首页 > 最新文献

Medical Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Assessing Decision Fatigue in General Practitioners' Prescribing Decisions Using the Australian BEACH Data Set. 利用澳大利亚 BEACH 数据集评估全科医生处方决策疲劳。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241263823
Mona Maier, Daniel Powell, Christopher Harrison, Julie Gordon, Peter Murchie, Julia L Allan

Background: General practitioners (GPs) make numerous care decisions throughout their workdays. Extended periods of decision making can result in decision fatigue, a gradual shift toward decisions that are less cognitively effortful. This study examines whether observed patterns in GPs' prescribing decisions are consistent with the decision fatigue phenomenon. We hypothesized that the likelihood of prescribing frequently overprescribed medications (antibiotics, benzodiazepines, opioids; less effortful to prescribe) will increase and the likelihood of prescribing frequently underprescribed medications (statins, osteoporosis medications; more effortful to prescribe) will decrease over the workday.

Methods: This study used nationally representative primary care data on GP-patient encounters from the Bettering the Evaluation and Care of Health program from Australia. The association between prescribing decisions and order of patient encounters over a GP's workday was assessed with generalized linear mixed models accounting for clustering and adjusting for patient, provider, and encounter characteristics.

Results: Among 262,456 encounters recorded by 2,909 GPs, the odds of prescribing antibiotics significantly increased by 8.7% with 15 additional patient encounters (odds ratio [OR] = 1.087; confidence interval [CI] = 1.059-1.116). The odds of prescribing decreased significantly with 15 additional patient encounters by 6.3% for benzodiazepines (OR = 0.937; CI = 0.893-0.983), 21.9% for statins (OR = 0.791; CI = 0.753-0.831), and 25.0% for osteoporosis medications (OR = 0.750; CI = 0.690-0.814). No significant effects were observed for opioids. All findings were replicated in confirmatory analyses except the effect of benzodiazepines.

Conclusions: GPs were increasingly likely to prescribe antibiotics and were less likely to prescribe statins and osteoporosis medications as the workday wore on, which was consistent with decision fatigue. There was no convincing evidence of decision fatigue effects in the prescribing of opioids or benzodiazepines. These findings establish decision fatigue as a promising target for optimizing prescribing behavior.

Highlights: We found that as general practitioners progress through their workday, they become more likely to prescribe antibiotics that are reportedly overprescribed and less likely to prescribe statins and osteoporosis medications that are reportedly underprescribed.This change in decision making over time is consistent with the decision fatigue phenomenon. Decision fatigue occurs when we make many decisions without taking a rest break. As we make those decisions, we become gradually more likely to make decisions that are less difficult.The findings of this study show that decision fatigue is a possible target for improving guideline-compliant prescribing of pharmacologic medications.

背景:全科医生(GPs)在整个工作日都要做出许多护理决策。长时间的决策会导致决策疲劳,即逐渐转向认知努力较少的决策。本研究探讨了在全科医生处方决策中观察到的模式是否与决策疲劳现象一致。我们假设,在工作日内,开出经常超量处方药物(抗生素、苯二氮卓类药物、阿片类药物;处方费力程度较低)的可能性会增加,而开出经常处方不足药物(他汀类药物、骨质疏松症药物;处方费力程度较高)的可能性会降低:本研究使用了澳大利亚 "更好的健康评估和护理 "项目中具有全国代表性的全科医生与患者之间的初级保健数据。采用广义线性混合模型评估了处方决定与全科医生工作日接诊患者顺序之间的关联,该模型考虑了聚类因素,并对患者、医疗服务提供者和接诊特征进行了调整:在 2,909 名全科医生记录的 262,456 次就诊中,每增加 15 次就诊,开具抗生素处方的几率就会显著增加 8.7%(几率比 [OR] = 1.087;置信区间 [CI] = 1.059-1.116)。苯二氮卓类药物(OR = 0.937;CI = 0.893-0.983)、他汀类药物(OR = 0.791;CI = 0.753-0.831)和骨质疏松症药物(OR = 0.750;CI = 0.690-0.814)的处方几率分别随着增加 15 次就诊次数而显著降低 6.3%、21.9% 和 25.0%。阿片类药物未观察到明显影响。除了苯二氮卓类药物的影响外,所有结果都在确认性分析中得到了重复:结论:随着工作日的延长,全科医生越来越倾向于开具抗生素处方,而较少开具他汀类药物和骨质疏松症药物处方,这与决策疲劳相符。没有令人信服的证据表明决策疲劳会影响阿片类药物或苯二氮卓类药物的处方。这些发现使决策疲劳成为优化处方行为的一个有希望的目标:我们发现,随着全科医生工作日的进展,他们更有可能开出据报道处方过多的抗生素,而较少开出据报道处方不足的他汀类药物和骨质疏松症药物。当我们在没有休息的情况下做出许多决定时,就会出现决策疲劳。本研究的结果表明,决策疲劳可能是改善符合指南的药物处方的一个目标。
{"title":"Assessing Decision Fatigue in General Practitioners' Prescribing Decisions Using the Australian BEACH Data Set.","authors":"Mona Maier, Daniel Powell, Christopher Harrison, Julie Gordon, Peter Murchie, Julia L Allan","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241263823","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241263823","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>General practitioners (GPs) make numerous care decisions throughout their workdays. Extended periods of decision making can result in decision fatigue, a gradual shift toward decisions that are less cognitively effortful. This study examines whether observed patterns in GPs' prescribing decisions are consistent with the decision fatigue phenomenon. We hypothesized that the likelihood of prescribing frequently overprescribed medications (antibiotics, benzodiazepines, opioids; less effortful to prescribe) will increase and the likelihood of prescribing frequently underprescribed medications (statins, osteoporosis medications; more effortful to prescribe) will decrease over the workday.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>This study used nationally representative primary care data on GP-patient encounters from the Bettering the Evaluation and Care of Health program from Australia. The association between prescribing decisions and order of patient encounters over a GP's workday was assessed with generalized linear mixed models accounting for clustering and adjusting for patient, provider, and encounter characteristics.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Among 262,456 encounters recorded by 2,909 GPs, the odds of prescribing antibiotics significantly increased by 8.7% with 15 additional patient encounters (odds ratio [OR] = 1.087; confidence interval [CI] = 1.059-1.116). The odds of prescribing decreased significantly with 15 additional patient encounters by 6.3% for benzodiazepines (OR = 0.937; CI = 0.893-0.983), 21.9% for statins (OR = 0.791; CI = 0.753-0.831), and 25.0% for osteoporosis medications (OR = 0.750; CI = 0.690-0.814). No significant effects were observed for opioids. All findings were replicated in confirmatory analyses except the effect of benzodiazepines.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>GPs were increasingly likely to prescribe antibiotics and were less likely to prescribe statins and osteoporosis medications as the workday wore on, which was consistent with decision fatigue. There was no convincing evidence of decision fatigue effects in the prescribing of opioids or benzodiazepines. These findings establish decision fatigue as a promising target for optimizing prescribing behavior.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>We found that as general practitioners progress through their workday, they become more likely to prescribe antibiotics that are reportedly overprescribed and less likely to prescribe statins and osteoporosis medications that are reportedly underprescribed.This change in decision making over time is consistent with the decision fatigue phenomenon. Decision fatigue occurs when we make many decisions without taking a rest break. As we make those decisions, we become gradually more likely to make decisions that are less difficult.The findings of this study show that decision fatigue is a possible target for improving guideline-compliant prescribing of pharmacologic medications.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"627-640"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11346129/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Discordant Care and Decision Quality: Patients' Reasons for Not Receiving Their Initial Test of Choice in Colorectal Cancer Screening. 不一致的护理和决策质量:大肠癌筛查中患者未接受首选检测的原因。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241262278
Joshua B Rager, Karen K Schmidt, Peter H Schwartz

