首页 > 最新文献

Psychophysiology最新文献

英文 中文
PEP reward reactivity moderates the effects of RSA reactivity on antisocial behavior and substance use. PEP奖励反应性调节RSA反应性对反社会行为和物质使用的影响。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14445
Frances R Chen, Katherine French

Integrating Polyvagal Theory and Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST), we examined pre-ejection period (PEP) reward reactivity, which was suggested to index trait impulsivity, as a moderator between respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) reactivity and antisocial behavior (ASB), and substance use in an urban male, adult sample. To understand the inconsistent findings between RSA reactivity and externalizing problems, we proposed to study both negatively and positively valenced tasks for RSA reactivity and to include PEP reward reactivity as a moderator for the RSA-behavior link. Data were collected from an urban sample of 131 male adults (active offenders, demographic controls, and college students). ICG (impedance cardiography) and ECG (electrocardiogram) were recorded, computing PEP (sympathetic nervous system activity marker) and RSA (parasympathetic nervous system activity marker), while participants completed the modified Trier Social Stress Test and a simple reward task. Reactivity was calculated by subtracting the baseline from the task activity. Consistent with prior studies, more RSA withdrawal to stress and less PEP shortening to reward predicted the most ASB and substance use. Less RSA withdrawal to reward and more PEP shortening to reward predicted the most ASB and substance use. We incorporated autonomic space, RST, and Polyvagal Theory to discuss our findings, and specifically highlight how clarifying what each reactivity captures based on the task demand (e.g., presence of social threat, need for vagal-mediated social affiliative behavior) can illuminate our understanding of the result patterns.

结合多元迷走神经理论和强化敏感性理论(RST),我们在城市男性成年样本中检测了射血前期(PEP)奖赏反应性,该反应性被认为是表征冲动性的指标,是呼吸窦性心律失常(RSA)反应性和反社会行为(ASB)以及物质使用之间的调节因素。为了理解RSA反应性和外化问题之间的不一致发现,我们建议研究RSA反应性的负价和正价任务,并将PEP奖励反应性作为RSA行为链接的调节因素。数据是从131名成年男性(活跃罪犯、人口统计学对照和大学生)的城市样本中收集的。记录ICG(阻抗心动图)和ECG(心电图),计算PEP(交感神经系统活动标记)和RSA(副交感神经系统活性标记),同时参与者完成了改良的Trier社会压力测试和一项简单的奖励任务。反应性是通过从任务活动中减去基线来计算的。与先前的研究一致,更多的RSA因压力而退出,更少的PEP因奖励而缩短,预测了最多的ASB和物质使用。更少的RSA提款以奖励,更多的PEP缩短以奖励,预测了最多的ASB和物质使用。我们结合了自主空间、RST和多元迷走神经理论来讨论我们的发现,并特别强调了如何根据任务需求(例如,社会威胁的存在、迷走神经介导的社会附属行为的需要)阐明每种反应捕捉到的内容,以阐明我们对结果模式的理解。
{"title":"PEP reward reactivity moderates the effects of RSA reactivity on antisocial behavior and substance use.","authors":"Frances R Chen, Katherine French","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14445","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14445","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Integrating Polyvagal Theory and Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST), we examined pre-ejection period (PEP) reward reactivity, which was suggested to index trait impulsivity, as a moderator between respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) reactivity and antisocial behavior (ASB), and substance use in an urban male, adult sample. To understand the inconsistent findings between RSA reactivity and externalizing problems, we proposed to study both negatively and positively valenced tasks for RSA reactivity and to include PEP reward reactivity as a moderator for the RSA-behavior link. Data were collected from an urban sample of 131 male adults (active offenders, demographic controls, and college students). ICG (impedance cardiography) and ECG (electrocardiogram) were recorded, computing PEP (sympathetic nervous system activity marker) and RSA (parasympathetic nervous system activity marker), while participants completed the modified Trier Social Stress Test and a simple reward task. Reactivity was calculated by subtracting the baseline from the task activity. Consistent with prior studies, more RSA withdrawal to stress and less PEP shortening to reward predicted the most ASB and substance use. Less RSA withdrawal to reward and more PEP shortening to reward predicted the most ASB and substance use. We incorporated autonomic space, RST, and Polyvagal Theory to discuss our findings, and specifically highlight how clarifying what each reactivity captures based on the task demand (e.g., presence of social threat, need for vagal-mediated social affiliative behavior) can illuminate our understanding of the result patterns.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41125510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An explorative multiverse study for extracting differences in P3 latencies between young and old adults. 一项探索性多元宇宙研究,用于提取年轻人和老年人P3潜伏期的差异。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14459
Kathrin Sadus, Anna-Lena Schubert, Christoph Löffler, Dirk Hagemann

It is well established that P3 latencies increase with age. Investigating these age-related differences requires numerous methodological decisions, resulting in pipelines of great variation. The aim of the present work was to investigate the effects of different analytical pipelines on the age-related differences in P3 latencies in real data. Therefore, we conducted an explorative multiverse study and varied the low-pass filter (4 Hz, 8 Hz, 16 Hz, 32 Hz, and no filter), the latency type (area vs. peak), the level of event-related potential analysis (single participant vs. jackknifing), and the extraction method (manual vs. automated). Thirty young (18-21 years) and 30 old (50-60 years) participants completed three tasks (Nback task, Switching task, Flanker task), while an EEG was recorded. The results show that different analysis strategies can have a tremendous impact on the detection and magnitude of the age effect, with effect sizes ranging from 0% to 88% explained variance. Likewise, regarding the psychometric properties of P3 latencies, we found that the reliabilities fluctuated between rtt  = .20 and 1.00, while the homogeneities ranged from rh  = -.12 to .90. Based on predefined criteria, we found that the most effective pipelines relied on a manual extraction based on a single participant's data. For peak latencies, manual extraction performed well for all filters except for 4 Hz, while for area latencies, filters above 8 Hz produced desirable results. Furthermore, our findings add to the evidence that jackknifing combined with peak latencies can lead to inconclusive results.

