首页 > 最新文献

Primates最新文献

英文 中文
Automatic identification of stone-handling behaviour in Japanese macaques using LabGym artificial intelligence. 利用 LabGym 人工智能自动识别日本猕猴的搬石行为。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01123-x
Théo Ardoin, Cédric Sueur

The latest advances in artificial intelligence technology have opened doors to the video analysis of complex behaviours. In light of this, ethologists are actively exploring the potential of these innovations to streamline the time-intensive behavioural analysis process using video data. Several tools have been developed for this purpose in primatology in the past decade. Nonetheless, each tool grapples with technical constraints. To address these limitations, we have established a comprehensive protocol designed to harness the capabilities of a cutting-edge artificial intelligence-assisted software, LabGym. The primary objective of this study was to evaluate the suitability of LabGym for the analysis of primate behaviour, focusing on Japanese macaques as our model subjects. First, we developed a model that accurately detects Japanese macaques, allowing us to analyse their actions using LabGym. Our behavioural analysis model succeeded in recognising stone-handling-like behaviours on video. However, the absence of quantitative data within the specified time frame limits the ability of our study to draw definitive conclusions regarding the quality of the behavioural analysis. Nevertheless, to the best of our knowledge, this study represents the first instance of applying the LabGym tool specifically for the analysis of primate behaviours, with our model focusing on the automated recognition and categorisation of specific behaviours in Japanese macaques. It lays the groundwork for future research in this promising field to complexify our model using the latest version of LabGym and associated tools, such as multi-class detection and interactive behaviour analysis.

人工智能技术的最新进展为复杂行为的视频分析打开了大门。有鉴于此,人种学家们正在积极探索这些创新技术的潜力,以便利用视频数据简化耗时的行为分析过程。在过去的十年中,灵长类动物学界已经为此开发了多种工具。然而,每种工具都受到技术上的限制。为了解决这些局限性,我们建立了一个综合方案,旨在利用尖端人工智能辅助软件 LabGym 的功能。本研究的主要目的是评估 LabGym 在灵长类动物行为分析中的适用性,重点是以日本猕猴为模型对象。首先,我们开发了一个能够准确检测日本猕猴的模型,以便使用 LabGym 分析它们的行为。我们的行为分析模型成功地在视频中识别出了类似握石的行为。然而,由于没有在规定时间内获得定量数据,我们的研究无法就行为分析的质量得出明确结论。尽管如此,据我们所知,本研究是首次将 LabGym 工具专门用于灵长类动物行为分析的实例,我们的模型侧重于日本猕猴特定行为的自动识别和分类。这项研究为今后在这一前景广阔的领域开展研究奠定了基础,以便利用最新版本的 LabGym 和相关工具(如多类检测和交互式行为分析)使我们的模型更加复杂。
{"title":"Automatic identification of stone-handling behaviour in Japanese macaques using LabGym artificial intelligence.","authors":"Théo Ardoin, Cédric Sueur","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01123-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-024-01123-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The latest advances in artificial intelligence technology have opened doors to the video analysis of complex behaviours. In light of this, ethologists are actively exploring the potential of these innovations to streamline the time-intensive behavioural analysis process using video data. Several tools have been developed for this purpose in primatology in the past decade. Nonetheless, each tool grapples with technical constraints. To address these limitations, we have established a comprehensive protocol designed to harness the capabilities of a cutting-edge artificial intelligence-assisted software, LabGym. The primary objective of this study was to evaluate the suitability of LabGym for the analysis of primate behaviour, focusing on Japanese macaques as our model subjects. First, we developed a model that accurately detects Japanese macaques, allowing us to analyse their actions using LabGym. Our behavioural analysis model succeeded in recognising stone-handling-like behaviours on video. However, the absence of quantitative data within the specified time frame limits the ability of our study to draw definitive conclusions regarding the quality of the behavioural analysis. Nevertheless, to the best of our knowledge, this study represents the first instance of applying the LabGym tool specifically for the analysis of primate behaviours, with our model focusing on the automated recognition and categorisation of specific behaviours in Japanese macaques. It lays the groundwork for future research in this promising field to complexify our model using the latest version of LabGym and associated tools, such as multi-class detection and interactive behaviour analysis.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"159-172"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140194424","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mantled howler monkey males assess their rivals through formant spacing of long-distance calls. 蝠鲼雄性吼猴通过长途鸣叫的声调间距来评估对手。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01120-0
Natalia Maya Lastra, Ariadna Rangel Negrín, Alejandro Coyohua Fuentes, Pedro A D Dias