Background: Concordance between a person's values and the test or treatment they ultimately receive is widely considered to be an essential outcome for good decision quality. There is little research, however, on why patients receive "discordant" care. A large, randomized trial of decision aids for colorectal cancer (CRC) screening provided an opportunity to assess why some patients received a different test than the one they preferred at an earlier time point.

Methods: Of 688 patients who participated in the trial, 43 received a different CRC screening test than the one they selected after viewing a decision aid 6 mo prior. These patients answered 2 brief, open-ended questions about the reasons for this discordance. The research team analyzed their answers using qualitative description.

Results: Patient responses reflected 6 major categories: barriers or risks of initially favored test, benefits of alternative test, costs or health insurance coverage, discussion with family or friends, provider factors or recommendation, and health issues.

Conclusions: Some of the patients' explanations fit well with the informed concordance approach, which infers poor decision quality from the existence of discordant care, since in these cases it appears that the patient's values and preferences were not adequately respected. Other statements suggest that the patient had an informed rationale for changing their mind about which test to undergo. These cases may reflect high-quality decision making, despite the existence of discordance as measured in the trial. This analysis highlights a major challenge to a popular approach for assessing decision quality, the difficulty of normatively assessing the quality of decision making when apparent discordant care has been provided, and the need to assess patient values and preference over time.

Highlights: Value-choice concordance is an accepted measure for assessing decision quality in decision aid trials, but greater exploration of apparently discordant care challenges key assumptions of this method; this study provides evidence that discordance as typically measured may not always reflect low-quality patient decision making.Researchers evaluating decision aids and assessing decision quality should consider the use of qualitative methods to supplement measures of decision quality and consider assessing patient preferences at multiple time points.

背景:人们普遍认为,一个人的价值观与他们最终接受的检查或治疗之间的一致性是良好决策质量的基本结果。然而,关于患者接受 "不一致 "治疗的原因却鲜有研究。一项针对结直肠癌(CRC)筛查决策辅助工具的大型随机试验为我们提供了一个机会,以评估为什么一些患者在较早的时间点接受了与他们所偏好的不同的检查:在参与试验的 688 名患者中,有 43 名患者在 6 个月前观看了决策辅助工具后,接受了与他们选择的不同的 CRC 筛查试验。这些患者回答了两个简短的开放式问题,说明了出现这种不一致的原因。研究小组采用定性描述的方法对他们的回答进行了分析:患者的回答反映了 6 个主要类别:最初选择的检查的障碍或风险、替代检查的益处、费用或医疗保险范围、与家人或朋友的讨论、医疗服务提供者的因素或建议以及健康问题:患者的一些解释非常符合知情同意方法,该方法从存在不一致的护理推断出决策质量不佳,因为在这些案例中,患者的价值观和偏好似乎没有得到充分尊重。其他陈述则表明,患者在知情的情况下有理由改变主意接受哪种检查。这些病例可能反映了高质量的决策,尽管试验中存在不一致的情况。这项分析凸显了评估决策质量的流行方法所面临的一个重大挑战,即在提供了明显不一致的护理服务时很难对决策质量进行规范性评估,而且需要对患者的价值观和偏好进行长期评估:价值选择一致性是决策辅助工具试验中评估决策质量的一种公认方法,但对明显不一致的护理进行更深入的探讨对该方法的关键假设提出了挑战;本研究提供的证据表明,通常所衡量的不一致可能并不总能反映低质量的患者决策。评估决策辅助工具和决策质量的研究人员应考虑使用定性方法来补充决策质量的衡量标准,并考虑在多个时间点评估患者的偏好。
{"title":"Discordant Care and Decision Quality: Patients' Reasons for Not Receiving Their Initial Test of Choice in Colorectal Cancer Screening.","authors":"Joshua B Rager, Karen K Schmidt, Peter H Schwartz","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241262278","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241262278","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Concordance between a person's values and the test or treatment they ultimately receive is widely considered to be an essential outcome for good decision quality. There is little research, however, on why patients receive \"discordant\" care. A large, randomized trial of decision aids for colorectal cancer (CRC) screening provided an opportunity to assess why some patients received a different test than the one they preferred at an earlier time point.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Of 688 patients who participated in the trial, 43 received a different CRC screening test than the one they selected after viewing a decision aid 6 mo prior. These patients answered 2 brief, open-ended questions about the reasons for this discordance. The research team analyzed their answers using qualitative description.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Patient responses reflected 6 major categories: barriers or risks of initially favored test, benefits of alternative test, costs or health insurance coverage, discussion with family or friends, provider factors or recommendation, and health issues.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Some of the patients' explanations fit well with the informed concordance approach, which infers poor decision quality from the existence of discordant care, since in these cases it appears that the patient's values and preferences were not adequately respected. Other statements suggest that the patient had an informed rationale for changing their mind about which test to undergo. These cases may reflect high-quality decision making, despite the existence of discordance as measured in the trial. This analysis highlights a major challenge to a popular approach for assessing decision quality, the difficulty of normatively assessing the quality of decision making when apparent discordant care has been provided, and the need to assess patient values and preference over time.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>Value-choice concordance is an accepted measure for assessing decision quality in decision aid trials, but greater exploration of apparently discordant care challenges key assumptions of this method; this study provides evidence that discordance as typically measured may not always reflect low-quality patient decision making.Researchers evaluating decision aids and assessing decision quality should consider the use of qualitative methods to supplement measures of decision quality and consider assessing patient preferences at multiple time points.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"705-714"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762107","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Icon Arrays for Medical Risk Communication: Do Icon Type and Color Influence Cardiovascular Risk Perception and Recall? 用于医疗风险交流的图标阵列:图标类型和颜色会影响心血管风险认知和记忆吗?
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241263040
Rebecca Blase, Julia Meis-Harris, Birgitta Weltermann, Simone Dohle

Background: Icon arrays have been shown to be an effective method for communicating medical risk information. However, in practice, icon arrays used to visualize personal risks often differ in the type and color of the icons. The aim of this study was to examine the influence of icon type and color on the perception and recall of cardiovascular risk, as little is known about how color affects the perception of icon arrays.

