Pub Date : 2025-01-21DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2025.2455325
Zahra Khalili, Afshin Mozafarimanesh, Hadi Najafi, Ahmad Vakili-Basir, Maedeh Salehi Sarookollaei, Shahab Papi
Objective: Cognitive dysfunction is a significant issue in old age and can cause many problems in older adults, especially those with diabetes. This study aimed to investigate the association between oral health status and DMFT index with cognitive dysfunction in community-dwelling older adults with T2D (type 2 diabetes).
Methods: This was a cross-sectional study that included 245 older people aged 60 years and older with T2D, visiting healthcare centers in north of Iran, using the cluster sampling method. Data collection tools included clinical-demographic questionnaire, the Geriatric Oral Health Assessment Index (GOHAI) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) questionnaire. A P-value <.05 was considered significant.
Results: The mean age of older adults in this study was 64.86 ± 3.99 years. The mean and standard deviation for the DMFT index, GOHAI index and MMSE scale were 9.39 ± 3.83, 34.29 ± 17.93 and 15.18 ± 10.04 respectively. The results showed statistically significant relationship between poor oral health status and the cognitive status of older adults with T2D (p < .001). Additionally, the relationship between oral health index domains (physical function, psychosocial function and pain) and cognitive status in older adults was significant (p < .001). Another finding indicated an inverse and significant relationship between the DMFT index and cognitive dysfunction in older adults (p < .001).
Conclusion: Poor oral health was associated with cognitive dysfunction among the community-dwelling older adults with T2D. Given the high prevalence of T2D in older adults and the critical importance of oral and dental health for cognition, it is necessary to develop interventions to improve oral health in older adults with T2D.
目的:认知功能障碍是老年人的一个重要问题,它可以导致许多问题,尤其是那些患有糖尿病的老年人。本研究旨在探讨社区居住老年T2D(2型糖尿病)患者口腔健康状况和DMFT指数与认知功能障碍之间的关系。方法:这是一项横断面研究,包括245名60岁及以上T2D老年人,就诊于伊朗北部的医疗中心,采用整群抽样方法。数据收集工具包括临床人口调查问卷、老年口腔健康评估指数(GOHAI)和简易精神状态检查(MMSE)问卷。A p值结果:本组老年人的平均年龄为64.86±3.99岁。DMFT指数、GOHAI指数和MMSE量表的均值和标准差分别为9.39±3.83、34.29±17.93和15.18±10.04。结果显示口腔健康状况不佳与老年T2D患者认知功能存在显著相关性(p p p)。结论:口腔健康状况不佳与社区老年T2D患者认知功能障碍相关。鉴于老年人T2D的高患病率以及口腔和牙齿健康对认知的至关重要,有必要制定干预措施来改善老年T2D患者的口腔健康。
{"title":"Association Between Oral Health Status and DMFT Index with Cognitive Dysfunction in Community-Dwelling Older Adults with Type 2 Diabetes: A Cross-Sectional Study.","authors":"Zahra Khalili, Afshin Mozafarimanesh, Hadi Najafi, Ahmad Vakili-Basir, Maedeh Salehi Sarookollaei, Shahab Papi","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2025.2455325","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2025.2455325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cognitive dysfunction is a significant issue in old age and can cause many problems in older adults, especially those with diabetes. This study aimed to investigate the association between oral health status and DMFT index with cognitive dysfunction in community-dwelling older adults with T2D (type 2 diabetes).</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>This was a cross-sectional study that included 245 older people aged 60 years and older with T2D, visiting healthcare centers in north of Iran, using the cluster sampling method. Data collection tools included clinical-demographic questionnaire, the Geriatric Oral Health Assessment Index (GOHAI) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) questionnaire. A P-value <.05 was considered significant.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The mean age of older adults in this study was 64.86 ± 3.99 years. The mean and standard deviation for the DMFT index, GOHAI index and MMSE scale were 9.39 ± 3.83, 34.29 ± 17.93 and 15.18 ± 10.04 respectively. The results showed statistically significant relationship between poor oral health status and the cognitive status of older adults with T2D (<i>p</i> < .001). Additionally, the relationship between oral health index domains (physical function, psychosocial function and pain) and cognitive status in older adults was significant (<i>p</i> < .001). Another finding indicated an inverse and significant relationship between the DMFT index and cognitive dysfunction in older adults (<i>p</i> < .001).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Poor oral health was associated with cognitive dysfunction among the community-dwelling older adults with T2D. Given the high prevalence of T2D in older adults and the critical importance of oral and dental health for cognition, it is necessary to develop interventions to improve oral health in older adults with T2D.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143002912","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2025-01-01Epub Date: 2023-12-21DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2023.2295202