Formant frequency spacing of long-distance vocalizations is allometrically related to body size and could represent an honest signal of fighting potential. There is, however, only limited evidence that primates use formant spacing to assess the competitive potential of rivals during interactions with extragroup males, a risky context. We hypothesized that if formant spacing of long-distance calls is inversely related to the fighting potential of male mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata), then males should: (1) be more likely and (2) faster to display vocal responses to calling rivals; (3) be more likely and (4) faster to approach calling rivals; and have higher fecal (5) glucocorticoid and (6) testosterone metabolite concentrations in response to rivals calling at intermediate and high formant spacing than to those with low formant spacing. We studied the behavioral responses of 11 adult males to playback experiments of long-distance calls from unknown individuals with low (i.e., emulating large individuals), intermediate, and high (i.e., small individuals) formant spacing (n = 36 experiments). We assayed fecal glucocorticoid and testosterone metabolite concentrations (n = 174). Playbacks always elicited vocal responses, but males responded quicker to intermediate than to low formant spacing playbacks. Low formant spacing calls were less likely to elicit approaches whereas high formant spacing calls resulted in quicker approaches. Males showed stronger hormonal responses to low than to both intermediate and high formant spacing calls. It is possible that males do not escalate conflicts with rivals with low formant spacing calls if these are perceived as large, and against whom winning probabilities should decrease and confrontation costs increase; but are willing to escalate conflicts with rivals of high formant spacing. Formant spacing may therefore be an important signal for rival assessment in this species.

远距离发声的共振频率间距与体型呈等比关系,可能是战斗潜力的真实信号。然而,只有有限的证据表明,灵长类动物在与群外雄性动物(一种危险的环境)进行互动时,会利用声调间距来评估对手的竞争潜力。我们假设,如果雄性蝠鲼吼猴(Alouatta palliata)长距离鸣叫的声调间距与战斗潜力成反比,那么雄性吼猴就应该(1)更有可能(2)更多地使用声调间距:(1)更有可能和(2)更快地对呼唤对手的声音做出反应;(3)更有可能和(4)更快地接近呼唤对手;对中高声调间距呼唤对手的粪便中(5)糖皮质激素和(6)睾酮代谢物浓度高于低声调间距呼唤对手的粪便中(5)糖皮质激素和(6)睾酮代谢物浓度。我们研究了11只成年雄性个体对未知个体以低度(即模仿大型个体)、中度和高度(即小型个体)声调间距进行的长途呼叫回放实验(n = 36次实验)的行为反应。我们检测了粪便中糖皮质激素和睾酮代谢物的浓度(n = 174)。回放总是能引起雄性的声音反应,但雄性对中等声调间距的回放比对低声调间距的回放反应更快。低声调间距的叫声不太可能引起雄性接近,而高声调间距的叫声则会导致雄性更快地接近。雄性对低声调间距鸣叫的荷尔蒙反应强于对中声调间距和高声调间距鸣叫的荷尔蒙反应。雄性可能不会升级与低声调间距鸣叫的对手的冲突,如果这些对手被认为是大个子的话,与他们的冲突中获胜的概率会降低,对抗成本会增加;但雄性却愿意升级与高声调间距的对手的冲突。因此,在该物种中,声调间距可能是评估对手的一个重要信号。
{"title":"Mantled howler monkey males assess their rivals through formant spacing of long-distance calls.","authors":"Natalia Maya Lastra, Ariadna Rangel Negrín, Alejandro Coyohua Fuentes, Pedro A D Dias","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01120-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-024-01120-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Formant frequency spacing of long-distance vocalizations is allometrically related to body size and could represent an honest signal of fighting potential. There is, however, only limited evidence that primates use formant spacing to assess the competitive potential of rivals during interactions with extragroup males, a risky context. We hypothesized that if formant spacing of long-distance calls is inversely related to the fighting potential of male mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata), then males should: (1) be more likely and (2) faster to display vocal responses to calling rivals; (3) be more likely and (4) faster to approach calling rivals; and have higher fecal (5) glucocorticoid and (6) testosterone metabolite concentrations in response to rivals calling at intermediate and high formant spacing than to those with low formant spacing. We studied the behavioral responses of 11 adult males to playback experiments of long-distance calls from unknown individuals with low (i.e., emulating large individuals), intermediate, and high (i.e., small individuals) formant spacing (n = 36 experiments). We assayed fecal glucocorticoid and testosterone metabolite concentrations (n = 174). Playbacks always elicited vocal responses, but males responded quicker to intermediate than to low formant spacing playbacks. Low formant spacing calls were less likely to elicit approaches whereas high formant spacing calls resulted in quicker approaches. Males showed stronger hormonal responses to low than to both intermediate and high formant spacing calls. It is possible that males do not escalate conflicts with rivals with low formant spacing calls if these are perceived as large, and against whom winning probabilities should decrease and confrontation costs increase; but are willing to escalate conflicts with rivals of high formant spacing. Formant spacing may therefore be an important signal for rival assessment in this species.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"183-190"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139913281","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Let's not use it! A dynamic no-use zone between the home ranges of two spider monkey groups. 我们不要使用它!两个蜘蛛猴群的家园之间有一个动态禁用区。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01119-7
Edoardo Pietrangeli, Amor Aline Saldaña-Sánchez, Denise Spaan, Filippo Aureli

