首页 > 最新文献

Journal of neurophysiology最新文献

英文 中文
The precision of signals encoding active self-movement. 编码主动自我移动信号的精确度。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00370.2023
Joshua D Haynes, Maria Gallagher, John F Culling, Tom C A Freeman

Everyday actions like moving the head, walking around, and grasping objects are typically self-controlled. This presents a problem when studying the signals encoding such actions because active self-movement is difficult to control experimentally. Available techniques demand repeatable trials, but each action is unique, making it difficult to measure fundamental properties like psychophysical thresholds. We present a novel paradigm that recovers both precision and bias of self-movement signals with minimal constraint on the participant. The paradigm relies on linking image motion to previous self-movement, and two experimental phases to extract the signal encoding the latter. The paradigm takes care of a hidden source of external noise not previously accounted for in techniques that link display motion to self-movement in real time (e.g., virtual reality). We use head rotations as an example of self-movement, and show that the precision of the signals encoding head movement depends on whether they are being used to judge visual motion or auditory motion. We find that perceived motion is slowed during head movement in both cases. The "nonimage" signals encoding active head rotation (motor commands, proprioception, and vestibular cues) are therefore biased toward lower speeds and/or displacements. In a second experiment, we trained participants to rotate their heads at different rates and found that the imprecision of the head rotation signal rises proportionally with head speed (Weber's law). We discuss the findings in terms of the different motion cues used by vision and hearing, and the implications they have for Bayesian models of motion perception.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We present a psychophysical technique for measuring the precision of signals encoding active self-movements. Using head movements, we show that 1) precision is greater when active head rotation is performed using visual comparison stimuli versus auditory; 2) precision decreases with head speed (Weber's law); 3) perceived speed is lower during head rotation. The findings may reflect the steps needed to convert different cues into common units, and challenge standard Bayesian models of motion perception.

头部移动、走动和抓握物体等日常动作通常都是自我控制的。这给研究这类动作的编码信号带来了难题,因为主动的自我运动很难在实验中控制。现有技术要求可重复试验,但每个动作都是独一无二的,因此很难测量心理物理阈值等基本属性。我们提出了一种新颖的范式,它能以对参与者最小的限制来恢复自我动作信号的精确性和偏差。该范式依赖于将图像运动与之前的自我运动联系起来,并通过两个实验阶段来提取编码后者的信号。该范例解决了以前将显示运动与自我运动实时联系起来的技术(如虚拟现实)中没有考虑到的隐藏的外部噪声源问题。我们将头部旋转作为自我运动的一个例子,并证明头部运动编码信号的精确度取决于它们是用于判断视觉运动还是听觉运动。我们发现,在这两种情况下,头部运动时感知运动的速度都会减慢。因此,编码头部主动旋转的 "非图像 "信号(运动指令、本体感觉和前庭提示)偏向于较低的速度和/或位移。在第二个实验中,我们训练参与者以不同的速度旋转头部,结果发现头部旋转信号的不精确度与头部速度成正比上升(韦伯定律)。我们将从视觉和听觉使用的不同运动线索以及它们对运动感知的贝叶斯模型的影响角度来讨论这些发现。
{"title":"The precision of signals encoding active self-movement.","authors":"Joshua D Haynes, Maria Gallagher, John F Culling, Tom C A Freeman","doi":"10.1152/jn.00370.2023","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00370.2023","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Everyday actions like moving the head, walking around, and grasping objects are typically self-controlled. This presents a problem when studying the signals encoding such actions because active self-movement is difficult to control experimentally. Available techniques demand repeatable trials, but each action is unique, making it difficult to measure fundamental properties like psychophysical thresholds. We present a novel paradigm that recovers both precision and bias of self-movement signals with minimal constraint on the participant. The paradigm relies on linking image motion to previous self-movement, and two experimental phases to extract the signal encoding the latter. The paradigm takes care of a hidden source of external noise not previously accounted for in techniques that link display motion to self-movement in real time (e.g., virtual reality). We use head rotations as an example of self-movement, and show that the precision of the signals encoding head movement depends on whether they are being used to judge visual motion or auditory motion. We find that perceived motion is slowed during head movement in both cases. The \"nonimage\" signals encoding active head rotation (motor commands, proprioception, and vestibular cues) are therefore biased toward lower speeds and/or displacements. In a second experiment, we trained participants to rotate their heads at different rates and found that the imprecision of the head rotation signal rises proportionally with head speed (Weber's law). We discuss the findings in terms of the different motion cues used by vision and hearing, and the implications they have for Bayesian models of motion perception.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> We present a psychophysical technique for measuring the precision of signals encoding active self-movements. Using head movements, we show that <i>1</i>) precision is greater when active head rotation is performed using visual comparison stimuli versus auditory; <i>2</i>) precision decreases with head speed (Weber's law); <i>3</i>) perceived speed is lower during head rotation. The findings may reflect the steps needed to convert different cues into common units, and challenge standard Bayesian models of motion perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141306103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The complexity of fentanyl's impact on brain and peripheral oxygenation. 芬太尼对大脑和外周血氧的复杂影响
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00256.2024
Nicholas J Burgraff
{"title":"The complexity of fentanyl's impact on brain and peripheral oxygenation.","authors":"Nicholas J Burgraff","doi":"10.1152/jn.00256.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00256.2024","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensorimotor prediction is used to direct gaze toward task-relevant locations in a goal-directed throwing task. 在目标引导的投掷任务中,感知运动预测被用来引导目光投向任务相关的位置。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00052.2024
Theresa K Brand, Alexander C Schütz, Hermann Müller, Heiko Maurer, Mathias Hegele, Lisa K Maurer