Tara T Lineweaver, Annie C Wetli, Coty Nicoson, Ashley J Tucker, Christopher Hertzog
This study examined the combined influence of assimilation and contrast effects on the memory self-efficacy and objective memory of young (ages 18-25, n = 114), middle-age (ages 26-59, n = 48), and older (ages 60-98, n = 59) adults. We reminded participants that they matched positive, not negative, memory-relevant or memory-irrelevant stereotypes of aging either before (experimental conditions) or after (control condition) they completed a memory self-efficacy questionnaire and took a memory test. Participants exposed to memory-relevant aging stereotypes prior to other measures reported higher memory self-efficacy than those exposed to memory-irrelevant stereotypes; this effect did not depend on age group. In contrast, the effect of stereotype exposure on memory performance differed with age. Young and older, but not middle-aged, adults showed differences in their memory scores depending on whether they were exposed to memory-relevant, memory-irrelevant or no aging stereotypes. In general, exposure to stereotypes (particularly those relevant to memory) had a negative influence on memory that contrasted with its positive effect on memory self-efficacy. Together, these results indicate that exposure to aging stereotypes can have opposing effects on the memory self-efficacy and objective test performance of adults of various ages and that the relevance of the stereotypes to the cognitive domain being assessed matters.
{"title":"Exposure to Memory-Relevant versus Memory-Irrelevant Aging Stereotypes Differentially Affects Memory Self-Perceptions and Memory Test Scores of Young, Middle, and Older Age Adults.","authors":"Tara T Lineweaver, Annie C Wetli, Coty Nicoson, Ashley J Tucker, Christopher Hertzog","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2295202","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2295202","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined the combined influence of assimilation and contrast effects on the memory self-efficacy and objective memory of young (ages 18-25, <i>n</i> = 114), middle-age (ages 26-59, <i>n</i> = 48), and older (ages 60-98, <i>n</i> = 59) adults. We reminded participants that they matched positive, not negative, memory-relevant or memory-irrelevant stereotypes of aging either before (experimental conditions) or after (control condition) they completed a memory self-efficacy questionnaire and took a memory test. Participants exposed to memory-relevant aging stereotypes prior to other measures reported higher memory self-efficacy than those exposed to memory-irrelevant stereotypes; this effect did not depend on age group. In contrast, the effect of stereotype exposure on memory performance differed with age. Young and older, but not middle-aged, adults showed differences in their memory scores depending on whether they were exposed to memory-relevant, memory-irrelevant or no aging stereotypes. In general, exposure to stereotypes (particularly those relevant to memory) had a negative influence on memory that contrasted with its positive effect on memory self-efficacy. Together, these results indicate that exposure to aging stereotypes can have opposing effects on the memory self-efficacy and objective test performance of adults of various ages and that the relevance of the stereotypes to the cognitive domain being assessed matters.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"73-91"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138829120","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Background: The present study examined age differences in and the relationship between two indices of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression: the degree of emotion regulation and the habitual use of emotion-regulation strategies.
Method: In total, 101 younger and 99 older participants were included in this study. The degree of emotion regulation was measured using an experimental task in which participants were presented with negative or positive pictures and required to regulate their emotions. Habitual use of emotion regulation strategies was measured using an emotion regulation questionnaire.
Results: The results showed that younger adults regulated their emotions to a greater extent than older adults in both reappraisal and suppression. Younger adults were more likely to use reappraisal than were older adults, although there were no age differences in the use of suppression. No significant correlations were found between the degree of emotion regulation and the habitual use of emotion regulation strategies.
Conclusion: These results suggest that age differences in emotion regulation depend on the regulation strategy and that the degree of emotion regulation and habitual use of emotion regulation strategies are independent and quite different indicators in nature.