Methods: A total of 866 participants aged 40 to 90 years representative of the German population in terms of gender and age completed an online experiment. Using a 2 × 2 between-subjects design, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 experimental groups. They received their hypothetical 10-year cardiovascular risk using an icon array that varied by icon type (smiley v. person) and color (black/white v. red/yellow). We measured risk perception, emotional response, intentions of taking action to reduce the risk (e.g., increasing one's physical activity), risk recall, and graph evaluation/trustworthiness, as well as numeracy and graphical literacy.

Results: Icon arrays using person icons were evaluated more positively. There was no effect of icons or color on risk perception, emotional response, intentions of taking action to reduce the risk, or trustworthiness of the graph. While more numerate/graphical literate participants were more likely to correctly recall the presented risk estimate, icon type and color did not influence the probability of correct recall.

Conclusions: Differences in the perception of the tested icon arrays were rather small, suggesting that they may be equally suitable for communicating medical risks. Further research on the robustness of these results across other colors, icons, and risk domains could add to guidelines on the design of visual aids.

Highlights: The use of different icons and colors did not influence the perception and the probability of recalling the 10-year cardiovascular risk, the emotional response, or the intentions to reduce the presented risk.Icon arrays with person icons were evaluated more positively.There was no evidence to suggest that the effectiveness of the studied icon arrays varied based on individuals' levels of numerical or graphical literacy, nor did it differ between people with or without a history of CVD or on medication for an increased CVD risk.

背景:图标阵列已被证明是传达医疗风险信息的有效方法。然而,在实践中,用于直观显示个人风险的图标阵列往往在图标的类型和颜色上有所不同。本研究旨在探讨图标类型和颜色对心血管风险的感知和回忆的影响,因为人们对颜色如何影响图标阵列的感知知之甚少:共有 866 名年龄在 40 岁至 90 岁之间、在性别和年龄方面代表德国人口的参与者完成了在线实验。实验采用 2 × 2 受试者间设计,参与者被随机分配到 4 个实验组中的一个。他们通过不同图标类型(笑脸与人物)和颜色(黑/白与红/黄)的图标阵列接收假设的 10 年心血管风险。我们测量了风险感知、情绪反应、采取行动降低风险的意愿(例如,增加体育锻炼)、风险回忆、图表评价/可信度以及计算能力和识图能力:结果:使用人物图标的图标阵列得到了更积极的评价。图标或颜色对风险认知、情绪反应、采取行动降低风险的意愿或图表的可信度没有影响。虽然更懂数字/图形的参与者更有可能正确回忆起所呈现的风险估计值,但图标类型和颜色并不影响正确回忆的概率:受试者对图标阵列的感知差异很小,这表明这些图标阵列同样适用于传达医疗风险。进一步研究这些结果在其他颜色、图标和风险领域中的稳健性,可以为视觉辅助工具的设计提供指导:使用不同的图标和颜色并不会影响人们对 10 年心血管风险的感知和回忆概率、情绪反应或降低所呈现风险的意愿。
{"title":"Icon Arrays for Medical Risk Communication: Do Icon Type and Color Influence Cardiovascular Risk Perception and Recall?","authors":"Rebecca Blase, Julia Meis-Harris, Birgitta Weltermann, Simone Dohle","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241263040","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241263040","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Icon arrays have been shown to be an effective method for communicating medical risk information. However, in practice, icon arrays used to visualize personal risks often differ in the type and color of the icons. The aim of this study was to examine the influence of icon type and color on the perception and recall of cardiovascular risk, as little is known about how color affects the perception of icon arrays.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>A total of 866 participants aged 40 to 90 years representative of the German population in terms of gender and age completed an online experiment. Using a 2 × 2 between-subjects design, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 experimental groups. They received their hypothetical 10-year cardiovascular risk using an icon array that varied by icon type (smiley v. person) and color (black/white v. red/yellow). We measured risk perception, emotional response, intentions of taking action to reduce the risk (e.g., increasing one's physical activity), risk recall, and graph evaluation/trustworthiness, as well as numeracy and graphical literacy.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Icon arrays using person icons were evaluated more positively. There was no effect of icons or color on risk perception, emotional response, intentions of taking action to reduce the risk, or trustworthiness of the graph. While more numerate/graphical literate participants were more likely to correctly recall the presented risk estimate, icon type and color did not influence the probability of correct recall.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Differences in the perception of the tested icon arrays were rather small, suggesting that they may be equally suitable for communicating medical risks. Further research on the robustness of these results across other colors, icons, and risk domains could add to guidelines on the design of visual aids.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>The use of different icons and colors did not influence the perception and the probability of recalling the 10-year cardiovascular risk, the emotional response, or the intentions to reduce the presented risk.Icon arrays with person icons were evaluated more positively.There was no evidence to suggest that the effectiveness of the studied icon arrays varied based on individuals' levels of numerical or graphical literacy, nor did it differ between people with or without a history of CVD or on medication for an increased CVD risk.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"661-673"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived Penalties for Sharing Patient Beliefs with Health Care Providers. 与医疗服务提供者分享患者信仰的惩罚感。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241262241
Jessecae K Marsh, Onur Asan, Samantha Kleinberg

Background: Health care interactions may require patients to share with a physician information they believe but is incorrect. While a key piece of physicians' work is educating their patients, people's concerns of being seen as uninformed or incompetent by physicians may lead them to think that sharing incorrect health beliefs comes with a penalty. We tested people's perceptions of patients who share incorrect information and how these perceptions vary by the reasonableness of the belief and its centrality to the patient's disease.

Design: We recruited 399 United States Prolific.co workers (357 retained after exclusions), 200 Prolific.co workers who reported having diabetes (139 after exclusions), and 244 primary care physicians (207 after exclusions). Participants read vignettes describing patients with type 2 diabetes sharing health beliefs that were central or peripheral to the management of diabetes. Beliefs included true and incorrect statements that were reasonable or unreasonable to believe. Participants rated how a doctor would perceive the patient, the patient's ability to manage their disease, and the patient's trust in doctors.

Results: Participants rated patients who shared more unreasonable beliefs more negatively. There was an extra penalty for incorrect statements central to the patient's diabetes management (sample 1). These results replicated for participants with type 2 diabetes (sample 2) and physician participants (sample 3).

Conclusions: Participants believed that patients who share incorrect information with their physicians will be penalized for their honesty. Physicians need to be educated on patients' concerns so they can help patients disclose what may be most important for education.

Highlights: Understanding how people think they will be perceived in a health care setting can help us understand what they may be wary to share with their physicians.People think that patients who share incorrect beliefs will be viewed negatively.Helping patients share incorrect beliefs can improve care.