Previous research has shown that action effects of self-generated movements are internally predicted before outcome feedback becomes available. To test whether these sensorimotor predictions are used to facilitate visual information uptake for feedback processing, we measured eye movements during the execution of a goal-directed throwing task. Participants could fully observe the effects of their throwing actions (ball trajectory and either hitting or missing a target) in most of the trials. In a portion of the trials, the ball trajectory was not visible, and participants only received static information about the outcome. We observed a large proportion of predictive saccades, shifting gaze toward the goal region before the ball arrived and outcome feedback became available. Fixation locations after predictive saccades systematically covaried with future ball positions in trials with continuous ball flight information, but notably also in trials with static outcome feedback and only efferent and proprioceptive information about the movement that could be used for predictions. Fixation durations at the chosen positions after feedback onset were modulated by action outcome (longer durations for misses than for hits) and outcome uncertainty (longer durations for narrow vs. clear outcomes). Combining both effects, durations were longest for narrow errors and shortest for clear hits, indicating that the chosen locations offer informational value for feedback processing. Thus, humans are able to use sensorimotor predictions to direct their gaze toward task-relevant feedback locations. Outcome-dependent saccade latency differences (miss vs. hit) indicate that also predictive valuation processes are involved in planning predictive saccades.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We elucidate the potential benefits of sensorimotor predictions, focusing on how the system actually uses this information to optimize feedback processing in goal-directed actions. Sensorimotor information is used to predict spatial parameters of movement outcomes, guiding predictive saccades toward future action effects. Saccade latencies and fixation durations are modulated by outcome quality, indicating that predictive valuation processes are considered and that the locations chosen are of high informational value for feedback processing.

先前的研究表明,在获得结果反馈之前,自我产生的动作效果已在内部得到预测。为了测试这些感觉运动预测是否用于促进视觉信息摄取以进行反馈处理,我们测量了在执行目标引导的投掷任务过程中的眼球运动。在大多数试验中,参与者都能完全观察到自己投掷动作的效果(球的轨迹、击中目标或未击中目标)。在部分试验中,看不到球的轨迹,参与者只能接收到有关结果的静态信息。我们观察到很大比例的预测性眼动,即在球到达和结果反馈可用之前将视线移向目标区域。在有连续球飞行信息的试验中,预测性囊回后的定点位置系统地与未来球的位置相关,但在有静态结果反馈且只有运动的传出信息和本体感觉信息可用于预测的试验中,这种情况也很明显。反馈开始后,在所选位置的定影持续时间受动作结果(失误的持续时间长于命中的持续时间)和结果不确定性(结果狭窄的持续时间长于结果清晰的持续时间)的调节。综合这两种效应,误差范围小的持续时间最长,误差范围大的持续时间最短,这表明所选位置为反馈处理提供了信息价值。因此,人类能够利用感觉运动预测将视线引向与任务相关的反馈位置。与结果相关的囊状移动延迟差异(失误与命中)表明,在规划预测性囊状移动时也涉及到预测估值过程。
{"title":"Sensorimotor prediction is used to direct gaze toward task-relevant locations in a goal-directed throwing task.","authors":"Theresa K Brand, Alexander C Schütz, Hermann Müller, Heiko Maurer, Mathias Hegele, Lisa K Maurer","doi":"10.1152/jn.00052.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00052.2024","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has shown that action effects of self-generated movements are internally predicted before outcome feedback becomes available. To test whether these sensorimotor predictions are used to facilitate visual information uptake for feedback processing, we measured eye movements during the execution of a goal-directed throwing task. Participants could fully observe the effects of their throwing actions (ball trajectory and either hitting or missing a target) in most of the trials. In a portion of the trials, the ball trajectory was not visible, and participants only received static information about the outcome. We observed a large proportion of predictive saccades, shifting gaze toward the goal region before the ball arrived and outcome feedback became available. Fixation locations after predictive saccades systematically covaried with future ball positions in trials with continuous ball flight information, but notably also in trials with static outcome feedback and only efferent and proprioceptive information about the movement that could be used for predictions. Fixation durations at the chosen positions after feedback onset were modulated by action outcome (longer durations for misses than for hits) and outcome uncertainty (longer durations for narrow vs. clear outcomes). Combining both effects, durations were longest for narrow errors and shortest for clear hits, indicating that the chosen locations offer informational value for feedback processing. Thus, humans are able to use sensorimotor predictions to direct their gaze toward task-relevant feedback locations. Outcome-dependent saccade latency differences (miss vs. hit) indicate that also predictive valuation processes are involved in planning predictive saccades.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> We elucidate the potential benefits of sensorimotor predictions, focusing on how the system actually uses this information to optimize feedback processing in goal-directed actions. Sensorimotor information is used to predict spatial parameters of movement outcomes, guiding predictive saccades toward future action effects. Saccade latencies and fixation durations are modulated by outcome quality, indicating that predictive valuation processes are considered and that the locations chosen are of high informational value for feedback processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141450730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not so early! Revisiting the question of visual pathway selectivity of saccadic suppression. 没那么早重新审视眼动抑制的视觉通路选择性问题
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00254.2024
Ziad M Hafed, Saad Idrees, Matthias P Baumann
{"title":"Not so early! Revisiting the question of visual pathway selectivity of saccadic suppression.","authors":"Ziad M Hafed, Saad Idrees, Matthias P Baumann","doi":"10.1152/jn.00254.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00254.2024","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141626994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between frontal alpha asymmetry and behavioral and brain activity indices of reactive inhibitory control. 额叶阿尔法不对称与反应性抑制控制的行为和大脑活动指数之间的关系。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-12 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00046.2024
Atakan M Akil, Renáta Cserjési, Tamás Nagy, Zsolt Demetrovics, Dezső Németh, H N Alexander Logemann