{"title":"Relationship Between Habitual Use and Degree of Emotion Regulation: Age Differences in Cognitive Reappraisal and Expressive Suppression.","authors":"Kei Oriyama, Kyoko Mukai, Kazuhiro Harada, Kouhei Masumoto","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2315917","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2315917","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>The present study examined age differences in and the relationship between two indices of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression: the degree of emotion regulation and the habitual use of emotion-regulation strategies.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In total, 101 younger and 99 older participants were included in this study. The degree of emotion regulation was measured using an experimental task in which participants were presented with negative or positive pictures and required to regulate their emotions. Habitual use of emotion regulation strategies was measured using an emotion regulation questionnaire.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The results showed that younger adults regulated their emotions to a greater extent than older adults in both reappraisal and suppression. Younger adults were more likely to use reappraisal than were older adults, although there were no age differences in the use of suppression. No significant correlations were found between the degree of emotion regulation and the habitual use of emotion regulation strategies.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results suggest that age differences in emotion regulation depend on the regulation strategy and that the degree of emotion regulation and habitual use of emotion regulation strategies are independent and quite different indicators in nature.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"59-72"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139899606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2025-01-01Epub Date: 2024-02-15DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2024.2313940
Tongpeng Chu, Yajun Liu, Bin Gui, Zhongsheng Zhang, Gang Zhang, Fanghui Dong, Jianli Dong, Shujuan Lin
The aim was to examine the diagnostic efficacy of hippocampal subregions volume and texture in differentiating amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) from normal aging changes. Ninety MCI subjects and eighty-eight well-matched healthy controls (HCs) were selected. Twelve hippocampal subregions volume and texture features were extracted using Freesurfer and MaZda based on T1 weighted MRI. Then, two-sample t-test and Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) regression were developed to select a subset of the original features. Support vector machine (SVM) was used to perform the classification task and the area under the curve (AUC), sensitivity, specificity and accuracy were calculated to evaluate the diagnostic efficacy of the model. The volume features with high discriminative power were mainly located in the bilateral CA1 and CA4, while texture feature were gray-level non-uniformity, run length non-uniformity and fraction. Our model based on hippocampal subregions volume and texture features achieved better classification performance with an AUC of 0.90. The volume and texture of hippocampal subregions can be utilized for the diagnosis of MCI. Moreover, we found that the features that contributed most to the model were mainly textural features, followed by volume. These results may guide future studies using structural scans to classify patients with MCI.
{"title":"Hippocampal Subregions Volume and Texture for the Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment.","authors":"Tongpeng Chu, Yajun Liu, Bin Gui, Zhongsheng Zhang, Gang Zhang, Fanghui Dong, Jianli Dong, Shujuan Lin","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2313940","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2313940","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim was to examine the diagnostic efficacy of hippocampal subregions volume and texture in differentiating amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) from normal aging changes. Ninety MCI subjects and eighty-eight well-matched healthy controls (HCs) were selected. Twelve hippocampal subregions volume and texture features were extracted using Freesurfer and MaZda based on T1 weighted MRI. Then, two-sample t-test and Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) regression were developed to select a subset of the original features. Support vector machine (SVM) was used to perform the classification task and the area under the curve (AUC), sensitivity, specificity and accuracy were calculated to evaluate the diagnostic efficacy of the model. The volume features with high discriminative power were mainly located in the bilateral CA1 and CA4, while texture feature were gray-level non-uniformity, run length non-uniformity and fraction. Our model based on hippocampal subregions volume and texture features achieved better classification performance with an AUC of 0.90. The volume and texture of hippocampal subregions can be utilized for the diagnosis of MCI. Moreover, we found that the features that contributed most to the model were mainly textural features, followed by volume. These results may guide future studies using structural scans to classify patients with MCI.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"125-136"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139734803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2025-01-01Epub Date: 2024-01-26DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2024.2306457
Giverny J Parker, Catherine Haslam, Jaimee Stuart, David H K Shum, Tamara Ownsworth
This study examined the effectiveness of a multiple group membership intervention for reducing the negative effects of age-based stereotype threat (ABST) on older adults' objective memory performance and subjective memory concerns. Healthy older adults (N = 68) were randomly allocated to an ABST + threat-removal (ABST+TR) or ABST + active-control (ABST+AC) condition. After activating ABST, the ABST+TR condition completed a group-listing task and the ABST+AC condition completed a meal-listing task. Participants then completed the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and Everyday Memory Questionnaire - Revised. One significant difference was found in memory performance between conditions; specifically, after controlling for age, gender, and number of items listed, those in the ABST+TR condition performed significantly better on the RAVLT memory interference trial. Further, listing a greater number of group memberships was associated with better memory performance in the ABST+TR condition. No significant difference was found in subjective memory concerns between the ABST+TR condition and the ABST+AC condition. Overall, the current findings indicated that raising the salience of multiple group memberships offered limited protection for older adults' cognitive test performance in the context of ABST.