众所周知,P3潜伏期随着年龄的增长而增加。调查这些与年龄相关的差异需要大量的方法决策,从而产生巨大的差异。本工作的目的是研究不同分析管道对真实数据中P3潜伏期与年龄相关差异的影响。因此,我们进行了一项探索性的多元宇宙研究,并改变了低通滤波器(4 赫兹,8 赫兹,16 赫兹,32 Hz,无滤波器)、潜伏期类型(面积与峰值)、事件相关电位分析水平(单个参与者与夹刀)和提取方法(手动与自动)。30岁(18-21岁) 岁)和30岁(50-60岁 年)参与者完成了三项任务(Nback任务、Switching任务、Flanker任务),同时记录了脑电图。结果表明,不同的分析策略会对年龄效应的检测和大小产生巨大影响,效应大小在0%到88%之间,可以解释方差。同样,关于P3潜伏期的心理测量特性,我们发现可靠性在rtt之间波动 = .20和1.00,而均匀性范围为rh = -.12至0.90。基于预定义的标准,我们发现最有效的管道依赖于基于单个参与者数据的手动提取。对于峰值延迟,除4个滤波器外,所有滤波器的手动提取都表现良好 Hz,而对于区域延迟,滤波器高于8 Hz产生了理想的结果。此外,我们的研究结果进一步证明,夹刀和峰值潜伏期相结合会导致不确定的结果。
{"title":"An explorative multiverse study for extracting differences in P3 latencies between young and old adults.","authors":"Kathrin Sadus, Anna-Lena Schubert, Christoph Löffler, Dirk Hagemann","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14459","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14459","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well established that P3 latencies increase with age. Investigating these age-related differences requires numerous methodological decisions, resulting in pipelines of great variation. The aim of the present work was to investigate the effects of different analytical pipelines on the age-related differences in P3 latencies in real data. Therefore, we conducted an explorative multiverse study and varied the low-pass filter (4 Hz, 8 Hz, 16 Hz, 32 Hz, and no filter), the latency type (area vs. peak), the level of event-related potential analysis (single participant vs. jackknifing), and the extraction method (manual vs. automated). Thirty young (18-21 years) and 30 old (50-60 years) participants completed three tasks (Nback task, Switching task, Flanker task), while an EEG was recorded. The results show that different analysis strategies can have a tremendous impact on the detection and magnitude of the age effect, with effect sizes ranging from 0% to 88% explained variance. Likewise, regarding the psychometric properties of P3 latencies, we found that the reliabilities fluctuated between r<sub>tt</sub>  = .20 and 1.00, while the homogeneities ranged from r<sub>h</sub>  = -.12 to .90. Based on predefined criteria, we found that the most effective pipelines relied on a manual extraction based on a single participant's data. For peak latencies, manual extraction performed well for all filters except for 4 Hz, while for area latencies, filters above 8 Hz produced desirable results. Furthermore, our findings add to the evidence that jackknifing combined with peak latencies can lead to inconclusive results.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72212494","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Linear integration of multisensory signals in the pupil. 瞳孔中多感官信号的线性积分。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-09 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14453
Wenjie Liu, Yuhui Cheng, Xiangyong Yuan, Yi Jiang

The pupil of the eye responds to various salient signals from different modalities, but there is no consensus on how these pupillary responses are integrated when multiple signals appear simultaneously. Both linear and nonlinear integration have been found previously. The current study aimed to reexamine the nature of pupillary integration, and specifically focused on the early, transient pupillary responses due to its close relationship with orienting. To separate the early pupillary responses out of the pupil time series, we adopted a pupil oscillation paradigm in which sensory stimuli were periodically presented. The simulation analysis confirmed that the amplitude of the pupil oscillation, induced by stimuli repeatedly presented at relatively high rates, can precisely reflect the early, transient pupillary responses without involving the late and sustained pupillary responses. The experimental results then showed that the amplitude of pupil oscillation induced by a series of simultaneous audiovisual stimuli equaled to a linear summation of the oscillatory amplitudes when unisensory stimuli were presented alone. Moreover, the tonic arousal levels, indicated by the baseline pupil size, cannot shift the summation from linear to nonlinear. These findings together support the additive nature of multisensory pupillary integration for the early, orienting-related pupillary responses. The additive nature of pupillary integration further implies that multiple pupillary responses may be independent of each other, irrespective of their potential cognitive and neural drivers.