Reactive inhibitory control plays an important role in phenotype of different diseases/different phases of a disease. One candidate electrophysiological marker of inhibitory control is frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA). FAA reflects the relative difference in contralateral frontal brain activity. However, the relationship between FAA and potential behavioral/brain activity indices of reactive inhibitory control is not yet clear. We assessed the relationship between resting-state FAA and indicators of reactive inhibitory control. Additionally, we investigated the effect of modulation of FAA via transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). We implemented a randomized sham-controlled design with 65 healthy humans (Mage = 23.93, SDage = 6.08; 46 female). Before and after 2-mA anodal tDCS of the right frontal site (with the cathode at the contralateral site) for 20 min, we collected EEG data and reactive inhibitory performance in neutral and food-reward conditions, using the stop signal task (SST). There was no support for the effect of tDCS on FAA or any indices of reactive inhibitory control. Our correlation analysis revealed an association between inhibitory brain activity in the food-reward condition and (pre-tDCS) asymmetry. Higher right relative to left frontal brain activity was correlated with reduced early-onset inhibitory activity and, in contrast, linked with higher late-onset inhibitory control in the food-reward condition. Similarly, event-related potential analyses showed reduced early-onset and enhanced late-onset inhibitory brain activity over time, particularly in the food-reward condition. These results suggest that there can be a dissociation regarding the lateralization of frontal brain activity and early- and late-onset inhibitory brain activity.NEW & NOTEWORTHY This research reveals dissociation between baseline frontal alpha asymmetry and the timing of reactive inhibitory brain activities in food-reward contexts. Whereas inhibitory control performance decreases over time in a stop signal task, electrophysiological indices show reduced early- and heightened late-onset inhibitory brain activity, especially in the reward condition. Additionally, greater right frontal activity correlates with reduced early-onset and increased late-onset inhibitory brain activity.

反应性抑制控制在不同疾病/疾病不同阶段的表型中起着重要作用。抑制控制的一个候选电生理标记是额叶α不对称(FAA)。额叶α不对称反映了对侧额叶大脑活动的相对差异。然而,FAA 与反应性抑制控制的潜在行为/脑活动指标之间的关系尚不清楚。我们评估了静息态 FAA 与反应性抑制控制指标之间的关系。此外,我们还研究了通过经颅直流电刺激(tDCS)调节 FAA 的效果。我们对 65 名健康人(平均年龄 = 23.93 岁;平均年龄 = 6.08 岁;46 名女性)进行了随机假对照设计。在对右侧额叶部位(阴极位于对侧部位)进行 2 mA 阳极 tDCS 刺激 20 分钟前后,我们收集了脑电图数据以及在中性和食物奖赏条件下使用停止信号任务(SST)的反应性抑制表现。tDCS 对 FAA 或任何反应性抑制控制指标的影响均未得到支持。我们的相关分析表明,食物奖赏条件下的大脑抑制活动与(tDCS 前)不对称之间存在关联。相对于左侧额叶,右侧额叶的大脑活动较高,这与早期抑制活动减少有关,相反,这与食物奖赏条件下晚期抑制控制较高有关。同样,事件相关电位分析表明,随着时间的推移,大脑早期抑制活动减少,晚期抑制活动增强,尤其是在食物奖赏条件下。这些结果表明,额叶大脑活动的侧向性与早期和晚期抑制性大脑活动可能存在差异。
{"title":"The relationship between frontal alpha asymmetry and behavioral and brain activity indices of reactive inhibitory control.","authors":"Atakan M Akil, Renáta Cserjési, Tamás Nagy, Zsolt Demetrovics, Dezső Németh, H N Alexander Logemann","doi":"10.1152/jn.00046.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00046.2024","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reactive inhibitory control plays an important role in phenotype of different diseases/different phases of a disease. One candidate electrophysiological marker of inhibitory control is frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA). FAA reflects the relative difference in contralateral frontal brain activity. However, the relationship between FAA and potential behavioral/brain activity indices of reactive inhibitory control is not yet clear. We assessed the relationship between resting-state FAA and indicators of reactive inhibitory control. Additionally, we investigated the effect of modulation of FAA via transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). We implemented a randomized sham-controlled design with 65 healthy humans (M<sub>age</sub> = 23.93, SD<sub>age</sub> = 6.08; 46 female). Before and after 2-mA anodal tDCS of the right frontal site (with the cathode at the contralateral site) for 20 min, we collected EEG data and reactive inhibitory performance in neutral and food-reward conditions, using the stop signal task (SST). There was no support for the effect of tDCS on FAA or any indices of reactive inhibitory control. Our correlation analysis revealed an association between inhibitory brain activity in the food-reward condition and (pre-tDCS) asymmetry. Higher right relative to left frontal brain activity was correlated with reduced early-onset inhibitory activity and, in contrast, linked with higher late-onset inhibitory control in the food-reward condition. Similarly, event-related potential analyses showed reduced early-onset and enhanced late-onset inhibitory brain activity over time, particularly in the food-reward condition. These results suggest that there can be a dissociation regarding the lateralization of frontal brain activity and early- and late-onset inhibitory brain activity.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> This research reveals dissociation between baseline frontal alpha asymmetry and the timing of reactive inhibitory brain activities in food-reward contexts. Whereas inhibitory control performance decreases over time in a stop signal task, electrophysiological indices show reduced early- and heightened late-onset inhibitory brain activity, especially in the reward condition. Additionally, greater right frontal activity correlates with reduced early-onset and increased late-onset inhibitory brain activity.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11302602/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141306128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beadex, the Drosophila LIM only protein, is required for the growth of the larval neuromuscular junction. 果蝇 LIM 唯一蛋白 Beadex 是幼虫神经肌肉接头生长所必需的。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00064.2024
Kripa Chitre, Subhash Kairamkonda, Manish Kumar Dwivedi, Saumitra Yadav, Vimlesh Kumar, Sujit K Sikdar, Upendra Nongthomba