{"title":"Examining the Utility of a Multiple Group Membership Intervention for Alleviating the Effects of Age-Based Stereotype Threat on Older adults' Memory Performance.","authors":"Giverny J Parker, Catherine Haslam, Jaimee Stuart, David H K Shum, Tamara Ownsworth","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2306457","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2306457","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined the effectiveness of a multiple group membership intervention for reducing the negative effects of age-based stereotype threat (ABST) on older adults' objective memory performance and subjective memory concerns. Healthy older adults (<i>N</i> = 68) were randomly allocated to an ABST + threat-removal (ABST+TR) or ABST + active-control (ABST+AC) condition. After activating ABST, the ABST+TR condition completed a group-listing task and the ABST+AC condition completed a meal-listing task. Participants then completed the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and Everyday Memory Questionnaire - Revised. One significant difference was found in memory performance between conditions; specifically, after controlling for age, gender, and number of items listed, those in the ABST+TR condition performed significantly better on the RAVLT memory interference trial. Further, listing a greater number of group memberships was associated with better memory performance in the ABST+TR condition. No significant difference was found in subjective memory concerns between the ABST+TR condition and the ABST+AC condition. Overall, the current findings indicated that raising the salience of multiple group memberships offered limited protection for older adults' cognitive test performance in the context of ABST.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"103-124"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139566943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2025-01-01Epub Date: 2023-11-27DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286874
Songlan Liang, Xu Han, Shuang Diao, Hui Li
Introduction: This study aimed to evaluate the modifying role of dietary fiber intake in the relationship between COPD and cognitive performance.
Methods: Data of adults aged ≥60 years were extracted from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011-2014. Participants with information on cognitive function measures were included. Dietary fiber intake, identified using participants' 24-h recall surveys, was grouped into high (>25 g/day) and low (≤25 g/day) levels. COPD was identified through self-reported physician diagnoses. Associations between dietary fiber intake, cognitive function and COPD were evaluated using the regression analysis.
Results: Data of 2,189 participants were analyzed. Multivariate analysis revealed that COPD was significantly associated with lowered CERAD (adjusted beta [aBeta]: -0.17, 95% confidence interval [CI]: -0.33 to -0.002, p = .047) and DSST (aBeta: -2.23, 95% CI: -4.25 to -0.2, p = .032) scores in older adults. The analysis on the association between COPD and cognitive function stratified by dietary fiber intake revealed that COPD remained significantly associated with lowered CREAD among individuals with a high fiber intake (aBeta: -0.54, 95% CI: -1.00 to -0.08, p = .024).
Conclusions: In US older adults, COPD is associated with reduced cognitive function. However, the findings do not support that high dietary fiber intake may modify the association between COPD and cognitive impairment.
{"title":"COPD, Dietary Fiber Intake, and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study from NHANES 2011-2014.","authors":"Songlan Liang, Xu Han, Shuang Diao, Hui Li","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286874","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286874","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>This study aimed to evaluate the modifying role of dietary fiber intake in the relationship between COPD and cognitive performance.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Data of adults aged ≥60 years were extracted from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011-2014. Participants with information on cognitive function measures were included. Dietary fiber intake, identified using participants' 24-h recall surveys, was grouped into high (>25 g/day) and low (≤25 g/day) levels. COPD was identified through self-reported physician diagnoses. Associations between dietary fiber intake, cognitive function and COPD were evaluated using the regression analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Data of 2,189 participants were analyzed. Multivariate analysis revealed that COPD was significantly associated with lowered CERAD (adjusted beta [aBeta]: -0.17, 95% confidence interval [CI]: -0.33 to -0.002, <i>p</i> = .047) and DSST (aBeta: -2.23, 95% CI: -4.25 to -0.2, <i>p</i> = .032) scores in older adults. The analysis on the association between COPD and cognitive function stratified by dietary fiber intake revealed that COPD remained significantly associated with lowered CREAD among individuals with a high fiber intake (aBeta: -0.54, 95% CI: -1.00 to -0.08, <i>p</i> = .024).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>In US older adults, COPD is associated with reduced cognitive function. However, the findings do not support that high dietary fiber intake may modify the association between COPD and cognitive impairment.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"92-102"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138444418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2025-01-01Epub Date: 2023-11-22DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286873
Malakeh Z Malak, Anas H Khalifeh
Objective: The current study aimed to examine the relationship between depressive symptoms and quality of life among Jordanian community-dwelling older adults.