It is common that neighboring groups of the same species use some of the same areas, resulting in home-range overlap. Areas between the home ranges of neighboring groups not used by either group (no-use zone or NUZ) are rarely reported. Here, we report the existence of a NUZ between the home ranges of two Geoffroy's spider monkey groups, and examine its spatial changes over time and the ecological and behavioral underpinnings of such phenomenon. Although its size and location changed between 2017 and 2022, the NUZ was always present. We did not find any differences in the vegetation structure and composition between the NUZ and the ranging areas and in the monkeys' activity patterns between areas adjacent to the NUZ and the other parts of the ranging areas. The number of monkey vocalizations was lower and subgroup size was smaller (although the number of males did not differ) in areas adjacent to the NUZ than in the other parts of the ranging areas. Both changes possibly reflect the tendency to conceal their presence to the neighboring group. Our findings contribute to the understanding of primate space use and highlight the need to focus on the areas delimiting home ranges.

同一物种的相邻类群使用相同区域的情况很常见,这就造成了家园范围的重叠。但很少有报道称,相邻种群的巢区之间存在未被任何一个种群使用的区域(禁用区或 NUZ)。在此,我们报告了两个杰弗里蛛猴群体的家园范围之间存在一个NUZ,并研究了其空间随时间的变化以及这种现象的生态和行为基础。虽然其大小和位置在2017年至2022年间发生了变化,但NUZ始终存在。我们没有发现NUZ与分布区之间植被结构和组成的差异,也没有发现NUZ附近区域与分布区其他区域之间猴子活动模式的差异。在NUZ邻近地区,猴子的发声次数比在其他地区少,亚群的规模也比其他地区小(尽管雄性猴子的数量没有差异)。这两种变化可能都反映了向邻近群体隐藏其存在的趋势。我们的研究结果有助于了解灵长类动物的空间利用情况,并强调了关注划定家园范围的区域的必要性。
{"title":"Let's not use it! A dynamic no-use zone between the home ranges of two spider monkey groups.","authors":"Edoardo Pietrangeli, Amor Aline Saldaña-Sánchez, Denise Spaan, Filippo Aureli","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01119-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-024-01119-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is common that neighboring groups of the same species use some of the same areas, resulting in home-range overlap. Areas between the home ranges of neighboring groups not used by either group (no-use zone or NUZ) are rarely reported. Here, we report the existence of a NUZ between the home ranges of two Geoffroy's spider monkey groups, and examine its spatial changes over time and the ecological and behavioral underpinnings of such phenomenon. Although its size and location changed between 2017 and 2022, the NUZ was always present. We did not find any differences in the vegetation structure and composition between the NUZ and the ranging areas and in the monkeys' activity patterns between areas adjacent to the NUZ and the other parts of the ranging areas. The number of monkey vocalizations was lower and subgroup size was smaller (although the number of males did not differ) in areas adjacent to the NUZ than in the other parts of the ranging areas. Both changes possibly reflect the tendency to conceal their presence to the neighboring group. Our findings contribute to the understanding of primate space use and highlight the need to focus on the areas delimiting home ranges.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"173-181"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140023964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Building resilience in primate tourism: insights from the COVID-19 pandemic and future directions. 建立灵长类旅游业的复原力:COVID-19 大流行的启示和未来方向。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01126-8
Rie Usui, Lori K Sheeran, Ashton M Asbury, Lene Pedersen

The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic revealed the vulnerability of the tourism industry, triggering a call for a structural shift. This study focuses on COVID-19 impacts on primate tourism sites using the resilience-based wildlife tourism study of Jones et al. (2023) as an interpretive framework. Using an online survey, we collected data on impacts, changes, and challenges experienced at primate tourism destinations in various parts of the world. Based on 33 responses, the study found that the most profound impacts were financial, compromising the ability to run facilities and facilitate tourism due mainly to limitation of access to sites for tourists and/or staff/researchers. Seventeen respondents reported that their sites did not make substantial changes in response to the pandemic. This warrants further study to elicit the reasons for the lack of response. It may indicate difficulties in adaptation or implementation due to limited resources or other factors amid COVID-19 pandemic. We suggest that the framework proposed by Jones et al. (2023) has limitations in effectively addressing rapid and extensive repercussions of a disruption such as the COVID-19 pandemic.