眼睛的瞳孔对来自不同模态的各种显著信号做出反应,但当多个信号同时出现时,这些瞳孔反应是如何整合的,目前还没有达成共识。线性积分和非线性积分都已被发现。目前的研究旨在重新审视瞳孔整合的本质,特别关注早期短暂的瞳孔反应,因为它与定向有密切关系。为了将早期瞳孔反应从瞳孔时间序列中分离出来,我们采用了瞳孔振荡范式,其中感官刺激是周期性的。模拟分析证实,由以相对较高的速率重复呈现的刺激引起的瞳孔振荡的幅度可以准确地反映早期的短暂瞳孔反应,而不涉及晚期和持续的瞳孔反应。实验结果表明,一系列同时出现的视听刺激引起的瞳孔振荡幅度等于单独出现单一感觉刺激时振荡幅度的线性总和。此外,由基线瞳孔大小指示的紧张性唤醒水平不能将总和从线性转变为非线性。这些发现共同支持了多感觉瞳孔整合对早期定向相关瞳孔反应的相加性质。瞳孔整合的相加性质进一步表明,多种瞳孔反应可能相互独立,而不考虑其潜在的认知和神经驱动因素。
{"title":"Linear integration of multisensory signals in the pupil.","authors":"Wenjie Liu, Yuhui Cheng, Xiangyong Yuan, Yi Jiang","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14453","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14453","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The pupil of the eye responds to various salient signals from different modalities, but there is no consensus on how these pupillary responses are integrated when multiple signals appear simultaneously. Both linear and nonlinear integration have been found previously. The current study aimed to reexamine the nature of pupillary integration, and specifically focused on the early, transient pupillary responses due to its close relationship with orienting. To separate the early pupillary responses out of the pupil time series, we adopted a pupil oscillation paradigm in which sensory stimuli were periodically presented. The simulation analysis confirmed that the amplitude of the pupil oscillation, induced by stimuli repeatedly presented at relatively high rates, can precisely reflect the early, transient pupillary responses without involving the late and sustained pupillary responses. The experimental results then showed that the amplitude of pupil oscillation induced by a series of simultaneous audiovisual stimuli equaled to a linear summation of the oscillatory amplitudes when unisensory stimuli were presented alone. Moreover, the tonic arousal levels, indicated by the baseline pupil size, cannot shift the summation from linear to nonlinear. These findings together support the additive nature of multisensory pupillary integration for the early, orienting-related pupillary responses. The additive nature of pupillary integration further implies that multiple pupillary responses may be independent of each other, irrespective of their potential cognitive and neural drivers.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41184593","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of acute exercise and a nap on heart rate variability and memory in young sedentary adults. 急性运动和小睡对久坐不动的年轻成年人心率变异性和记忆力的影响。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-19 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14454
Melodee Mograss, Emmanuel Frimpong, Franck Vilcourt, Florian Chouchou, Tehila Zvionow, Thien Thanh Dang-Vu

Recent evidence suggests that the autonomic nervous system can contribute to memory consolidation during sleep. Whether fluctuations in cardiac autonomic activity during sleep following physical exercise contribute to the process of memory consolidation has not been studied. We assessed the effects of a non-rapid eye movement (NREM) nap following acute exercise on cardiac autonomic regulation assessed with heart rate variability (HRV) to examine if HRV influences memory processes. Fifty-six (59% female) healthy young adults (23.14 ± 3.74 years) were randomly allocated to either the exercise plus nap (ExNap, n = 27) or nap alone (NoExNap, n = 29) groups. The ExNap group performed a 40-minute moderate-intensity cycling, while the NoExNap group was sedentary prior to learning 45 neutral pictures for a later test. Subsequently, participants underwent a 60-minute NREM nap while measuring EKG, followed by a visual recognition test. Our results indicated that heart rate did not significantly differ between the groups (p = .243), whereas vagally mediated HRV indices were lower in the ExNap group compared to the NoExNap group (p < .05). There were no significant differences in sleep variables between the groups (p > .05). Recognition accuracy was significantly higher in the ExNap group than in the NoExNap group (p = .027). In addition, the recognition accuracy of the ExNap group was negatively associated with vagally mediated HRV (p < .05). Pre-nap acute exercise appears to attenuate parasympathetic activity and to alter the relationship between memory and cardiac autonomic activity.