背景:医疗互动可能要求患者与医生分享他们认为不正确的信息。虽然医生的一项重要工作是教育病人,但人们担心被医生视为不知情或不称职,这可能会使他们认为分享不正确的健康观念会受到惩罚。我们测试了人们对分享错误信息的患者的看法,以及这些看法如何因信念的合理性及其对患者疾病的核心作用而有所不同:我们招募了 399 名美国 Prolific.co 工作者(排除后保留 357 人)、200 名报告患有糖尿病的 Prolific.co 工作者(排除后保留 139 人)和 244 名初级保健医生(排除后保留 207 人)。参与者阅读了描述 2 型糖尿病患者分享健康信念的小故事,这些信念是糖尿病管理的核心或外围。这些信念包括合理或不合理的真实和错误陈述。参与者对医生如何看待患者、患者控制疾病的能力以及患者对医生的信任度进行评分:结果:参与者对持有更多不合理信念的患者给予了更负面的评价。与患者糖尿病管理相关的错误陈述会受到额外惩罚(样本 1)。这些结果在 2 型糖尿病参与者(样本 2)和医生参与者(样本 3)身上得到了复制:参与者认为,与医生分享错误信息的患者会因诚实而受到惩罚。医生需要了解患者的顾虑,以便帮助患者披露最重要的教育信息:了解人们认为自己在医疗环境中会被如何看待,有助于我们了解他们在与医生分享信息时可能会有哪些顾虑。
{"title":"Perceived Penalties for Sharing Patient Beliefs with Health Care Providers.","authors":"Jessecae K Marsh, Onur Asan, Samantha Kleinberg","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241262241","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241262241","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Health care interactions may require patients to share with a physician information they believe but is incorrect. While a key piece of physicians' work is educating their patients, people's concerns of being seen as uninformed or incompetent by physicians may lead them to think that sharing incorrect health beliefs comes with a penalty. We tested people's perceptions of patients who share incorrect information and how these perceptions vary by the reasonableness of the belief and its centrality to the patient's disease.</p><p><strong>Design: </strong>We recruited 399 United States Prolific.co workers (357 retained after exclusions), 200 Prolific.co workers who reported having diabetes (139 after exclusions), and 244 primary care physicians (207 after exclusions). Participants read vignettes describing patients with type 2 diabetes sharing health beliefs that were central or peripheral to the management of diabetes. Beliefs included true and incorrect statements that were reasonable or unreasonable to believe. Participants rated how a doctor would perceive the patient, the patient's ability to manage their disease, and the patient's trust in doctors.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants rated patients who shared more unreasonable beliefs more negatively. There was an extra penalty for incorrect statements central to the patient's diabetes management (sample 1). These results replicated for participants with type 2 diabetes (sample 2) and physician participants (sample 3).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Participants believed that patients who share incorrect information with their physicians will be penalized for their honesty. Physicians need to be educated on patients' concerns so they can help patients disclose what may be most important for education.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>Understanding how people think they will be perceived in a health care setting can help us understand what they may be wary to share with their physicians.People think that patients who share incorrect beliefs will be viewed negatively.Helping patients share incorrect beliefs can improve care.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"617-626"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11346123/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141876477","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Making Drug Approval Decisions in the Face of Uncertainty: Cumulative Evidence versus Value of Information. 面对不确定性做出药品审批决定:累积证据与信息价值》。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241255047
Stijntje W Dijk, Eline Krijkamp, Natalia Kunst, Jeremy A Labrecque, Cary P Gross, Aradhana Pandit, Chia-Ping Lu, Loes E Visser, John B Wong, M G Myriam Hunink

Background: The COVID-19 pandemic underscored the criticality and complexity of decision making for novel treatment approval and further research. Our study aims to assess potential decision-making methodologies, an evaluation vital for refining future public health crisis responses.

Methods: We compared 4 decision-making approaches to drug approval and research: the Food and Drug Administration's policy decisions, cumulative meta-analysis, a prospective value-of-information (VOI) approach (using information available at the time of decision), and a reference standard (retrospective VOI analysis using information available in hindsight). Possible decisions were to reject, accept, provide emergency use authorization, or allow access to new therapies only in research settings. We used monoclonal antibodies provided to hospitalized COVID-19 patients as a case study, examining the evidence from September 2020 to December 2021 and focusing on each method's capacity to optimize health outcomes and resource allocation.

Results: Our findings indicate a notable discrepancy between policy decisions and the reference standard retrospective VOI approach with expected losses up to $269 billion USD, suggesting suboptimal resource use during the wait for emergency use authorization. Relying solely on cumulative meta-analysis for decision making results in the largest expected loss, while the policy approach showed a loss up to $16 billion and the prospective VOI approach presented the least loss (up to $2 billion).

Conclusion: Our research suggests that incorporating VOI analysis may be particularly useful for research prioritization and treatment implementation decisions during pandemics. While the prospective VOI approach was favored in this case study, further studies should validate the ideal decision-making method across various contexts. This study's findings not only enhance our understanding of decision-making strategies during a health crisis but also provide a potential framework for future pandemic responses.

Highlights: This study reviews discrepancies between a reference standard (retrospective VOI, using hindsight information) and 3 conceivable real-time approaches to research-treatment decisions during a pandemic, suggesting suboptimal use of resources.Of all prospective decision-making approaches considered, VOI closely mirrored the reference standard, yielding the least expected value loss across our study timeline.This study illustrates the possible benefit of VOI results and the need for evidence accumulation accompanied by modeling in health technology assessment for emerging therapies.