冠状病毒病 2019(COVID-19)大流行揭示了旅游业的脆弱性,引发了结构转变的呼声。本研究以 Jones 等人(2023 年)基于恢复力的野生动物旅游研究为解释框架,重点关注 COVID-19 对灵长类动物旅游景点的影响。通过在线调查,我们收集了世界各地灵长类旅游目的地所经历的影响、变化和挑战的相关数据。根据 33 份回复,研究发现最深远的影响是财务影响,主要是由于游客和/或工作人员/研究人员进入景点受到限制,从而影响了运营设施和促进旅游的能力。17 个受访者报告说,他们的旅游景点没有针对大流行病做出实质性改变。这一点值得进一步研究,以找出缺乏应对措施的原因。这可能表明,在 COVID-19 大流行期间,由于资源有限或其他因素,在适应或实施方面存在困难。我们认为,Jones 等人(2023 年)提出的框架在有效应对 COVID-19 大流行等破坏性事件的快速和广泛影响方面存在局限性。
{"title":"Building resilience in primate tourism: insights from the COVID-19 pandemic and future directions.","authors":"Rie Usui, Lori K Sheeran, Ashton M Asbury, Lene Pedersen","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01126-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-024-01126-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic revealed the vulnerability of the tourism industry, triggering a call for a structural shift. This study focuses on COVID-19 impacts on primate tourism sites using the resilience-based wildlife tourism study of Jones et al. (2023) as an interpretive framework. Using an online survey, we collected data on impacts, changes, and challenges experienced at primate tourism destinations in various parts of the world. Based on 33 responses, the study found that the most profound impacts were financial, compromising the ability to run facilities and facilitate tourism due mainly to limitation of access to sites for tourists and/or staff/researchers. Seventeen respondents reported that their sites did not make substantial changes in response to the pandemic. This warrants further study to elicit the reasons for the lack of response. It may indicate difficulties in adaptation or implementation due to limited resources or other factors amid COVID-19 pandemic. We suggest that the framework proposed by Jones et al. (2023) has limitations in effectively addressing rapid and extensive repercussions of a disruption such as the COVID-19 pandemic.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"191-201"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140319043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Water scooping: tool use by a wild bonobo (Pan paniscus) at LuiKotale, a case report. 舀水:LuiKotale 野生倭黑猩猩(Pan paniscus)使用工具的案例报告。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01121-z
Sonya Pashchevskaya, Barbara Fruth, Gottfried Hohmann

Tool use diversity is often considered to differentiate our two closest living relatives: the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and the bonobo (P. paniscus). Chimpanzees appear to have the largest repertoire of tools amongst nonhuman primates, and in this species, many forms of tool use enhance food and water acquisition. In captivity, bonobos seem as adept as chimpanzees in tool use complexity, including in the foraging context. However, in the wild, bonobos have only been observed engaging in habitual tool use in the contexts of comfort, play, self-directed behaviour and communication, whilst no tool-assisted food acquisition has been reported. Whereas captive bonobos use tools for drinking, so far, the only report from the wild populations comes down to four observations of moss sponges used at Lomako. Here, we present the first report of tool use in the form of water scooping by a wild bonobo at LuiKotale. An adult female was observed and videotaped whilst using an emptied Cola chlamydantha pod to scoop and drink water from a stream. We discuss the conditions for such observations and the importance of looking out for rare behaviours and attempt to put the observation into the context of the opportunity versus necessity hypotheses. By adding novel information on tool use, our report contributes to the ongoing efforts to differentiate population-specific traits in the behavioural ecology of the bonobo.