The appropriate growth of the neurons, accurate organization of their synapses, and successful neurotransmission are indispensable for sensorimotor activities. These processes are highly dynamic and tightly regulated. Extensive genetic, molecular, physiological, and behavioral studies have identified many molecular candidates and investigated their roles in various neuromuscular processes. In this article, we show that Beadex (Bx), the Drosophila LIM only (LMO) protein, is required for motor activities and neuromuscular growth of Drosophila. The larvae bearing Bx7, a null allele of Bx, and the RNAi-mediated neuronal-specific knockdown of Bx show drastically reduced crawling behavior, a diminished synaptic span of the neuromuscular junctions (NMJs) and an increased spontaneous neuronal firing with altered motor patterns in the central pattern generators (CPGs). Microarray studies identified multiple targets of Beadex that are involved in different cellular and molecular pathways, including those associated with the cytoskeleton and mitochondria that could be responsible for the observed neuromuscular defects. With genetic interaction studies, we further show that Highwire (Hiw), a negative regulator of synaptic growth at the NMJs, negatively regulates Bx, as the latter's deficiency was able to rescue the phenotype of the Hiw null mutant, HiwDN. Thus, our data indicate that Beadex functions downstream of Hiw to regulate the larval synaptic growth and physiology.NEW & NOTEWORTHY A novel role for Beadex (Bx) regulates the larval neuromuscular junction (NMJ) structure and function in a tissue-specific manner. Bx is expressed in a subset of Toll-6-expressing neurons and is involved in regulating synaptic span and physiology, possibly through its negative interaction with Highwire (Hiw). The findings of this study provide insights into the molecular mechanisms underlying NMJ development and function and warrant further investigation to understand the role of Bx in these processes fully.

神经元的适当生长、神经突触的准确组织以及成功的神经传递是感觉运动活动不可或缺的条件。这些过程是高度动态和严格调控的。广泛的遗传学、分子学、生理学和行为学研究发现了许多候选分子,并研究了它们在各种神经肌肉过程中的作用。在本文中,我们发现果蝇仅有的 LIM 蛋白(LMO)Beadex(Bx)是果蝇运动活动和神经肌肉生长所必需的。携带 Bx7(Bx 的无效等位基因)和 RNAi- 介导的神经元特异性敲除 Bx 的幼虫表现出爬行行为急剧减少、神经肌肉接头(NMJ)的突触跨度减小、自发神经元发射增加以及中央模式发生器(CPG)的运动模式改变。微阵列研究确定了比德士的多个靶点,这些靶点涉及不同的细胞和分子通路,包括与细胞骨架和线粒体相关的通路,可能是造成所观察到的神经肌肉缺陷的原因。通过基因相互作用研究,我们进一步发现,NMJs突触生长的负调控因子Highwire(Hiw)对Bx有负向调控作用,因为缺乏Bx能挽救Hiw无效突变体HiwDN的表型。因此,我们的数据表明,Beadex 在 Hiw 的下游发挥着调节幼虫突触生长和生理的功能。
{"title":"Beadex, the <i>Drosophila</i> LIM only protein, is required for the growth of the larval neuromuscular junction.","authors":"Kripa Chitre, Subhash Kairamkonda, Manish Kumar Dwivedi, Saumitra Yadav, Vimlesh Kumar, Sujit K Sikdar, Upendra Nongthomba","doi":"10.1152/jn.00064.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00064.2024","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The appropriate growth of the neurons, accurate organization of their synapses, and successful neurotransmission are indispensable for sensorimotor activities. These processes are highly dynamic and tightly regulated. Extensive genetic, molecular, physiological, and behavioral studies have identified many molecular candidates and investigated their roles in various neuromuscular processes. In this article, we show that Beadex (Bx), the <i>Drosophila</i> LIM only (LMO) protein, is required for motor activities and neuromuscular growth of <i>Drosophila</i>. The larvae bearing <i>Bx</i><sup>7</sup>, a null allele of <i>Bx</i>, and the RNAi-mediated neuronal-specific knockdown of <i>Bx</i> show drastically reduced crawling behavior, a diminished synaptic span of the neuromuscular junctions (NMJs) and an increased spontaneous neuronal firing with altered motor patterns in the central pattern generators (CPGs). Microarray studies identified multiple targets of Beadex that are involved in different cellular and molecular pathways, including those associated with the cytoskeleton and mitochondria that could be responsible for the observed neuromuscular defects. With genetic interaction studies, we further show that <i>Highwire</i> (<i>Hiw</i>), a negative regulator of synaptic growth at the NMJs, negatively regulates <i>Bx</i>, as the latter's deficiency was able to rescue the phenotype of the <i>Hiw</i> null mutant, <i>Hiw<sup>DN</sup></i>. Thus, our data indicate that Beadex functions downstream of Hiw to regulate the larval synaptic growth and physiology.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> A novel role for Beadex (Bx) regulates the larval neuromuscular junction (NMJ) structure and function in a tissue-specific manner. Bx is expressed in a subset of Toll-6-expressing neurons and is involved in regulating synaptic span and physiology, possibly through its negative interaction with Highwire (Hiw). The findings of this study provide insights into the molecular mechanisms underlying NMJ development and function and warrant further investigation to understand the role of Bx in these processes fully.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141262135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hue and orientation pinwheels in macaque area V4. 猕猴 V4 区域的色调和方位针盘。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00366.2023
Arun Parajuli, Daniel J Felleman