背景:COVID-19 大流行凸显了新疗法审批和进一步研究决策的关键性和复杂性。我们的研究旨在评估潜在的决策方法,这一评估对于完善未来的公共卫生危机应对措施至关重要:我们比较了药物审批和研究的 4 种决策方法:食品药品管理局的政策决定、累积荟萃分析、前瞻性信息价值(VOI)方法(使用决策时可用的信息)和参考标准(使用事后可用的信息进行回顾性 VOI 分析)。可能做出的决定包括拒绝、接受、提供紧急使用授权或仅允许在研究环境中使用新疗法。我们将提供给 COVID-19 住院患者的单克隆抗体作为案例研究,检查了 2020 年 9 月至 2021 年 12 月期间的证据,并重点关注了每种方法优化医疗结果和资源分配的能力:我们的研究结果表明,政策决策与参考标准的回顾性 VOI 方法之间存在明显差异,预计损失高达 2,690 亿美元,这表明在等待紧急用药授权期间,资源的使用未达到最佳状态。仅依靠累积荟萃分析进行决策会导致最大的预期损失,而政策方法显示的损失高达 160 亿美元,前瞻性 VOI 方法的损失最小(最多 20 亿美元):我们的研究表明,在大流行病期间,纳入 VOI 分析可能对确定研究优先次序和治疗实施决策特别有用。虽然在本案例研究中采用前瞻性 VOI 方法更受青睐,但进一步的研究应验证不同情况下的理想决策方法。本研究的发现不仅加深了我们对健康危机期间决策策略的理解,还为未来的大流行病应对措施提供了一个潜在框架:本研究回顾了大流行期间研究治疗决策参考标准(使用事后信息的回顾性 VOI)与 3 种可设想的实时方法之间的差异,表明资源的使用未达到最佳状态。在所考虑的所有前瞻性决策方法中,VOI 密切反映了参考标准,在我们的研究时限内产生的预期价值损失最小。
{"title":"Making Drug Approval Decisions in the Face of Uncertainty: Cumulative Evidence versus Value of Information.","authors":"Stijntje W Dijk, Eline Krijkamp, Natalia Kunst, Jeremy A Labrecque, Cary P Gross, Aradhana Pandit, Chia-Ping Lu, Loes E Visser, John B Wong, M G Myriam Hunink","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241255047","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241255047","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>The COVID-19 pandemic underscored the criticality and complexity of decision making for novel treatment approval and further research. Our study aims to assess potential decision-making methodologies, an evaluation vital for refining future public health crisis responses.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We compared 4 decision-making approaches to drug approval and research: the Food and Drug Administration's policy decisions, cumulative meta-analysis, a prospective value-of-information (VOI) approach (using information available at the time of decision), and a reference standard (retrospective VOI analysis using information available in hindsight). Possible decisions were to reject, accept, provide emergency use authorization, or allow access to new therapies only in research settings. We used monoclonal antibodies provided to hospitalized COVID-19 patients as a case study, examining the evidence from September 2020 to December 2021 and focusing on each method's capacity to optimize health outcomes and resource allocation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Our findings indicate a notable discrepancy between policy decisions and the reference standard retrospective VOI approach with expected losses up to $269 billion USD, suggesting suboptimal resource use during the wait for emergency use authorization. Relying solely on cumulative meta-analysis for decision making results in the largest expected loss, while the policy approach showed a loss up to $16 billion and the prospective VOI approach presented the least loss (up to $2 billion).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Our research suggests that incorporating VOI analysis may be particularly useful for research prioritization and treatment implementation decisions during pandemics. While the prospective VOI approach was favored in this case study, further studies should validate the ideal decision-making method across various contexts. This study's findings not only enhance our understanding of decision-making strategies during a health crisis but also provide a potential framework for future pandemic responses.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>This study reviews discrepancies between a reference standard (retrospective VOI, using hindsight information) and 3 conceivable real-time approaches to research-treatment decisions during a pandemic, suggesting suboptimal use of resources.Of all prospective decision-making approaches considered, VOI closely mirrored the reference standard, yielding the least expected value loss across our study timeline.This study illustrates the possible benefit of VOI results and the need for evidence accumulation accompanied by modeling in health technology assessment for emerging therapies.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"512-528"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11283736/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thinking Fast, Slow, and Forever: Daniel Kahneman Obituary. 快思、慢思、永思:丹尼尔-卡尼曼讣告
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-31 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241256121
Donald A Redelmeier
{"title":"Thinking Fast, Slow, and Forever: Daniel Kahneman Obituary.","authors":"Donald A Redelmeier","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241256121","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X241256121","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":"44 5","pages":"467-469"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stability of Willingness to Pay: Does Time and Treatment Allocation in a Randomized Controlled Trial Influence Willingness to Pay? 支付意愿的稳定性:随机对照试验中的时间和治疗分配会影响支付意愿吗?
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241249654
Marjon van der Pol, Verity Watson, Dwayne Boyers

Background: Willingness-to-pay (WTP) estimates are useful to policy makers only if they are generalizable beyond the moment when they are collected. To understand the "shelf life" of preference estimates, preference stability needs be tested over substantial periods of time.

Methods: We tested the stability of WTP for preventative dental care (scale and polish) using a payment-card contingent valuation question administered to 909 randomized controlled trial participants at 4 time points: baseline (prerandomization) and at annual intervals for 3 years. Trial participants were regular attenders at National Health Service dental practices. Participants were randomly offered different frequencies (intensities) of scale polish (no scale and polish, 1 scale and polish per year, 2 scale and polishes per year). We also examined whether treatment allocation to these different treatment intensities influenced the stability of WTP. Interval regression methods were used to test for changes in WTP over time while controlling for changes in 2 determinants of WTP. Individual-level changes were also examined as well as the WTP function over time.

Results: We found that at the aggregate level, mean WTP values were stable over time. The results were similar by trial arm. Individuals allocated to the arm with the highest scale and polish intensity (2 per year) had a slight increase in WTP toward the latter part of the trial. There was considerable variation at the individual level. The WTP function was stable over time.

Conclusions: The payment-card contingent valuation method can produce stable WTP values in health over time. Future research should explore the generalizability of these results in other populations, for less familiar health care services, and using alternative elicitation methods.

Highlights: Stated preferences are commonly used to value health care.Willingness-to-pay (WTP) estimates are useful only if they have a "shelf life."Little is known about the stability of WTP for health care.We test the stability of WTP for dental care over 3 y.Our results show that the contingent valuation method can produce stable WTP values.

背景:支付意愿(WTP)估算值只有在收集后具有普遍性时,才能对政策制定者有用。为了了解偏好估计值的 "保质期",需要在相当长的一段时间内对偏好稳定性进行测试:我们使用支付卡或然估价问题测试了预防性牙科护理(洗牙和抛光)WTP 的稳定性,该问题在 4 个时间点对 909 名随机对照试验参与者进行了测试:基线(随机前)和 3 年内的年度间隔。试验参与者都是国民健康服务牙科诊所的常客。参与者被随机提供不同频率(强度)的牙垢抛光(不抛光、每年抛光 1 次、每年抛光 2 次)。我们还研究了这些不同治疗强度的治疗分配是否会影响 WTP 的稳定性。我们使用区间回归法检验了 WTP 随时间的变化,同时控制了 WTP 的两个决定因素的变化。我们还检验了个人层面的变化以及随时间变化的 WTP 函数:结果:我们发现,在总体水平上,WTP 的平均值随着时间的推移保持稳定。不同试验组的结果相似。在试验后期,被分配到规模最大、抛光强度最高(每年 2 次)的试验组的个人的 WTP 值略有增加。个人层面的差异很大。随着时间的推移,WTP函数保持稳定:结论:支付卡或然估价法可以产生稳定的健康 WTP 值。未来的研究应探索这些结果在其他人群、不太熟悉的医疗保健服务以及使用其他激发方法时的通用性:我们测试了牙科保健的 WTP 值在 3 年内的稳定性,结果表明或然估价法可以产生稳定的 WTP 值。
{"title":"Stability of Willingness to Pay: Does Time and Treatment Allocation in a Randomized Controlled Trial Influence Willingness to Pay?","authors":"Marjon van der Pol, Verity Watson, Dwayne Boyers","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241249654","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241249654","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Willingness-to-pay (WTP) estimates are useful to policy makers only if they are generalizable beyond the moment when they are collected. To understand the \"shelf life\" of preference estimates, preference stability needs be tested over substantial periods of time.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We tested the stability of WTP for preventative dental care (scale and polish) using a payment-card contingent valuation question administered to 909 randomized controlled trial participants at 4 time points: baseline (prerandomization) and at annual intervals for 3 years. Trial participants were regular attenders at National Health Service dental practices. Participants were randomly offered different frequencies (intensities) of scale polish (no scale and polish, 1 scale and polish per year, 2 scale and polishes per year). We also examined whether treatment allocation to these different treatment intensities influenced the stability of WTP. Interval regression methods were used to test for changes in WTP over time while controlling for changes in 2 determinants of WTP. Individual-level changes were also examined as well as the WTP function over time.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found that at the aggregate level, mean WTP values were stable over time. The results were similar by trial arm. Individuals allocated to the arm with the highest scale and polish intensity (2 per year) had a slight increase in WTP toward the latter part of the trial. There was considerable variation at the individual level. The WTP function was stable over time.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The payment-card contingent valuation method can produce stable WTP values in health over time. Future research should explore the generalizability of these results in other populations, for less familiar health care services, and using alternative elicitation methods.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>Stated preferences are commonly used to value health care.Willingness-to-pay (WTP) estimates are useful only if they have a \"shelf life.\"Little is known about the stability of WTP for health care.We test the stability of WTP for dental care over 3 y.Our results show that the contingent valuation method can produce stable WTP values.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"470-480"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11282685/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140913151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cost-effectiveness Analysis of Colorectal Cancer Screening Strategies Using Active Learning and Monte Carlo Simulation. 利用主动学习和蒙特卡罗模拟对结直肠癌筛查策略进行成本效益分析。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-22 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241258224
Amirhossein Fouladi, Amin Asadi, Eric A Sherer, Mahboubeh Madadi