工具使用的多样性通常被认为是区分我们的两个近亲:黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)和倭黑猩猩(P. paniscus)的标志。在非人类灵长类动物中,黑猩猩似乎拥有最多的工具,在这一物种中,许多工具的使用形式都有助于获取食物和水。在人工饲养条件下,倭黑猩猩似乎与黑猩猩一样善于使用复杂的工具,包括觅食工具。然而,在野外,人们只观察到倭黑猩猩在舒适、玩耍、自主行为和交流时习惯性地使用工具,而没有观察到倭黑猩猩使用工具辅助获取食物。虽然圈养倭黑猩猩使用工具饮水,但迄今为止,来自野生种群的唯一报告是在洛马科观察到的四次使用苔藓海绵的情况。在这里,我们首次报告了吕科塔莱野生倭黑猩猩使用工具舀水的情况。我们观察并录制了一只成年雌性倭黑猩猩使用掏空的Cola chlamydantha豆荚从溪流中舀水喝的过程。我们讨论了进行此类观察的条件和注意罕见行为的重要性,并试图将观察结果纳入机会与必要性假说的背景中。我们的报告增加了关于工具使用的新信息,有助于在倭黑猩猩行为生态学中区分种群特异性的持续努力。
{"title":"Water scooping: tool use by a wild bonobo (Pan paniscus) at LuiKotale, a case report.","authors":"Sonya Pashchevskaya, Barbara Fruth, Gottfried Hohmann","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01121-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-024-01121-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Tool use diversity is often considered to differentiate our two closest living relatives: the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and the bonobo (P. paniscus). Chimpanzees appear to have the largest repertoire of tools amongst nonhuman primates, and in this species, many forms of tool use enhance food and water acquisition. In captivity, bonobos seem as adept as chimpanzees in tool use complexity, including in the foraging context. However, in the wild, bonobos have only been observed engaging in habitual tool use in the contexts of comfort, play, self-directed behaviour and communication, whilst no tool-assisted food acquisition has been reported. Whereas captive bonobos use tools for drinking, so far, the only report from the wild populations comes down to four observations of moss sponges used at Lomako. Here, we present the first report of tool use in the form of water scooping by a wild bonobo at LuiKotale. An adult female was observed and videotaped whilst using an emptied Cola chlamydantha pod to scoop and drink water from a stream. We discuss the conditions for such observations and the importance of looking out for rare behaviours and attempt to put the observation into the context of the opportunity versus necessity hypotheses. By adding novel information on tool use, our report contributes to the ongoing efforts to differentiate population-specific traits in the behavioural ecology of the bonobo.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"145-150"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11018684/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140137195","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Savanna chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) crop feeding at Dindefelo, Senegal: challenges and implications for conservation 塞内加尔 Dindefelo 的热带稀树草原黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)农作物取食:保护工作面临的挑战和影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01125-9
Laia Dotras, Amanda Barciela, Manuel Llana, Jordi Galbany, R. Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are categorized as Endangered by the International Union for Conservation of Nature, and habitat loss due to conversion of land for agriculture is one of the major threats to wild populations of this species. This challenging scenario can lead to negative human-chimpanzee interactions, including crop feeding. Chimpanzees consume crops across their geographical range, although little is known about this behavior in savanna habitats. Here we provide new evidence of crop feeding by savanna chimpanzees. We conducted our observations at Dindefelo, a community nature reserve in southeastern Senegal. The chimpanzees were observed to feed on mango (Mangifera indica) and also on baobab (Adansonia digitata), a wild species considered a crop by local people when found in and around villages. Although local people use the fruits of these species for food and income, they tolerated crop-feeding events until recently. In 2023, a case of harassment of a crop-feeding chimpanzee in a mango orchard was witnessed, and four days later a chimpanzee corpse was found at the same place. We conclude that habitat conversion into agricultural fields, uncontrolled bush fires and extraction of wild fruits are the important factors influencing crop-feeding events at Dindefelo. Our findings highlight the need to better understand human-chimpanzee interactions in the anthropogenic landscape of Dindefelo to help mitigate negative attitudes and behaviors towards chimpanzees.

黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)被国际自然保护联盟列为濒危物种,而土地转为农业用地导致的栖息地丧失是该物种野生种群面临的主要威胁之一。这种具有挑战性的情况可能会导致人类与黑猩猩之间的负面互动,包括以农作物为食。黑猩猩在其地理分布范围内都会捕食农作物,但人们对其在热带稀树草原栖息地的捕食行为知之甚少。在这里,我们提供了热带稀树草原黑猩猩取食农作物的新证据。我们在塞内加尔东南部的一个社区自然保护区 Dindefelo 进行了观察。我们观察到黑猩猩以芒果(Mangifera indica)和猴面包树(Adansonia digitata)为食。虽然当地人用这些物种的果实作为食物和收入,但直到最近,他们对以农作物为食的行为一直持容忍态度。2023 年,芒果园里发生了一起黑猩猩骚扰农作物的事件,四天后在同一地点发现了一具黑猩猩尸体。我们的结论是,将栖息地转化为农田、无节制的丛林火灾和采摘野果是影响丁德费洛黑猩猩以农作物为食事件的重要因素。我们的研究结果突出表明,有必要更好地了解人类与黑猩猩在丁德费洛人为景观中的互动,以帮助减轻人们对黑猩猩的负面态度和行为。
{"title":"Savanna chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) crop feeding at Dindefelo, Senegal: challenges and implications for conservation","authors":"Laia Dotras, Amanda Barciela, Manuel Llana, Jordi Galbany, R. Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01125-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-024-01125-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Chimpanzees (<i>Pan troglodytes</i>) are categorized as Endangered by the International Union for Conservation of Nature, and habitat loss due to conversion of land for agriculture is one of the major threats to wild populations of this species. This challenging scenario can lead to negative human-chimpanzee interactions, including crop feeding. Chimpanzees consume crops across their geographical range, although little is known about this behavior in savanna habitats. Here we provide new evidence of crop feeding by savanna chimpanzees. We conducted our observations at Dindefelo, a community nature reserve in southeastern Senegal. The chimpanzees were observed to feed on mango (<i>Mangifera indica</i>) and also on baobab (<i>Adansonia digitata</i>), a wild species considered a crop by local people when found in and around villages. Although local people use the fruits of these species for food and income, they tolerated crop-feeding events until recently. In 2023, a case of harassment of a crop-feeding chimpanzee in a mango orchard was witnessed, and four days later a chimpanzee corpse was found at the same place. We conclude that habitat conversion into agricultural fields, uncontrolled bush fires and extraction of wild fruits are the important factors influencing crop-feeding events at Dindefelo. Our findings highlight the need to better understand human-chimpanzee interactions in the anthropogenic landscape of Dindefelo to help mitigate negative attitudes and behaviors towards chimpanzees.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"43 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140829348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Twinning in wild, endangered lion-tailed macaques Macaca silenus in the Anamalai Hills of the Western Ghats, India 印度西高止山阿纳马莱山野生濒危狮尾猕猴的孪生现象
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01129-5
Ashni Kumar Dhawale, Anindya Sinha