Methods: A cross-sectional, descriptive correlational design was used. A convenience sample (N = 602) was selected to recruit the participants in the Amman governorate during the period from August to November 2021.
Results: Findings demonstrated that the mean (SD) age of older adults was 67.5 (7.0) years and 51.5% of participants were females. Also, 54.1% of the participants experienced moderate to severe depressive symptoms with a total mean (SD) score was 8.57 on a scale of 0 to 15, while the mean (SD) for the quality of life scale was 12.12 (3.85) on a scale of 4 to 20. Significant differences existed in quality of life and depressive symptoms based on marital status (p < .001), educational level (p < .001), working status (p < .01), income (p < .001), and chronic disease (p < .01). Quality of life and its domains were negatively associated with depressive symptoms (B= - 0.596, p < .001). Also, marital status, working, educational level, income, and chronic disease were associated with depressive symptoms.
Conclusion: Strategies to improve quality of life should be promoted to minimize depressive symptoms among older adults and consider significant demographic factors.
目的:本研究旨在探讨约旦社区老年人抑郁症状与生活质量之间的关系。方法:采用横断面描述性相关设计。在2021年8月至11月期间,选择了一个方便样本(N = 602)在安曼省招募参与者。结果:研究结果显示,老年人的平均(SD)年龄为67.5(7.0)岁,51.5%的参与者为女性。此外,54.1%的参与者经历中度至重度抑郁症状,在0至15的量表中总平均(SD)得分为8.57,而在4至20的量表中,生活质量量表的平均(SD)得分为12.12(3.85)。婚姻状况对生活质量和抑郁症状的影响存在显著差异(p p p p p p p)结论:提高老年人生活质量的策略应尽量减少抑郁症状,并考虑重要的人口统计学因素。
{"title":"The Relationship Between Quality of Life and Depressive Symptoms Among Jordanian Community-Dwelling Older Adults.","authors":"Malakeh Z Malak, Anas H Khalifeh","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286873","DOIUrl":"10.1080/0361073X.2023.2286873","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The current study aimed to examine the relationship between depressive symptoms and quality of life among Jordanian community-dwelling older adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>A cross-sectional, descriptive correlational design was used. A convenience sample (<i>N</i> = 602) was selected to recruit the participants in the Amman governorate during the period from August to November 2021.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Findings demonstrated that the mean (SD) age of older adults was 67.5 (7.0) years and 51.5% of participants were females. Also, 54.1% of the participants experienced moderate to severe depressive symptoms with a total mean (SD) score was 8.57 on a scale of 0 to 15, while the mean (SD) for the quality of life scale was 12.12 (3.85) on a scale of 4 to 20. Significant differences existed in quality of life and depressive symptoms based on marital status (<i>p</i> < .001), educational level (<i>p</i> < .001), working status (<i>p</i> < .01), income (<i>p</i> < .001), and chronic disease (<i>p</i> < .01). Quality of life and its domains were negatively associated with depressive symptoms (B= - 0.596, <i>p</i> < .001). Also, marital status, working, educational level, income, and chronic disease were associated with depressive symptoms.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Strategies to improve quality of life should be promoted to minimize depressive symptoms among older adults and consider significant demographic factors.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"19-34"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138290693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-12-13DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439743
Takahito Nakamura, Yuki Suda, Takahiro Higuchi
A sudden turn initiated at an unpredictable moment is referred to as reactive turning. This study was designed to seek an age-related decrease in reactive turning when the task demand for turning was increased using a task-switching paradigm. Twenty healthy older adults and 14 younger adults were instructed to walk and execute a 90-degree turn under two conditions. In the "reactive turning condition," participants were asked to turn abruptly when the initially presented travel direction (e.g. right) switched to the opposite direction (e.g. left) while walking. This switch occurred in only one-fifth of 40 trials. In the "pre-planned turning condition," participants were informed of the direction beforehand. The onset of the center of mass (COM) shift in older participants failed to reach significance compared to younger participants. However, correlation analyses showed impaired turning performance in older participants, as those with delayed COM shift exhibited larger pelvic rotations over a short period during reactive turning. This suggests that, owing to increased task demand, older adults had difficulty quickly responding and sufficiently rotating their bodies in the new direction. We conclude that reactive turning with increased task demand, using a task-switching paradigm, is a key factor in detecting age-related performance decline.