Introduction: Detection of colorectal cancer (CRC) in the early stages through available screening tests increases the patient's survival chances. Multimodal screening policies can benefit patients by providing more diverse screening options and balancing the risks and benefits of screening tests. We investigate the cost-effectiveness of a wide variety of multimodal CRC screening policies.

Methods: We developed a Monte Carlo simulation framework to model CRC dynamics. We proposed an innovative calibration process using machine learning models to estimate age- and size-specific adenomatous polyps' progression and regression rates. The proposed approach significantly expedites the model parameter space search.

Results: Two multimodal proposed policies (i.e., 1] colonoscopy at 50 y and fecal occult blood test annually between 60 and 75 y and 2] colonoscopy at 50 and 60 y and fecal immunochemical test annually between 70 and 75 y) are identified as efficient frontier policies. Both policies are cost-effective at a willingness to pay of $50,000. Sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the sensitivity of results to a change in screening test costs as well as adherence behavior. The sensitivity analysis results suggest that the proposed policies are mostly robust to the considered changes in screening test costs, as there is a significant overlap between the efficient frontier policies of the baseline and the sensitivity analysis cases. However, the efficient frontier policies were more sensitive to changes in adherence behavior.

Conclusion: Generally, combining stool-based tests with visual tests will benefit patients with higher life expectancy and a lower expected cost compared with unimodal screening policies. Colonoscopy at younger ages (when the colonoscopy complication risk is lower) and stool-based tests at older ages are shown to be more effective.

Highlights: We propose a detailed Markov model to capture the colorectal cancer (CRC) dynamics. The proposed Markov model presents the detailed dynamics of adenomas progression to CRC.We use more than 44,000 colonoscopy reports and available data in the literature to calibrate the proposed Markov model using an innovative approach that leverages machine learning models to expedite the calibration process.We investigate the cost-effectiveness of a wide variety of multimodal CRC screening policies and compare their performances with the current in-practice policies.

导言:通过现有的筛查测试在早期阶段发现结直肠癌(CRC)可增加患者的生存机会。多模式筛查政策可提供更多样化的筛查选择,并平衡筛查测试的风险和益处,从而使患者受益。我们研究了多种多模式 CRC 筛查政策的成本效益:方法:我们开发了一个蒙特卡罗模拟框架来模拟 CRC 动态变化。我们提出了一个创新的校准过程,利用机器学习模型来估算特定年龄和大小的腺瘤息肉的进展率和消退率。该方法大大加快了模型参数空间搜索的速度:结果:两种多模式拟议政策(即 1] 50 岁时进行结肠镜检查,60 至 75 岁时每年进行粪便隐血试验;2] 50 至 60 岁时进行结肠镜检查,70 至 75 岁时每年进行粪便免疫化学试验)被确定为有效的前沿政策。按 50,000 美元的支付意愿计算,这两项政策都具有成本效益。我们进行了敏感性分析,以评估结果对筛查成本和坚持行为变化的敏感性。敏感性分析结果表明,由于基线和敏感性分析案例的有效前沿政策之间存在显著重叠,因此建议的政策对所考虑的筛查测试成本变化大多是稳健的。然而,有效前沿政策对坚持行为的变化更为敏感:一般来说,与单模式筛查政策相比,将粪便检测与肉眼检测相结合将使患者受益,预期寿命更长,预期成本更低。较年轻时进行结肠镜检查(此时结肠镜检查并发症风险较低)和较年长时进行粪便检测更有效:我们提出了一个详细的马尔可夫模型来捕捉结直肠癌(CRC)的动态变化。我们使用超过 44,000 份结肠镜检查报告和文献中的可用数据来校准所提出的马尔可夫模型,并采用一种创新方法,利用机器学习模型来加快校准过程。我们研究了多种多模式 CRC 筛查政策的成本效益,并将其表现与当前的实际政策进行了比较。
{"title":"Cost-effectiveness Analysis of Colorectal Cancer Screening Strategies Using Active Learning and Monte Carlo Simulation.","authors":"Amirhossein Fouladi, Amin Asadi, Eric A Sherer, Mahboubeh Madadi","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241258224","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241258224","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Detection of colorectal cancer (CRC) in the early stages through available screening tests increases the patient's survival chances. Multimodal screening policies can benefit patients by providing more diverse screening options and balancing the risks and benefits of screening tests. We investigate the cost-effectiveness of a wide variety of multimodal CRC screening policies.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We developed a Monte Carlo simulation framework to model CRC dynamics. We proposed an innovative calibration process using machine learning models to estimate age- and size-specific adenomatous polyps' progression and regression rates. The proposed approach significantly expedites the model parameter space search.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Two multimodal proposed policies (i.e., 1] colonoscopy at 50 y and fecal occult blood test annually between 60 and 75 y and 2] colonoscopy at 50 and 60 y and fecal immunochemical test annually between 70 and 75 y) are identified as efficient frontier policies. Both policies are cost-effective at a willingness to pay of $50,000. Sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the sensitivity of results to a change in screening test costs as well as adherence behavior. The sensitivity analysis results suggest that the proposed policies are mostly robust to the considered changes in screening test costs, as there is a significant overlap between the efficient frontier policies of the baseline and the sensitivity analysis cases. However, the efficient frontier policies were more sensitive to changes in adherence behavior.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Generally, combining stool-based tests with visual tests will benefit patients with higher life expectancy and a lower expected cost compared with unimodal screening policies. Colonoscopy at younger ages (when the colonoscopy complication risk is lower) and stool-based tests at older ages are shown to be more effective.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>We propose a detailed Markov model to capture the colorectal cancer (CRC) dynamics. The proposed Markov model presents the detailed dynamics of adenomas progression to CRC.We use more than 44,000 colonoscopy reports and available data in the literature to calibrate the proposed Markov model using an innovative approach that leverages machine learning models to expedite the calibration process.We investigate the cost-effectiveness of a wide variety of multimodal CRC screening policies and compare their performances with the current in-practice policies.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"554-571"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11325561/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141441046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Risk-Adapted Breast Screening for Women at Low Predicted Risk of Breast Cancer: An Online Discrete Choice Experiment. 针对乳腺癌低预测风险妇女的风险适应性乳腺筛查:在线离散选择实验。
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241254828
Charlotte Kelley Jones, Suzanne Scott, Nora Pashayan, Stephen Morris, Yasmina Okan, Jo Waller