Area V4 is an intermediate-level area of the macaque visual cortical hierarchy that serves key functions in visual processing by integrating inputs from lower areas such as V1 and V2 and providing feedforward inputs to many higher cortical areas. Previous V4 imaging studies have focused on differential responses to color, orientation, disparity, and motion stimuli, but many details of the spatial organization of significant hue and orientation tuning have not been fully described. We used support vector machine (SVM) decoding of intrinsic cortical single-condition responses to generate high-resolution maps of hue and orientation tuning and to describe the organization of hue and orientation pinwheels in V4. Like V1 and V2, V4 contains maps of orientation that are organized as pinwheels. V4 also contains maps of hue that are organized as pinwheels, whose circular organization more closely represents the perception of hue than is observed in antecedent cortical areas. Unlike V1, where orientation is continuously mapped across the surface, V4 hue and orientation pinwheels are organized in limited numbers of pinwheel sequences. The organization of these sequences and the distance between pinwheels may provide insight into the functional organization of V4. Regions significantly tuned for hue occupy roughly four times that of the orientation, are largely separated from each other, and overlap by roughly 5%. This spatial organization is largely consistent with segregated inputs arising from V2 thin and interstripes. This modular organization of V4 suggests that further integration of color and shape might occur in higher areas in inferotemporal cortical.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The representation of hue and orientation in macaque monkey area V4 was determined by intrinsic cortical imaging of responses to isoluminant hues and achromatic grating stimuli. Vector summation of support vector machine (SVM) decoded single-condition responses was used to generate hue and orientation maps that, like V1 orientation maps, were both characterized by distinct pinwheel patterns. These data suggest that pinwheels are an important structure to represent different stimulus features across multiple visual cortical areas.

V4区是猕猴视觉皮层层次结构的中间级区域,在皮层视觉处理过程中发挥着关键作用,它整合了来自V1和V2等低级区域多个功能区的前馈输入,并为颞下叶、顶叶和前额叶皮层的许多区域提供前馈输入。虽然以前的许多 V4 成像研究分析了对颜色、方位、色差和运动刺激的不同反应,但对重要色调和方位调谐的空间组织的许多细节还没有充分描述。支持向量机(SVM)对皮层单条件反应的解码被用来生成 V4 中色调和方位调谐的高分辨率地图。与 V1 和 V2 类似,V4 也包含围绕针轮中心组织的等方位域地图。V4 包含的色调图由围绕风车中心的等色调域组成。这些风车的环形组织比在先行皮层区域观察到的更接近色调感知。对色调有明显调节作用的区域所占面积大约是方位区域的四倍,这些区域在很大程度上相互隔离,重叠率大约为 5%。色调和方位风车及其区域的空间组织与来自 V2 细区和纹间区的基本分离的输入基本一致。这种模块化的分离处理表明,颜色和形状的进一步整合必须发生在接受来自 V4 直接投射的颞下皮层区域。
{"title":"Hue and orientation pinwheels in macaque area V4.","authors":"Arun Parajuli, Daniel J Felleman","doi":"10.1152/jn.00366.2023","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00366.2023","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Area V4 is an intermediate-level area of the macaque visual cortical hierarchy that serves key functions in visual processing by integrating inputs from lower areas such as V1 and V2 and providing feedforward inputs to many higher cortical areas. Previous V4 imaging studies have focused on differential responses to color, orientation, disparity, and motion stimuli, but many details of the spatial organization of significant hue and orientation tuning have not been fully described. We used support vector machine (SVM) decoding of intrinsic cortical single-condition responses to generate high-resolution maps of hue and orientation tuning and to describe the organization of hue and orientation pinwheels in V4. Like V1 and V2, V4 contains maps of orientation that are organized as pinwheels. V4 also contains maps of hue that are organized as pinwheels, whose circular organization more closely represents the perception of hue than is observed in antecedent cortical areas. Unlike V1, where orientation is continuously mapped across the surface, V4 hue and orientation pinwheels are organized in limited numbers of pinwheel sequences. The organization of these sequences and the distance between pinwheels may provide insight into the functional organization of V4. Regions significantly tuned for hue occupy roughly four times that of the orientation, are largely separated from each other, and overlap by roughly 5%. This spatial organization is largely consistent with segregated inputs arising from V2 thin and interstripes. This modular organization of V4 suggests that further integration of color and shape might occur in higher areas in inferotemporal cortical.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> The representation of hue and orientation in macaque monkey area V4 was determined by intrinsic cortical imaging of responses to isoluminant hues and achromatic grating stimuli. Vector summation of support vector machine (SVM) decoded single-condition responses was used to generate hue and orientation maps that, like V1 orientation maps, were both characterized by distinct pinwheel patterns. These data suggest that pinwheels are an important structure to represent different stimulus features across multiple visual cortical areas.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427060/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141579964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dopamine enhances GABAA receptor-mediated current amplitude in a subset of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. 多巴胺能增强一部分固有光敏视网膜神经节细胞中 GABAA 受体介导的电流振幅。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00457.2023
Nikolas Bergum, Casey-Tyler Berezin, Jozsef Vigh