{"title":"Reactive Turning Behavior in Older Adults: Age-Related Decrease is Evident under Increased Task Demand.","authors":"Takahito Nakamura, Yuki Suda, Takahiro Higuchi","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439743","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439743","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A sudden turn initiated at an unpredictable moment is referred to as reactive turning. This study was designed to seek an age-related decrease in reactive turning when the task demand for turning was increased using a task-switching paradigm. Twenty healthy older adults and 14 younger adults were instructed to walk and execute a 90-degree turn under two conditions. In the \"reactive turning condition,\" participants were asked to turn abruptly when the initially presented travel direction (e.g. right) switched to the opposite direction (e.g. left) while walking. This switch occurred in only one-fifth of 40 trials. In the \"pre-planned turning condition,\" participants were informed of the direction beforehand. The onset of the center of mass (COM) shift in older participants failed to reach significance compared to younger participants. However, correlation analyses showed impaired turning performance in older participants, as those with delayed COM shift exhibited larger pelvic rotations over a short period during reactive turning. This suggests that, owing to increased task demand, older adults had difficulty quickly responding and sufficiently rotating their bodies in the new direction. We conclude that reactive turning with increased task demand, using a task-switching paradigm, is a key factor in detecting age-related performance decline.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142817688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-12-12DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439741
Marina Gonçalves Leal, José Eduardo Dos Martírios Luz, Yara Lucy Fidelix, Eric Roig-Hierro, Giordano Marcio Gatinho Bonuzzi
Background/study context: Research on older adults has shown impairments in nocturnal sleep, impacting motor memory consolidation and learning. However, previous studies primarily focus on simple tasks, limiting generalization to complex motor activities. Moreover, no evidence exists on how sleep influences adaptability and relearning in older adults.
Methods: Sixty older adults and 60 young adults practiced an underarm dart-throwing task. The participants were divided into 2 sub-groups: SLEEP, which practiced in the evening and was retested on the morning of the following day, and CONTROL, which practiced in the morning and was retested in the evening on the same day. The practice and retention phases were spaced 12 hours. We analyzed motor learning through persistence, adaptability and relearning rate.
Results: Sleep did not enhance motor learning for any group. While young adults exhibited retention, older adults did not, especially after nocturnal sleep. There was no difference between sub-groups in adaptability. Older adults demonstrated inferior relearning compared to young adults, independently of sleep.
Conclusion: Nocturnal sleep did not influence memory consolidation in any group. On the contrary, our findings suggest that nocturnal sleep harms retention in older adults. Age-related characteristics induce a worse relearning rate regardless of sleep occurrence.
{"title":"The Effects of the Nocturnal Sleep on Learning of a Complex Motor Skill in Young and Older Adults.","authors":"Marina Gonçalves Leal, José Eduardo Dos Martírios Luz, Yara Lucy Fidelix, Eric Roig-Hierro, Giordano Marcio Gatinho Bonuzzi","doi":"10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439741","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2024.2439741","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background/study context: </strong>Research on older adults has shown impairments in nocturnal sleep, impacting motor memory consolidation and learning. However, previous studies primarily focus on simple tasks, limiting generalization to complex motor activities. Moreover, no evidence exists on how sleep influences adaptability and relearning in older adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Sixty older adults and 60 young adults practiced an underarm dart-throwing task. The participants were divided into 2 sub-groups: SLEEP, which practiced in the evening and was retested on the morning of the following day, and CONTROL, which practiced in the morning and was retested in the evening on the same day. The practice and retention phases were spaced 12 hours. We analyzed motor learning through persistence, adaptability and relearning rate.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Sleep did not enhance motor learning for any group. While young adults exhibited retention, older adults did not, especially after nocturnal sleep. There was no difference between sub-groups in adaptability. Older adults demonstrated inferior relearning compared to young adults, independently of sleep.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Nocturnal sleep did not influence memory consolidation in any group. On the contrary, our findings suggest that nocturnal sleep harms retention in older adults. Age-related characteristics induce a worse relearning rate regardless of sleep occurrence.</p>","PeriodicalId":12240,"journal":{"name":"Experimental Aging Research","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142812456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}