of administration as it is a self-report questionnaire. In view of the fact that this inventory and its translations increasingly contribute to research on musical expertise, skills, and abilities, having a Japanese translation may enhance future research in these areas even further.","PeriodicalId":47219,"journal":{"name":"Musicae Scientiae","volume":"40 1","pages":"798 - 810"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2022-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80307352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Musicae Scientiae
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1