Neuromodulation in the retina is crucial for effective processing of retinal signal at different levels of illuminance. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), the neurons that drive nonimage-forming visual functions, express a variety of neuromodulatory receptors that tune intrinsic excitability as well as synaptic inputs. Past research has examined actions of neuromodulators on light responsiveness of ipRGCs, but less is known about how neuromodulation affects synaptic currents in ipRGCs. To better understand how neuromodulators affect synaptic processing in ipRGC, we examine actions of opioid and dopamine agonists have on inhibitory synaptic currents in ipRGCs. Although µ-opioid receptor (MOR) activation had no effect on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) currents, dopamine [via the D1-type dopamine receptor (D1R)]) amplified GABAergic currents in a subset of ipRGCs. Furthermore, this D1R-mediated facilitation of the GABA conductance in ipRGCs was mediated by a cAMP/PKA-dependent mechanism. Taken together, these findings reinforce the idea that dopamine's modulatory role in retinal adaptation affects both nonimage-forming and image-forming visual functions.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Neuromodulators such as dopamine are important regulators of retinal function. Here, we demonstrate that dopamine increases inhibitory inputs to intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), in addition to its previously established effect on intrinsic light responsiveness. This indicates that dopamine, in addition to its ability to intrinsically modulate ipRGC activity, can also affect synaptic inputs to ipRGCs, thereby tuning retina circuits involved in nonimage-forming visual functions.

视网膜中的神经调节对于在不同照度下有效处理视网膜信号至关重要。本征光敏视网膜神经节细胞(ipRGCs)是驱动非图像形成视觉功能的神经元,它们表达多种神经调节受体,可调节本征兴奋性和突触输入。过去的研究已经考察了神经调节剂对 ipRGCs 光反应性的作用,但对神经调节如何影响 ipRGCs 中的突触电流却知之甚少。为了更好地了解神经调节剂如何影响ipRGC的突触处理,我们研究了阿片类和多巴胺激动剂对ipRGC抑制性突触电流的作用。虽然μ-阿片受体(MOR)激活对γ-氨基丁酸(GABA)电流没有影响,但多巴胺(通过D1R)却放大了一部分ipRGCs的GABA能电流。此外,这种由 D1R 介导的对 ipRGCs 中 GABA 传导的促进作用是由 cAMP/PKA 依赖性机制介导的。总之,这些发现强化了多巴胺在视网膜适应中的调节作用既影响非图像形成,也影响图像形成的视觉功能这一观点。
{"title":"Dopamine enhances GABA<sub>A</sub> receptor-mediated current amplitude in a subset of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells.","authors":"Nikolas Bergum, Casey-Tyler Berezin, Jozsef Vigh","doi":"10.1152/jn.00457.2023","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00457.2023","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Neuromodulation in the retina is crucial for effective processing of retinal signal at different levels of illuminance. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), the neurons that drive nonimage-forming visual functions, express a variety of neuromodulatory receptors that tune intrinsic excitability as well as synaptic inputs. Past research has examined actions of neuromodulators on light responsiveness of ipRGCs, but less is known about how neuromodulation affects synaptic currents in ipRGCs. To better understand how neuromodulators affect synaptic processing in ipRGC, we examine actions of opioid and dopamine agonists have on inhibitory synaptic currents in ipRGCs. Although µ-opioid receptor (MOR) activation had no effect on γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) currents, dopamine [via the D1-type dopamine receptor (D1R)]) amplified GABAergic currents in a subset of ipRGCs. Furthermore, this D1R-mediated facilitation of the GABA conductance in ipRGCs was mediated by a cAMP/PKA-dependent mechanism. Taken together, these findings reinforce the idea that dopamine's modulatory role in retinal adaptation affects both nonimage-forming and image-forming visual functions.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> Neuromodulators such as dopamine are important regulators of retinal function. Here, we demonstrate that dopamine increases inhibitory inputs to intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), in addition to its previously established effect on intrinsic light responsiveness. This indicates that dopamine, in addition to its ability to intrinsically modulate ipRGC activity, can also affect synaptic inputs to ipRGCs, thereby tuning retina circuits involved in nonimage-forming visual functions.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427049/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141492335","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Initial and corrective submovement encoding differences within primary motor cortex during precision reaching. 精确伸手过程中初级运动皮层内的初始和修正子运动编码差异
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00269.2023
Kevin C Schwartze, Wei-Hsien Lee, Adam G Rouse