Background: A risk-stratified breast screening program could offer low-risk women less screening than is currently offered by the National Health Service. The acceptability of this approach may be enhanced if it corresponds to UK women's screening preferences and values.

Objectives: To elicit and quantify preferences for low-risk screening options.

Methods: Women aged 40 to 70 y with no history of breast cancer took part in an online discrete choice experiment. We generated 32 hypothetical low-risk screening programs defined by 5 attributes (start age, end age, screening interval, risk of dying from breast cancer, and risk of overdiagnosis), the levels of which were systematically varied between the programs. Respondents were presented with 8 choice sets and asked to choose between 2 screening alternatives or no screening. Preference data were analyzed using conditional logit regression models. The relative importance of attributes and the mean predicted probability of choosing each program were estimated.

Results: Participants (N = 502) preferred all screening programs over no screening. An older starting age of screening, younger end age of screening, longer intervals between screening, and increased risk of dying had a negative impact on support for screening programs (P < 0.01). Although the risk of overdiagnosis was of low relative importance, a decreased risk of this harm had a small positive impact on screening choices. The mean predicted probabilities that risk-adapted screening programs would be supported relative to current guidelines were low (range, 0.18 to 0.52).

Conclusions: A deintensified screening pathway for women at low risk of breast cancer, especially one that recommends a later screening start age, would run counter to women's breast screening preferences. Further research is needed to enhance the acceptability of offering less screening to those at low risk of breast cancer.

Highlights: Risk-based breast screening may involve the deintensification of screening for women at low risk of breast cancer.Low-risk screening pathways run counter to women's screening preferences and values.Longer screening intervals may be preferable to a later start age.Work is needed to enhance the acceptability of a low-risk screening pathway.

背景:风险分级的乳腺筛查计划可为低风险妇女提供比国民健康服务目前提供的筛查更少的筛查。如果这种方法符合英国妇女的筛查偏好和价值观,则可提高其可接受性:目的:了解并量化低风险筛查方案的偏好:方法:年龄在 40 岁至 70 岁之间、无乳腺癌病史的女性参加了在线离散选择实验。我们生成了 32 个假设的低风险筛查方案,这些方案由 5 个属性(开始年龄、结束年龄、筛查间隔、死于乳腺癌的风险和过度诊断的风险)定义,这些属性的水平在不同方案之间有系统地变化。向受访者提供了 8 个选择集,要求他们在 2 个筛查方案或不筛查方案中做出选择。偏好数据采用条件对数回归模型进行分析。估算了属性的相对重要性和选择每种方案的平均预测概率:结果:参与者(N = 502)倾向于所有筛查项目而非不筛查。筛查起始年龄越大、筛查结束年龄越小、筛查间隔时间越长以及死亡风险越高,对筛查项目的支持率都有负面影响(P 结论:筛查项目的支持率与筛查的起始年龄、筛查结束年龄、筛查间隔时间以及死亡风险都有负面影响:针对乳腺癌低风险女性的非强化筛查路径,尤其是建议较晚筛查起始年龄的筛查路径,将与女性的乳腺癌筛查偏好背道而驰。需要进一步开展研究,以提高乳腺癌低风险人群对减少筛查的接受度:基于风险的乳腺癌筛查可能涉及到对乳腺癌低风险女性的筛查力度的减弱。低风险筛查路径与女性的筛查偏好和价值观背道而驰。
{"title":"Risk-Adapted Breast Screening for Women at Low Predicted Risk of Breast Cancer: An Online Discrete Choice Experiment.","authors":"Charlotte Kelley Jones, Suzanne Scott, Nora Pashayan, Stephen Morris, Yasmina Okan, Jo Waller","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241254828","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241254828","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>A risk-stratified breast screening program could offer low-risk women less screening than is currently offered by the National Health Service. The acceptability of this approach may be enhanced if it corresponds to UK women's screening preferences and values.</p><p><strong>Objectives: </strong>To elicit and quantify preferences for low-risk screening options.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Women aged 40 to 70 y with no history of breast cancer took part in an online discrete choice experiment. We generated 32 hypothetical low-risk screening programs defined by 5 attributes (start age, end age, screening interval, risk of dying from breast cancer, and risk of overdiagnosis), the levels of which were systematically varied between the programs. Respondents were presented with 8 choice sets and asked to choose between 2 screening alternatives or no screening. Preference data were analyzed using conditional logit regression models. The relative importance of attributes and the mean predicted probability of choosing each program were estimated.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants (<i>N</i> = 502) preferred all screening programs over no screening. An older starting age of screening, younger end age of screening, longer intervals between screening, and increased risk of dying had a negative impact on support for screening programs (<i>P</i> < 0.01). Although the risk of overdiagnosis was of low relative importance, a decreased risk of this harm had a small positive impact on screening choices. The mean predicted probabilities that risk-adapted screening programs would be supported relative to current guidelines were low (range, 0.18 to 0.52).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>A deintensified screening pathway for women at low risk of breast cancer, especially one that recommends a later screening start age, would run counter to women's breast screening preferences. Further research is needed to enhance the acceptability of offering less screening to those at low risk of breast cancer.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>Risk-based breast screening may involve the deintensification of screening for women at low risk of breast cancer.Low-risk screening pathways run counter to women's screening preferences and values.Longer screening intervals may be preferable to a later start age.Work is needed to enhance the acceptability of a low-risk screening pathway.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"586-600"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11283735/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201041","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feedback Loop Failure Modes in Medical Diagnosis: How Biases Can Emerge and Be Reinforced. 医学诊断中的反馈回路失效模式:偏见是如何产生和强化的》(Feedback Loop Failure Modes in Medical Diagnosis: How Biases Can Emerge and Be Reinforced.
IF 3.1 3区 医学 Q2 HEALTH CARE SCIENCES & SERVICES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1177/0272989X241248612
Rachael C Aikens, Jonathan H Chen, Michael Baiocchi, Julia F Simard

Background: Medical diagnosis in practice connects to research through continuous feedback loops: Studies of diagnosed cases shape our understanding of disease, which shapes future diagnostic practice. Without accounting for an imperfect and complex diagnostic process in which some cases are more likely to be diagnosed correctly (or diagnosed at all), the feedback loop can inadvertently exacerbate future diagnostic errors and biases.