最近的证据表明,自主神经系统可以在睡眠中帮助巩固记忆。体育锻炼后睡眠中心脏自主神经活动的波动是否有助于记忆巩固过程尚未研究。我们通过心率变异性(HRV)评估了急性运动后非快速眼动(NREM)小睡对心脏自主神经调节的影响,以检查HRV是否影响记忆过程。五十六名(59%为女性)健康的年轻人(23.14 ± 3.74 年)被随机分配到运动加小睡(ExNap = 27)或单独打盹(NoExNap = 29)组。ExNap组进行了40分钟的中等强度自行车运动,而NoExNap小组在为稍后的测试学习45张中性图片之前是久坐的。随后,参与者在测量心电图的同时进行了60分钟的NREM小睡,然后进行了视觉识别测试。我们的研究结果表明,两组之间的心率没有显著差异(p = .243),而ExNap组的迷走神经介导的HRV指数低于NoExNap对照组(p  .05)。ExNap组的识别准确率显著高于NoExNap(p = .027)。此外,ExNap组的识别准确性与迷走神经介导的HRV呈负相关(p
{"title":"The effects of acute exercise and a nap on heart rate variability and memory in young sedentary adults.","authors":"Melodee Mograss, Emmanuel Frimpong, Franck Vilcourt, Florian Chouchou, Tehila Zvionow, Thien Thanh Dang-Vu","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14454","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14454","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent evidence suggests that the autonomic nervous system can contribute to memory consolidation during sleep. Whether fluctuations in cardiac autonomic activity during sleep following physical exercise contribute to the process of memory consolidation has not been studied. We assessed the effects of a non-rapid eye movement (NREM) nap following acute exercise on cardiac autonomic regulation assessed with heart rate variability (HRV) to examine if HRV influences memory processes. Fifty-six (59% female) healthy young adults (23.14 ± 3.74 years) were randomly allocated to either the exercise plus nap (ExNap, n = 27) or nap alone (NoExNap, n = 29) groups. The ExNap group performed a 40-minute moderate-intensity cycling, while the NoExNap group was sedentary prior to learning 45 neutral pictures for a later test. Subsequently, participants underwent a 60-minute NREM nap while measuring EKG, followed by a visual recognition test. Our results indicated that heart rate did not significantly differ between the groups (p = .243), whereas vagally mediated HRV indices were lower in the ExNap group compared to the NoExNap group (p < .05). There were no significant differences in sleep variables between the groups (p > .05). Recognition accuracy was significantly higher in the ExNap group than in the NoExNap group (p = .027). In addition, the recognition accuracy of the ExNap group was negatively associated with vagally mediated HRV (p < .05). Pre-nap acute exercise appears to attenuate parasympathetic activity and to alter the relationship between memory and cardiac autonomic activity.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49686880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct patterns of spatial attentional modulation of steady-state visual evoked magnetic fields (SSVEFs) in subdivisions of the human early visual cortex. 人类早期视觉皮层亚区稳态视觉诱发磁场(SSVEF)空间注意调制的不同模式。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14452
Stephan Moratti, Christopher Gundlach, Javier de Echegaray, Matthias M Müller

In recent years, steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs) became an increasingly valuable tool to investigate neural dynamics of competitive attentional interactions and brain-computer interfaces. This is due to their good signal-to-noise ratio, allowing for single-trial analysis, and their ongoing oscillating nature that enables to analyze temporal dynamics of facilitation and suppression. Given the popularity of SSVEPs, it is surprising that only a few studies looked at the cortical sources of these responses. This is in particular the case when searching for studies that assessed the cortical sources of attentional SSVEP amplitude modulations. To address this issue, we used a typical spatial attention task and recorded neuromagnetic fields (MEG) while presenting frequency-tagged stimuli in the left and right visual fields, respectively. Importantly, we controlled for attentional deployment in a baseline period before the shifting cue. Subjects either attended to a central fixation cross or to two peripheral stimuli simultaneously. Results clearly showed that signal sources and attention effects were restricted to the early visual cortex: V1, V2, hMT+, precuneus, occipital-parietal, and inferior-temporal cortex. When subjects attended to central fixation first, shifting attention to one of the peripheral stimuli resulted in a significant activation increase for the to-be-attended stimulus with no activation decrease for the to-be-ignored stimulus in hMT+ and inferio-temporal cortex, but significant SSVEF decreases from V1 to occipito-parietal cortex. When attention was first deployed to both rings, shifting attention away from one ring basically resulted in a significant activation decrease in all areas for the then-to-be-ignored stimulus.

近年来,稳态视觉诱发电位(SSVEPs)成为研究竞争性注意交互和脑机接口的神经动力学的一种越来越有价值的工具。这是因为它们具有良好的信噪比,可以进行单次试验分析,并且它们具有持续振荡的特性,可以分析促进和抑制的时间动力学。鉴于SSVEP的流行,令人惊讶的是,只有少数研究着眼于这些反应的皮层来源。在寻找评估注意SSVEP振幅调节的皮层来源的研究时,情况尤其如此。为了解决这个问题,我们使用了一个典型的空间注意力任务,并记录了神经磁场(MEG),同时分别在左视野和右视野中呈现频率标记的刺激。重要的是,我们在转换线索之前的基线期控制了注意力的部署。受试者要么接受中央注视交叉,要么同时接受两种外周刺激。结果清楚地表明,信号源和注意效应仅限于早期视觉皮层:V1、V2、hMT+、楔前叶、枕顶叶和颞下皮层。当受试者首先注意中央固定时,将注意力转移到一种外周刺激导致hMT+和颞下皮层中待注意刺激的激活显著增加,而被忽视刺激的激活没有减少,但从V1到枕顶叶皮层的SSVEF显著降低。当注意力首次转移到两个环时,将注意力从一个环转移基本上导致当时被忽视的刺激的所有区域的激活显著降低。
{"title":"Distinct patterns of spatial attentional modulation of steady-state visual evoked magnetic fields (SSVEFs) in subdivisions of the human early visual cortex.","authors":"Stephan Moratti, Christopher Gundlach, Javier de Echegaray, Matthias M Müller","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14452","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14452","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In recent years, steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs) became an increasingly valuable tool to investigate neural dynamics of competitive attentional interactions and brain-computer interfaces. This is due to their good signal-to-noise ratio, allowing for single-trial analysis, and their ongoing oscillating nature that enables to analyze temporal dynamics of facilitation and suppression. Given the popularity of SSVEPs, it is surprising that only a few studies looked at the cortical sources of these responses. This is in particular the case when searching for studies that assessed the cortical sources of attentional SSVEP amplitude modulations. To address this issue, we used a typical spatial attention task and recorded neuromagnetic fields (MEG) while presenting frequency-tagged stimuli in the left and right visual fields, respectively. Importantly, we controlled for attentional deployment in a baseline period before the shifting cue. Subjects either attended to a central fixation cross or to two peripheral stimuli simultaneously. Results clearly showed that signal sources and attention effects were restricted to the early visual cortex: V1, V2, hMT+, precuneus, occipital-parietal, and inferior-temporal cortex. When subjects attended to central fixation first, shifting attention to one of the peripheral stimuli resulted in a significant activation increase for the to-be-attended stimulus with no activation decrease for the to-be-ignored stimulus in hMT+ and inferio-temporal cortex, but significant SSVEF decreases from V1 to occipito-parietal cortex. When attention was first deployed to both rings, shifting attention away from one ring basically resulted in a significant activation decrease in all areas for the then-to-be-ignored stimulus.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41176545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ghrelin is related to lower brain reward activation during touch. Ghrelin与触摸过程中较低的大脑奖励激活有关。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14443
D M Pfabigan, E R Frogner, E Schéle, P M Thorsby, B S Skålhegg, S L Dickson, U Sailer