Many primate species show various behavioural and ecological adaptations to provisioning, one of which is the unusual occurrence of twins. Here, we report observations on two pairs of surviving twins in lion-tailed macaques Macaca silenus in the Anamalai Hills of the Western Ghats, India. The Puthuthottam population of lion-tailed macaques has historically been restricted to a rainforest fragment measuring 92 ha, situated adjacent to human settlements. Over the last 10 years, however, several groups from this population have begun to directly interact with the local human communities, visiting settlements at a rate of 0.52 events/day and exploiting various anthropogenic food resources. We followed and opportunistically collected behavioural ad libitum data on two sets of twins for a year, between March 2019 and March 2020. Both of the mothers were primarily terrestrial, although the mother with the younger set of twins also used the tree canopy and other precarious substrates, such as cables. Although two previous cases of twinning have been reported in this population, one in the late 1990s and one between 2000 and 2002, neither of those sets of twins survived beyond a few weeks, with at least one infant in each pair dying of unknown causes. We discuss, but discount, the possibility that one of the infants in either set of twins was an adoptee. Our observations indicate that some lion-tailed macaque twins can survive under free-ranging conditions if they receive adequate care from their biological mother or another female. Our findings also provide further evidence of increased rates of twinning as a consequence of dietary changes in synanthropic non-human primate populations.

许多灵长类动物在行为和生态方面都表现出对供给的各种适应,其中之一就是不寻常地出现双胞胎。在这里,我们报告了对印度西高止山阿纳马莱山狮尾猕猴(Macaca silenus)两对存活双胞胎的观察结果。Puthuthottam 的狮尾猕猴种群历来局限于一片面积为 92 公顷的雨林片区,毗邻人类居住区。然而,在过去 10 年中,该种群中的几个群体开始与当地人类社区直接互动,以每天 0.52 次的频率造访人类居住区,并利用各种人类食物资源。在 2019 年 3 月至 2020 年 3 月的一年时间里,我们对两对双胞胎进行了跟踪观察,并伺机收集了它们的日常行为数据。两对双胞胎的母亲都以陆地活动为主,但双胞胎中年龄较小的母亲也会利用树冠和电缆等其他不稳定的基质。虽然该种群曾有过两例双胞胎,一例发生在 20 世纪 90 年代末,另一例发生在 2000 年至 2002 年间,但这两例双胞胎都没有存活超过几周,每对双胞胎中至少有一名婴儿死于不明原因。我们讨论了这两对双胞胎中的一个婴儿是被收养者的可能性,但不考虑这种可能性。我们的观察结果表明,一些狮尾猕猴双胞胎如果能得到亲生母亲或其他雌性的充分照顾,就能在自由放养条件下存活下来。我们的研究结果还进一步证明,在同类非人灵长类种群中,由于饮食习惯的改变,双胞胎的比例会增加。
{"title":"Twinning in wild, endangered lion-tailed macaques Macaca silenus in the Anamalai Hills of the Western Ghats, India","authors":"Ashni Kumar Dhawale, Anindya Sinha","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01129-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-024-01129-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many primate species show various behavioural and ecological adaptations to provisioning, one of which is the unusual occurrence of twins. Here, we report observations on two pairs of surviving twins in lion-tailed macaques <i>Macaca silenus</i> in the Anamalai Hills of the Western Ghats, India. The Puthuthottam population of lion-tailed macaques has historically been restricted to a rainforest fragment measuring 92 ha, situated adjacent to human settlements. Over the last 10 years, however, several groups from this population have begun to directly interact with the local human communities, visiting settlements at a rate of 0.52 events/day and exploiting various anthropogenic food resources. We followed and opportunistically collected behavioural ad libitum data on two sets of twins for a year, between March 2019 and March 2020. Both of the mothers were primarily terrestrial, although the mother with the younger set of twins also used the tree canopy and other precarious substrates, such as cables. Although two previous cases of twinning have been reported in this population, one in the late 1990s and one between 2000 and 2002, neither of those sets of twins survived beyond a few weeks, with at least one infant in each pair dying of unknown causes. We discuss, but discount, the possibility that one of the infants in either set of twins was an adoptee. Our observations indicate that some lion-tailed macaque twins can survive under free-ranging conditions if they receive adequate care from their biological mother or another female. Our findings also provide further evidence of increased rates of twinning as a consequence of dietary changes in synanthropic non-human primate populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"356 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140602918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduction of bitter taste receptor gene family in folivorous colobine primates relative to omnivorous cercopithecine primates 相对于杂食性栉猴灵长类动物,食叶性疣猴灵长类动物的苦味受体基因家族减少了
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01124-w
Min Hou, Muhammad Shoaib Akhtar, Masahiro Hayashi, Ryuichi Ashino, Akiko Matsumoto-Oda, Takashi Hayakawa, Takafumi Ishida, Amanda D. Melin, Hiroo Imai, Shoji Kawamura