Framework: A feedback loop failure occurs if misleading evidence about disease etiology encourages systematic errors that self-perpetuate, compromising future diagnoses and patient care. This article defines scenarios for feedback loop failure in medical diagnosis.

Design: Through simulated cases, we characterize how disease incidence, presentation, and risk factors can be misunderstood when observational data are summarized naive to biases arising from diagnostic error. A fourth simulation extends to a progressive disease.

Results: When severe cases of a disease are diagnosed more readily, less severe cases go undiagnosed, increasingly leading to underestimation of the prevalence and heterogeneity of the disease presentation. Observed differences in incidence and symptoms between demographic groups may be driven by differences in risk, presentation, the diagnostic process itself, or a combination of these. We suggested how perceptions about risk factors and representativeness may drive the likelihood of diagnosis. Differing diagnosis rates between patient groups can feed back to increasingly greater diagnostic errors and disparities in the timing of diagnosis and treatment.

Conclusions: A feedback loop between past data and future medical practice may seem obviously beneficial. However, under plausible scenarios, poorly implemented feedback loops can degrade care. Direct summaries from observational data based on diagnosed individuals may be misleading, especially concerning those symptoms and risk factors that influence the diagnostic process itself.

Highlights: Current evidence about a disease can (and should) influence the diagnostic process. A feedback loop failure may occur if biased "evidence" encourages diagnostic errors, leading to future errors in the evidence base.When diagnostic accuracy varies for mild versus severe cases or between demographic groups, incorrect conclusions about disease prevalence and presentation will result without specifically accounting for such variability.Use of demographic characteristics in the diagnostic process should be done with careful justification, in particular avoiding potential cognitive biases and overcorrection.

背景:实践中的医学诊断通过持续的反馈回路与研究相联系:对已确诊病例的研究会影响我们对疾病的理解,而疾病的理解又会影响未来的诊断实践。在不完善和复杂的诊断过程中,有些病例更有可能被正确诊断(或根本无法诊断),如果不考虑到这一点,反馈回路可能会无意中加剧未来的诊断错误和偏差:如果关于疾病病因学的误导性证据助长了自我延续的系统性错误,损害了未来的诊断和病人护理,就会出现反馈回路失效。本文定义了医疗诊断中反馈环失效的情景:设计:通过模拟病例,我们描述了当观察数据被天真地归纳为诊断错误导致的偏差时,疾病的发病率、表现和风险因素是如何被误解的。第四次模拟扩展到一种进展性疾病:结果:当一种疾病的重症病例更容易被诊断出来时,轻症病例就会被漏诊,从而导致对该疾病的发病率和表现异质性的低估。观察到的不同人口群体之间发病率和症状的差异可能是由风险、表现形式、诊断过程本身的差异或这些因素的组合造成的。我们认为,对风险因素和代表性的认识可能会影响诊断的可能性。患者群体之间的诊断率差异可能会导致诊断误差越来越大,以及诊断和治疗时机的差异:过去的数据与未来的医疗实践之间的反馈回路显然是有益的。结论:过去的数据与未来的医疗实践之间的反馈循环看起来显然是有益的,但在合理的情况下,如果反馈循环执行不力,就会降低医疗水平。根据已确诊个人的观察数据进行直接总结可能会产生误导,尤其是在那些影响诊断过程本身的症状和风险因素方面:亮点:有关疾病的现有证据可以(也应该)影响诊断过程。如果有偏见的 "证据 "助长了诊断错误,可能会导致反馈回路失效,从而导致证据基础在未来出现错误。当轻度病例与重度病例或不同人口群体之间的诊断准确性存在差异时,如果不特别考虑这种差异,就会导致对疾病流行和表现得出不正确的结论。在诊断过程中使用人口特征时应仔细论证,尤其要避免潜在的认知偏差和过度校正。
{"title":"Feedback Loop Failure Modes in Medical Diagnosis: How Biases Can Emerge and Be Reinforced.","authors":"Rachael C Aikens, Jonathan H Chen, Michael Baiocchi, Julia F Simard","doi":"10.1177/0272989X241248612","DOIUrl":"10.1177/0272989X241248612","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Medical diagnosis in practice connects to research through continuous feedback loops: Studies of diagnosed cases shape our understanding of disease, which shapes future diagnostic practice. Without accounting for an imperfect and complex diagnostic process in which some cases are more likely to be diagnosed correctly (or diagnosed at all), the feedback loop can inadvertently exacerbate future diagnostic errors and biases.</p><p><strong>Framework: </strong>A feedback loop failure occurs if misleading evidence about disease etiology encourages systematic errors that self-perpetuate, compromising future diagnoses and patient care. This article defines scenarios for feedback loop failure in medical diagnosis.</p><p><strong>Design: </strong>Through simulated cases, we characterize how disease incidence, presentation, and risk factors can be misunderstood when observational data are summarized naive to biases arising from diagnostic error. A fourth simulation extends to a progressive disease.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>When severe cases of a disease are diagnosed more readily, less severe cases go undiagnosed, increasingly leading to underestimation of the prevalence and heterogeneity of the disease presentation. Observed differences in incidence and symptoms between demographic groups may be driven by differences in risk, presentation, the diagnostic process itself, or a combination of these. We suggested how perceptions about risk factors and representativeness may drive the likelihood of diagnosis. Differing diagnosis rates between patient groups can feed back to increasingly greater diagnostic errors and disparities in the timing of diagnosis and treatment.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>A feedback loop between past data and future medical practice may seem obviously beneficial. However, under plausible scenarios, poorly implemented feedback loops can degrade care. Direct summaries from observational data based on diagnosed individuals may be misleading, especially concerning those symptoms and risk factors that influence the diagnostic process itself.</p><p><strong>Highlights: </strong>Current evidence about a disease can (and should) influence the diagnostic process. A feedback loop failure may occur if biased \"evidence\" encourages diagnostic errors, leading to future errors in the evidence base.When diagnostic accuracy varies for mild versus severe cases or between demographic groups, incorrect conclusions about disease prevalence and presentation will result without specifically accounting for such variability.Use of demographic characteristics in the diagnostic process should be done with careful justification, in particular avoiding potential cognitive biases and overcorrection.</p>","PeriodicalId":49839,"journal":{"name":"Medical Decision Making","volume":" ","pages":"481-496"},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11281873/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140913150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Medical Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1