The gut hormone ghrelin drives food motivation and increases food intake, but it is also involved in the anticipation of and response to rewards other than food. This pre-registered study investigated how naturally varying ghrelin concentrations affect the processing of touch as a social reward in humans. Sixty-seven volunteers received slow caressing touch (so-called CT-targeted touch) as a social reward and control touch on their shins during 3T functional imaging on two test days. On one occasion, participants were fasted, and on another, they received a meal. On each occasion, plasma ghrelin was measured at three time points. All touch was rated as more pleasant after the meal, but there was no association between ghrelin concentrations and pleasantness. CT-targeted touch was rated as the most pleasant and activated somatosensory and reward networks (whole brain). A region-of-interest in the right medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) showed lower activation during all touches, the higher the ghrelin concentrations were. During CT-targeted touch, a larger satiety response (ghrelin decrease after the meal) was associated with higher mOFC activation, and this mOFC activation was associated with higher experienced pleasantness. Overall, higher ghrelin concentrations appear to be related to a lower reward value for touch. Ghrelin may reduce the value of social stimuli, such as touch, to promote food search and intake in a state of low energy. This suggests that the role of ghrelin goes beyond assigning value to food reward.

肠道激素胃饥饿素驱动食物动机并增加食物摄入,但它也参与对食物以外奖励的预期和反应。这项预先注册的研究调查了自然变化的胃饥饿素浓度如何影响人类将触摸作为社会奖励的过程。在两个测试日的3T功能成像期间,67名志愿者接受了缓慢的爱抚触摸(所谓的CT定向触摸)作为社会奖励,并对他们的胫骨进行了控制触摸。有一次,参与者禁食,另一次,他们吃了一顿饭。在每种情况下,在三个时间点测量血浆胃饥饿素。餐后所有的触摸都被评为更愉快,但胃饥饿素浓度与愉悦感之间没有关联。CT定向触摸被评为最令人愉快和最活跃的体感和奖励网络(全脑)。右侧内侧眶额皮质(mOFC)的一个感兴趣区域在所有触摸过程中都表现出较低的激活,胃饥饿素浓度越高。在CT靶向触摸过程中,较大的饱腹感反应(餐后胃饥饿素减少)与较高的mOFC激活有关,而这种mOFC活化与较高的体验愉悦感有关。总的来说,较高的胃饥饿素浓度似乎与较低的触摸奖励值有关。Ghrelin可能会降低社交刺激的价值,如触摸,以促进低能量状态下的食物搜索和摄入。这表明胃饥饿素的作用超出了食物奖励的价值。
{"title":"Ghrelin is related to lower brain reward activation during touch.","authors":"D M Pfabigan, E R Frogner, E Schéle, P M Thorsby, B S Skålhegg, S L Dickson, U Sailer","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14443","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14443","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The gut hormone ghrelin drives food motivation and increases food intake, but it is also involved in the anticipation of and response to rewards other than food. This pre-registered study investigated how naturally varying ghrelin concentrations affect the processing of touch as a social reward in humans. Sixty-seven volunteers received slow caressing touch (so-called CT-targeted touch) as a social reward and control touch on their shins during 3T functional imaging on two test days. On one occasion, participants were fasted, and on another, they received a meal. On each occasion, plasma ghrelin was measured at three time points. All touch was rated as more pleasant after the meal, but there was no association between ghrelin concentrations and pleasantness. CT-targeted touch was rated as the most pleasant and activated somatosensory and reward networks (whole brain). A region-of-interest in the right medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) showed lower activation during all touches, the higher the ghrelin concentrations were. During CT-targeted touch, a larger satiety response (ghrelin decrease after the meal) was associated with higher mOFC activation, and this mOFC activation was associated with higher experienced pleasantness. Overall, higher ghrelin concentrations appear to be related to a lower reward value for touch. Ghrelin may reduce the value of social stimuli, such as touch, to promote food search and intake in a state of low energy. This suggests that the role of ghrelin goes beyond assigning value to food reward.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41168277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reconfiguration of the costly punishment network architecture in punishment decision-making. 惩罚决策中代价高昂的惩罚网络架构的重构。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-08 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14458
Ting Li, Chunliang Feng, Jinhui Wang