Bitter taste perception is important in preventing animals from ingesting potentially toxic compounds. Whole-genome assembly (WGA) data have revealed that bitter taste receptor genes (TAS2Rs) comprise a multigene family with dozens of intact and disrupted genes in primates. However, publicly available WGA data are often incomplete, especially for multigene families. In this study, we employed a targeted capture (TC) approach specifically probing TAS2Rs for ten species of cercopithecid primates with diverse diets, including eight omnivorous cercopithecine species and two folivorous colobine species. We designed RNA probes for all TAS2Rs that we modeled to be intact in the common ancestor of cercopithecids (“ancestral-cercopithecid TAS2R gene set”). The TC was followed by short-read and high-depth massive-parallel sequencing. TC retrieved more intact TAS2R genes than found in WGA databases. We confirmed a large number of gene “births” at the common ancestor of cercopithecids and found that the colobine common ancestor and the cercopithecine common ancestor had contrasting trajectories: four gene “deaths” and three gene births, respectively. The number of intact TAS2R genes was markedly reduced in colobines (25–28 detected via TC and 20–26 detected via WGA analysis) as compared with cercopithecines (27–36 via TC and 19–30 via WGA). Birth or death events occurred at almost every phylogenetic-tree branch, making the composition of intact genes variable among species. These results show that evolutionary change in intact TAS2R genes is a complex process, refute a simple general prediction that herbivory favors more TAS2R genes, and have implications for understanding dietary adaptations and the evolution of detoxification abilities.

苦味感知对于防止动物摄入可能有毒的化合物非常重要。全基因组组装(WGA)数据显示,在灵长类动物中,苦味受体基因(TAS2Rs)组成了一个多基因家族,其中有数十个完整的基因和中断的基因。然而,公开的 WGA 数据往往不完整,尤其是多基因家族的数据。在这项研究中,我们采用了一种靶向捕获(TC)方法,专门探测了十种不同食性的纤毛灵长类动物的 TAS2Rs,其中包括八种杂食性纤毛灵长类动物和两种对叶食性疣猴。我们为所有 TAS2R 设计了 RNA 探针,我们认为这些探针在腕足动物的共同祖先中是完整的("祖先-腕足动物 TAS2R 基因组")。TC之后是短线程和高深度大规模平行测序。与 WGA 数据库相比,TC 检索到了更多完整的 TAS2R 基因。我们证实了栉猴类共同祖先有大量基因 "诞生",并发现疣猴类共同祖先和栉猴类共同祖先有着截然不同的轨迹:分别有四个基因 "死亡 "和三个基因 "诞生"。疣猴完整的 TAS2R 基因数量(通过 TC 分析检测到 25-28 个,通过 WGA 分析检测到 20-26 个)明显少于秧鸡(通过 TC 分析检测到 27-36 个,通过 WGA 分析检测到 19-30 个)。出生或死亡事件几乎发生在每一个系统发育树分支上,这使得物种间完整基因的组成各不相同。这些结果表明,完整的 TAS2R 基因的进化变化是一个复杂的过程,驳斥了食草动物偏爱更多 TAS2R 基因的简单一般预测,对理解饮食适应性和解毒能力的进化具有重要意义。
{"title":"Reduction of bitter taste receptor gene family in folivorous colobine primates relative to omnivorous cercopithecine primates","authors":"Min Hou, Muhammad Shoaib Akhtar, Masahiro Hayashi, Ryuichi Ashino, Akiko Matsumoto-Oda, Takashi Hayakawa, Takafumi Ishida, Amanda D. Melin, Hiroo Imai, Shoji Kawamura","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01124-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-024-01124-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Bitter taste perception is important in preventing animals from ingesting potentially toxic compounds. Whole-genome assembly (WGA) data have revealed that bitter taste receptor genes (<i>TAS2R</i>s) comprise a multigene family with dozens of intact and disrupted genes in primates. However, publicly available WGA data are often incomplete, especially for multigene families. In this study, we employed a targeted capture (TC) approach specifically probing <i>TAS2R</i>s for ten species of cercopithecid primates with diverse diets, including eight omnivorous cercopithecine species and two folivorous colobine species. We designed RNA probes for all <i>TAS2R</i>s that we modeled to be intact in the common ancestor of cercopithecids (“ancestral-cercopithecid <i>TAS2R</i> gene set”). The TC was followed by short-read and high-depth massive-parallel sequencing. TC retrieved more intact <i>TAS2R</i> genes than found in WGA databases. We confirmed a large number of gene “births” at the common ancestor of cercopithecids and found that the colobine common ancestor and the cercopithecine common ancestor had contrasting trajectories: four gene “deaths” and three gene births, respectively. The number of intact <i>TAS2R</i> genes was markedly reduced in colobines (25–28 detected via TC and 20–26 detected via WGA analysis) as compared with cercopithecines (27–36 via TC and 19–30 via WGA). Birth or death events occurred at almost every phylogenetic-tree branch, making the composition of intact genes variable among species. These results show that evolutionary change in intact <i>TAS2R</i> genes is a complex process, refute a simple general prediction that herbivory favors more <i>TAS2R</i> genes, and have implications for understanding dietary adaptations and the evolution of detoxification abilities. </p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"50 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140570136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The off-the-record stories primatologists tell each other 灵长类动物学家之间的秘密故事
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-024-01130-y
Bernard Thierry
{"title":"The off-the-record stories primatologists tell each other","authors":"Bernard Thierry","doi":"10.1007/s10329-024-01130-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-024-01130-y","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"117 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140570051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stillbirth of a mandrill (Mandrillus sphinx) in the wild: perinatal behaviors and delivery sequences. 野生山魈(Mandrillus sphinx)的死胎:围产期行为和分娩顺序。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01112-6
Berta Roura-Torres, Paul Amblard-Rambert, Pascal Lepou, Peter M Kappeler, Marie J E Charpentier