Precision reaching often requires corrective submovements to obtain the desired goal. Most studies of reaching have focused on single initial movements, and implied the cortical encoding model was the same for all submovements. However, corrective submovements may show different encoding patterns from the initial submovement with distinct patterns of activation across the population. Two rhesus macaques performed a precision center-out-task with small targets. Neural activity from single units in the primary motor cortex and associated behavioral data were recorded to evaluate movement characteristics. Neural population data and individual neuronal firing rates identified with a peak finding algorithm to identify peaks in hand speed were examined for encoding differences between initial and corrective submovements. Individual neurons were fitted with a regression model that included the reach vector, position, and speed to predict firing rate. For both initial and corrective submovements, the largest effect remained movement direction. We observed a large subset changed their preferred direction greater than 45° between initial and corrective submovements. Neuronal depth of modulation also showed considerable variation when adjusted for movement speed. By using principal component analysis, neural trajectories of initial and corrective submovements progressed through different neural subspaces. These findings all suggest that different neural encoding patterns exist for initial and corrective submovements within the cortex. We hypothesize that this variation in how neurons change to encode small, corrective submovements might allow for a larger portion of the neural space being used to encode a greater range of movements with varying amplitudes and levels of precision.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Neuronal recordings matched with kinematic behavior were collected in a precision center-out task that often required corrective movements. We reveal large differences in preferred direction and depth of modulation between initial and corrective submovements across the neural population. We then present a model of the neural population describing how these shifts in tuning create different subspaces for signaling initial and corrective movements likely to improve motor precision.

精确的伸手动作往往需要纠正性子动作才能达到预期目标。大多数关于伸手的研究都集中在单一的初始动作上,并暗示所有子动作的大脑皮层编码模式都是相同的。然而,纠正性子动作可能会显示出与初始子动作不同的编码模式,并在整个群体中显示出不同的激活模式。两只猕猴对小目标进行了精确的 "中心点移出 "任务。通过记录初级运动皮层单个单元的神经活动和相关行为数据来评估运动特征。神经群数据和单个神经元发射率通过峰值查找算法识别手速峰值,并检查初始动作和纠正动作之间的编码差异。单个神经元用一个回归模型进行拟合,该模型包括伸手矢量、位置和速度,以预测发射率。对于初始动作和纠正动作,最大的影响仍然是动作方向。我们观察到,在初始动作和纠正动作之间,很大一部分神经元的首选方向变化超过了 45⁰。神经元的调制深度在根据运动速度进行调整后也显示出相当大的差异。通过利用主成分分析,初始动作和纠正动作的神经轨迹在不同的神经子空间中发生了变化。这些发现都表明,在大脑皮层中,初始动作和纠正动作存在不同的神经编码模式。我们假设,神经元在编码小的纠正性子动作时所发生的这种变化,可能会使更大一部分神经空间用于编码幅度和精确度各不相同的更大范围的动作。
{"title":"Initial and corrective submovement encoding differences within primary motor cortex during precision reaching.","authors":"Kevin C Schwartze, Wei-Hsien Lee, Adam G Rouse","doi":"10.1152/jn.00269.2023","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00269.2023","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Precision reaching often requires corrective submovements to obtain the desired goal. Most studies of reaching have focused on single initial movements, and implied the cortical encoding model was the same for all submovements. However, corrective submovements may show different encoding patterns from the initial submovement with distinct patterns of activation across the population. Two rhesus macaques performed a precision center-out-task with small targets. Neural activity from single units in the primary motor cortex and associated behavioral data were recorded to evaluate movement characteristics. Neural population data and individual neuronal firing rates identified with a peak finding algorithm to identify peaks in hand speed were examined for encoding differences between initial and corrective submovements. Individual neurons were fitted with a regression model that included the reach vector, position, and speed to predict firing rate. For both initial and corrective submovements, the largest effect remained movement direction. We observed a large subset changed their preferred direction greater than 45° between initial and corrective submovements. Neuronal depth of modulation also showed considerable variation when adjusted for movement speed. By using principal component analysis, neural trajectories of initial and corrective submovements progressed through different neural subspaces. These findings all suggest that different neural encoding patterns exist for initial and corrective submovements within the cortex. We hypothesize that this variation in how neurons change to encode small, corrective submovements might allow for a larger portion of the neural space being used to encode a greater range of movements with varying amplitudes and levels of precision.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> Neuronal recordings matched with kinematic behavior were collected in a precision center-out task that often required corrective movements. We reveal large differences in preferred direction and depth of modulation between initial and corrective submovements across the neural population. We then present a model of the neural population describing how these shifts in tuning create different subspaces for signaling initial and corrective movements likely to improve motor precision.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427045/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141580008","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Biomechanical costs influence decisions made during ongoing actions. 生物力学成本会影响持续行动中的决策。
IF 2.1 3区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00090.2024
Cesar Augusto Canaveral, William Lata, Andrea M Green, Paul Cisek