Human costly punishment is rooted in multiple regions across large-scale functional systems, a collection of which constitutes the costly punishment network (CPN). Our previous study found that the CPN is intrinsically organized in an optimized and reliable manner to support individual costly punishment propensity. However, it remains unknown how the CPN is reconfigured in response to external cognitive demands in punishment decision-making. Here, we combined resting-state and task-functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the task-related reconfigurations of intrinsic organizations of the CPN when participants made decisions of costly punishment in the Ultimatum Game. Although a strong consistency was observed in the overall pattern and each nodal profile between the intrinsic (task-free) and extrinsic (task-evoked) functional connectivity of the CPN, condition-general and condition-specific reconfigurations were also evident. Specifically, both unfair and fair conditions induced increases in functional connectivity between a few specific pairs of regions, and the unfair condition additionally induced increases in network efficiency of the CPN. Intriguingly, the specific changes in global efficiency of the CPN in the unfair condition were associated with individual differences in costly punishment after adjusting for the corresponding results in the fair condition, which were further identified for females but not for males. These findings were largely reproducible on independent samples. Collectively, our findings provide novel insights into how the CPN adaptively reconfigures its network architecture to support costly punishment.

人类代价高昂的惩罚植根于大规模功能系统的多个区域,这些系统的集合构成了代价高昂的处罚网络(CPN)。我们之前的研究发现,CPN本质上是以优化和可靠的方式组织的,以支持个人代价高昂的惩罚倾向。然而,在惩罚决策中,CPN是如何响应外部认知需求而重新配置的,这仍然是未知的。在这里,我们结合静息状态和任务功能磁共振成像来检查当参与者在终极游戏中做出代价高昂的惩罚决定时,CPN内在组织的任务相关重构。尽管在CPN的内在(无任务)和外在(任务诱发)功能连接之间的总体模式和每个节点轮廓中观察到了很强的一致性,但条件通用和条件特定的重新配置也很明显。具体而言,不公平和公平条件都导致了少数特定区域对之间功能连接的增加,而不公平条件还导致了CPN网络效率的增加。有趣的是,在对公平条件下的相应结果进行调整后,CPN在不公平条件下全球效率的具体变化与代价高昂的惩罚的个体差异有关,这在女性中得到了进一步的确认,但在男性中没有。这些发现在很大程度上可在独立样本上重现。总之,我们的发现为CPN如何自适应地重新配置其网络架构以支持代价高昂的惩罚提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Reconfiguration of the costly punishment network architecture in punishment decision-making.","authors":"Ting Li, Chunliang Feng, Jinhui Wang","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14458","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14458","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human costly punishment is rooted in multiple regions across large-scale functional systems, a collection of which constitutes the costly punishment network (CPN). Our previous study found that the CPN is intrinsically organized in an optimized and reliable manner to support individual costly punishment propensity. However, it remains unknown how the CPN is reconfigured in response to external cognitive demands in punishment decision-making. Here, we combined resting-state and task-functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the task-related reconfigurations of intrinsic organizations of the CPN when participants made decisions of costly punishment in the Ultimatum Game. Although a strong consistency was observed in the overall pattern and each nodal profile between the intrinsic (task-free) and extrinsic (task-evoked) functional connectivity of the CPN, condition-general and condition-specific reconfigurations were also evident. Specifically, both unfair and fair conditions induced increases in functional connectivity between a few specific pairs of regions, and the unfair condition additionally induced increases in network efficiency of the CPN. Intriguingly, the specific changes in global efficiency of the CPN in the unfair condition were associated with individual differences in costly punishment after adjusting for the corresponding results in the fair condition, which were further identified for females but not for males. These findings were largely reproducible on independent samples. Collectively, our findings provide novel insights into how the CPN adaptively reconfigures its network architecture to support costly punishment.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71523994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal unpredictability increases error monitoring as revealed by EEG-EMG investigation. EEG-EMG调查显示,时间的不可预测性增加了错误监测。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-19 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14442
I Korolczuk, B Burle, J T Coull, H Ogińska, M Ociepka, M Senderecka, K Śmigasiewicz

Reacting in an unpredictable context increases error monitoring as evidenced by greater error-related negativity (ERN), an electrophysiological marker linked to an evaluation of response outcomes. We investigated whether ERN also increased when participants evaluated their responses to events that appeared in unpredictable versus predictable moments in time. We complemented electroencephalographic (EEG) analysis of cortical activity by measuring performance monitoring processes at the peripheral level using electromyography (EMG). Specifically, we used EMG data to quantify how temporal unpredictability would affect motor time (MT), the interval between the onset of muscle activity, and the mechanical response. MT increases following errors, indexing online error detection, and an attempt to stop incorrect actions. In our temporally cued version of the stop-signal task, symbolic cues predicted (temporally predictable condition) or not (temporally unpredictable condition) the onset of a target. In 25% of trials, an auditory signal occurred shortly after the target presentation, informing participants that they should inhibit their response completely. Response times were slower, and fewer inhibitory errors were made during temporally unpredictable than predictable trials, indicating enhanced control of unwanted actions when target onset time was unknown. Importantly, the ERN to inhibitory errors was greater in temporally unpredictable relative to temporally predictable conditions. Similarly, EMG data revealed prolonged MT when reactions to temporally unpredictable targets had not been stopped. Taken together, our results show that a temporally unpredictable environment increases the control of unwanted actions, both at cortical and peripheral levels, suggesting a higher subjective cost of maladaptive responses to temporally uncertain events.