Birth is a fundamental event in the life of animals, including our own species. More reports of wild non-human primate births and stillbirths are thus needed to better understand the evolutionary pressures shaping parturition behaviors in our lineage. In diurnal non-human primates, births generally occur at night, when individuals are resting. Consequently, they are difficult to observe in the wild and most of the current knowledge regarding perinatal behaviors comes from rare daytime births. Information about stillbirths is even rarer and their proximate causes are generally unknown. Here, we present detailed observations of a daytime birth of a stillborn wild mandrill (Mandrillus sphinx). During this event, which lasted an entire day, we recorded the behaviors of the parturient female ad libitum, using video recordings and photos. The 5-year-old female was primiparous and of low dominance rank. The length of her pregnancy was shorter than usual and the partum phase was extremely long compared to other birth reports in non-human primates. The female disappeared shortly after this event and was assumed to have died. We discuss the possible causes of this stillbirth including the infant's presentation at birth and maternal inexperience.

分娩是动物生命中的基本事件,包括我们这个物种。因此,我们需要更多有关野生非人灵长类动物分娩和死产的报道,以便更好地了解影响我们这一物种分娩行为的进化压力。在昼伏夜出的非人灵长类动物中,分娩一般发生在夜间,此时个体正在休息。因此,很难在野外观察到它们的分娩,目前有关围产期行为的知识大多来自罕见的白天分娩。有关死胎的信息则更为罕见,其近因通常也不为人所知。在这里,我们将详细观察一只野生山魈(Mandrillus sphinx)在白天分娩的死胎。在持续一整天的分娩过程中,我们用视频和照片记录了这只雌性山魈的分娩行为。这只 5 岁大的雌豹是初产雌豹,处于较低的统治地位。与其他非人灵长类动物的分娩报告相比,它的怀孕期比通常情况要短,分娩期也非常长。这只雌性灵长类动物在分娩后不久就消失了,人们认为它已经死亡。我们讨论了这次死胎的可能原因,包括婴儿出生时的表现和母性经验不足。
{"title":"Stillbirth of a mandrill (Mandrillus sphinx) in the wild: perinatal behaviors and delivery sequences.","authors":"Berta Roura-Torres, Paul Amblard-Rambert, Pascal Lepou, Peter M Kappeler, Marie J E Charpentier","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01112-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01112-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Birth is a fundamental event in the life of animals, including our own species. More reports of wild non-human primate births and stillbirths are thus needed to better understand the evolutionary pressures shaping parturition behaviors in our lineage. In diurnal non-human primates, births generally occur at night, when individuals are resting. Consequently, they are difficult to observe in the wild and most of the current knowledge regarding perinatal behaviors comes from rare daytime births. Information about stillbirths is even rarer and their proximate causes are generally unknown. Here, we present detailed observations of a daytime birth of a stillborn wild mandrill (Mandrillus sphinx). During this event, which lasted an entire day, we recorded the behaviors of the parturient female ad libitum, using video recordings and photos. The 5-year-old female was primiparous and of low dominance rank. The length of her pregnancy was shorter than usual and the partum phase was extremely long compared to other birth reports in non-human primates. The female disappeared shortly after this event and was assumed to have died. We discuss the possible causes of this stillbirth including the infant's presentation at birth and maternal inexperience.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"75-80"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10884356/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138831164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Primates
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1