Accurate interaction with the environment relies on the integration of external information about the spatial layout of potential actions and knowledge of their costs and benefits. Previous studies have shown that when given a choice between voluntary reaching movements, humans tend to prefer actions with lower biomechanical costs. However, these studies primarily focused on decisions made before the onset of movement ("decide-then-act" scenarios), and it is not known to what extent their conclusions generalize to many real-life situations, in which decisions occur during ongoing actions ("decide-while-acting"). For example, one recent study found that biomechanical costs did not influence decisions to switch from a continuous manual tracking movement to a point-to-point movement, suggesting that biomechanical costs may be disregarded in decide-while-acting scenarios. To better understand this surprising result, we designed an experiment in which participants were faced with the decision between continuing to track a target moving along a straight path or changing paths to track a new target that gradually moved along a direction that deviated from the initial one. We manipulated tracking direction, angular deviation rate, and side of deviation, allowing us to compare scenarios where biomechanical costs favored either continuing or changing the path. Crucially, here the choice was always between two continuous tracking actions. Our results show that in this situation decisions clearly took biomechanical costs into account. Thus we conclude that biomechanics are not disregarded during decide-while-acting scenarios but rather that cost comparisons can only be made between similar types of actions.NEW & NOTEWORTHY In this study, we aim to shed light on how biomechanical factors influence decisions made during ongoing actions. Previous work suggested that decisions made during actions disregard biomechanical costs, in contrast to decisions made before movement. Our results challenge that proposal and suggest instead that the effect of biomechanical factors is dependent on the types of actions being compared (e.g., continuous tracking vs. point-to-point reaching). These findings contribute to our understanding of the dynamic interplay between biomechanical considerations and action choices during ongoing interactions with the environment.

与环境的准确互动有赖于整合有关潜在行动空间布局的外部信息以及对其成本和收益的了解。以往的研究表明,当人类在自愿伸手动作之间做出选择时,往往倾向于选择生物力学成本较低的动作。然而,这些研究主要关注的是在动作开始前做出的决定("决定--然后--行动 "情景),至于他们的结论在多大程度上可以推广到现实生活中的许多情况(即决定发生在正在进行的动作过程中("决定--同时--行动")),目前还不得而知。例如,最近的一项研究发现,生物力学成本并不影响从连续手动跟踪运动转换到点对点运动的决策,这表明生物力学成本可能会在 "边行动边决策 "的情况下被忽略。为了更好地理解这一令人惊讶的结果,我们设计了一个实验,让参与者在继续追踪一个沿直线路径移动的目标,或改变路径追踪一个沿偏离初始目标的方向逐渐移动的新目标之间做出决定。我们操纵了追踪方向、角度偏差率和偏差侧,从而比较了生物力学成本对继续追踪或改变路径更有利的情况。最重要的是,这里的选择总是在两个连续的追踪动作之间进行。我们的结果表明,在这种情况下,决策显然考虑了生物力学成本。因此,我们得出结论,在 "边做决定边行动 "的情况下,生物力学并没有被忽视,相反,只有在类似类型的行动之间才能进行成本比较。
{"title":"Biomechanical costs influence decisions made during ongoing actions.","authors":"Cesar Augusto Canaveral, William Lata, Andrea M Green, Paul Cisek","doi":"10.1152/jn.00090.2024","DOIUrl":"10.1152/jn.00090.2024","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accurate interaction with the environment relies on the integration of external information about the spatial layout of potential actions and knowledge of their costs and benefits. Previous studies have shown that when given a choice between voluntary reaching movements, humans tend to prefer actions with lower biomechanical costs. However, these studies primarily focused on decisions made before the onset of movement (\"decide-then-act\" scenarios), and it is not known to what extent their conclusions generalize to many real-life situations, in which decisions occur during ongoing actions (\"decide-while-acting\"). For example, one recent study found that biomechanical costs did not influence decisions to switch from a continuous manual tracking movement to a point-to-point movement, suggesting that biomechanical costs may be disregarded in decide-while-acting scenarios. To better understand this surprising result, we designed an experiment in which participants were faced with the decision between continuing to track a target moving along a straight path or changing paths to track a new target that gradually moved along a direction that deviated from the initial one. We manipulated tracking direction, angular deviation rate, and side of deviation, allowing us to compare scenarios where biomechanical costs favored either continuing or changing the path. Crucially, here the choice was always between two continuous tracking actions. Our results show that in this situation decisions clearly took biomechanical costs into account. Thus we conclude that biomechanics are not disregarded during decide-while-acting scenarios but rather that cost comparisons can only be made between similar types of actions.<b>NEW & NOTEWORTHY</b> In this study, we aim to shed light on how biomechanical factors influence decisions made during ongoing actions. Previous work suggested that decisions made during actions disregard biomechanical costs, in contrast to decisions made before movement. Our results challenge that proposal and suggest instead that the effect of biomechanical factors is dependent on the types of actions being compared (e.g., continuous tracking vs. point-to-point reaching). These findings contribute to our understanding of the dynamic interplay between biomechanical considerations and action choices during ongoing interactions with the environment.</p>","PeriodicalId":16563,"journal":{"name":"Journal of neurophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11427048/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141579960","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of neurophysiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1