在不可预测的环境中做出反应会增加错误监测,更大的错误相关消极性(ERN)证明了这一点,ERN是一种与反应结果评估相关的电生理标志。我们调查了当参与者评估他们对出现在不可预测和可预测时刻的事件的反应时,ERN是否也会增加。我们通过使用肌电图(EMG)测量外周水平的表现监测过程来补充皮层活动的脑电图(EEG)分析。具体来说,我们使用EMG数据来量化时间的不可预测性如何影响运动时间(MT)、肌肉活动开始和机械反应之间的间隔。MT增加了跟踪错误,索引在线错误检测,并尝试停止不正确的操作。在我们的停止信号任务的时间提示版本中,符号提示预测(时间可预测的条件)或不预测(时间不可预测的情况)目标的开始。在25%的试验中,在目标陈述后不久出现听觉信号,告知参与者他们应该完全抑制自己的反应。与可预测的试验相比,在时间上不可预测的实验中,反应时间较慢,产生的抑制性错误更少,这表明在目标发病时间未知的情况下,对不想要的动作的控制得到了加强。重要的是,相对于时间可预测的条件,时间不可预测的ERN对抑制性错误的影响更大。同样,EMG数据显示,当对暂时不可预测的目标的反应没有停止时,MT会延长。总之,我们的研究结果表明,时间上不可预测的环境增加了对皮层和外周不想要的行为的控制,这表明对时间上不确定事件的不适应反应的主观成本更高。
{"title":"Temporal unpredictability increases error monitoring as revealed by EEG-EMG investigation.","authors":"I Korolczuk, B Burle, J T Coull, H Ogińska, M Ociepka, M Senderecka, K Śmigasiewicz","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14442","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reacting in an unpredictable context increases error monitoring as evidenced by greater error-related negativity (ERN), an electrophysiological marker linked to an evaluation of response outcomes. We investigated whether ERN also increased when participants evaluated their responses to events that appeared in unpredictable versus predictable moments in time. We complemented electroencephalographic (EEG) analysis of cortical activity by measuring performance monitoring processes at the peripheral level using electromyography (EMG). Specifically, we used EMG data to quantify how temporal unpredictability would affect motor time (MT), the interval between the onset of muscle activity, and the mechanical response. MT increases following errors, indexing online error detection, and an attempt to stop incorrect actions. In our temporally cued version of the stop-signal task, symbolic cues predicted (temporally predictable condition) or not (temporally unpredictable condition) the onset of a target. In 25% of trials, an auditory signal occurred shortly after the target presentation, informing participants that they should inhibit their response completely. Response times were slower, and fewer inhibitory errors were made during temporally unpredictable than predictable trials, indicating enhanced control of unwanted actions when target onset time was unknown. Importantly, the ERN to inhibitory errors was greater in temporally unpredictable relative to temporally predictable conditions. Similarly, EMG data revealed prolonged MT when reactions to temporally unpredictable targets had not been stopped. Taken together, our results show that a temporally unpredictable environment increases the control of unwanted actions, both at cortical and peripheral levels, suggesting a higher subjective cost of maladaptive responses to temporally uncertain events.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41165636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effort discounts reward-based control allocation: A neurodynamic perspective. 基于努力折扣的控制分配:一个神经动力学的视角。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14451
Ya Zheng, Mang Zhang, Menglin Wu

The amount of cognitive and neural resources allocated to a task is largely determined by the reward we can expect. However, it remains under-appreciated how this reward-expectation-based control allocation is modulated by effort expenditure. The present event-related potential study investigated this issue through the lens of neural dynamics. Thirty-four participants completed an effort-based monetary incentive delay task while their EEG was recorded. Effort demand was manipulated by adding no (low effort) or much (high effort) noise to the target. Behaviorally, participants exhibited reward-related speeding regardless of effort expenditure, as revealed by faster RTs for reward than neutral trials. Our ERP results demonstrated a widespread facilitatory influence of reward expectation on neural dynamics extending from cue evaluation as indexed by the cue-P3, to control preparation as indexed by the contingent negative variation (CNV), and finally to control engagement as indexed by the target-P3. Critically, the neural facilitation was discounted by effort expenditure during both the control-preparation and control-engagement stages instead of the cue-evaluation stage. Overall, this study provides neurodynamic evidence that control allocation is determined by reward and effort via a cost-benefit analysis.

分配给一项任务的认知和神经资源的数量在很大程度上取决于我们所能期望的回报。然而,这种基于回报期望的控制分配是如何通过努力支出来调节的,这一点仍然没有得到充分的认识。目前的事件相关电位研究通过神经动力学的视角来研究这个问题。34名参与者在记录脑电图的同时完成了一项基于努力的金钱激励延迟任务。通过不向目标添加(低努力)或添加大量(高努力)噪声来操纵努力需求。在行为上,参与者表现出与奖励相关的超速,无论努力支出如何,正如奖励的RT比中性试验更快所揭示的那样。我们的ERP结果证明了奖励期望对神经动力学的广泛促进作用,从线索P3索引的线索评估,到偶然负变异(CNV)索引的控制准备,最后到目标P3索引的控制参与。至关重要的是,在控制准备和控制参与阶段,而不是线索评估阶段,神经促进作用都被努力支出所忽视。总的来说,这项研究通过成本效益分析提供了神经动力学证据,证明控制分配是由奖励和努力决定的。
{"title":"Effort discounts reward-based control allocation: A neurodynamic perspective.","authors":"Ya Zheng, Mang Zhang, Menglin Wu","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14451","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14451","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The amount of cognitive and neural resources allocated to a task is largely determined by the reward we can expect. However, it remains under-appreciated how this reward-expectation-based control allocation is modulated by effort expenditure. The present event-related potential study investigated this issue through the lens of neural dynamics. Thirty-four participants completed an effort-based monetary incentive delay task while their EEG was recorded. Effort demand was manipulated by adding no (low effort) or much (high effort) noise to the target. Behaviorally, participants exhibited reward-related speeding regardless of effort expenditure, as revealed by faster RTs for reward than neutral trials. Our ERP results demonstrated a widespread facilitatory influence of reward expectation on neural dynamics extending from cue evaluation as indexed by the cue-P3, to control preparation as indexed by the contingent negative variation (CNV), and finally to control engagement as indexed by the target-P3. Critically, the neural facilitation was discounted by effort expenditure during both the control-preparation and control-engagement stages instead of the cue-evaluation stage. Overall, this study provides neurodynamic evidence that control allocation is determined by reward and effort via a cost-benefit analysis.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41176550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Event-related alpha power in early stage of facial expression processing in social anxiety: Influence of language context. 社交焦虑中面部表情处理早期阶段的事件相关阿尔法力:语言语境的影响。
Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-10 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14455
Sutao Song, Aixin Liu, Zeyuan Gao, Xiaodong Tian, Lingkai Zhu, Haiqing Shang, Shihao Gao, Mingxian Zhang, Shimeng Zhao, Guanlai Xiao, Yuanjie Zheng, Ruiyang Ge

Accurate interpretation of the emotional information conveyed by others' facial expressions is crucial for social interactions. Event-related alpha power, measured by time-frequency analysis, is a frequently used EEG index of emotional information processing. However, it is still unclear how event-related alpha power varies in emotional information processing in social anxiety groups. In the present study, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) while participants from the social anxiety and healthy control groups viewed facial expressions (angry, happy, neutral) preceded by contextual sentences conveying either a positive or negative evaluation of the subject. The impact of context on facial expression processing in both groups of participants was explored by assessing behavioral ratings and event-related alpha power (0-200 ms after expression presentation). In comparison to the healthy control group, the social anxiety group exhibited significantly lower occipital alpha power in response to angry facial expressions in negative contexts and neutral facial expressions in positive contexts. The influence of language context on facial expression processing in individuals with social anxiety may occur at an early stage of processing.

准确解读他人面部表情传达的情感信息对社交互动至关重要。通过时频分析测量的事件相关α功率是情绪信息处理的常用脑电图指标。然而,目前尚不清楚社交焦虑群体在情绪信息处理中与事件相关的阿尔法能力是如何变化的。在本研究中,我们记录了事件相关电位(ERPs),而来自社交焦虑组和健康对照组的参与者在观看面部表情(愤怒、快乐、中性)之前,会用上下文语句表达对受试者的积极或消极评价。通过评估行为评级和事件相关α功率(0-200 表达后ms)。与健康对照组相比,社交焦虑组在消极环境中对愤怒的面部表情和积极环境中对中性面部表情的反应表现出显著较低的枕叶α功率。语言语境对社交焦虑个体面部表情处理的影响可能发生在处理的早期阶段。
{"title":"Event-related alpha power in early stage of facial expression processing in social anxiety: Influence of language context.","authors":"Sutao Song, Aixin Liu, Zeyuan Gao, Xiaodong Tian, Lingkai Zhu, Haiqing Shang, Shihao Gao, Mingxian Zhang, Shimeng Zhao, Guanlai Xiao, Yuanjie Zheng, Ruiyang Ge","doi":"10.1111/psyp.14455","DOIUrl":"10.1111/psyp.14455","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accurate interpretation of the emotional information conveyed by others' facial expressions is crucial for social interactions. Event-related alpha power, measured by time-frequency analysis, is a frequently used EEG index of emotional information processing. However, it is still unclear how event-related alpha power varies in emotional information processing in social anxiety groups. In the present study, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) while participants from the social anxiety and healthy control groups viewed facial expressions (angry, happy, neutral) preceded by contextual sentences conveying either a positive or negative evaluation of the subject. The impact of context on facial expression processing in both groups of participants was explored by assessing behavioral ratings and event-related alpha power (0-200 ms after expression presentation). In comparison to the healthy control group, the social anxiety group exhibited significantly lower occipital alpha power in response to angry facial expressions in negative contexts and neutral facial expressions in positive contexts. The influence of language context on facial expression processing in individuals with social anxiety may occur at an early stage of processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":94182,"journal":{"name":"Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41224376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychophysiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1