首页 > 最新文献

Primates最新文献

英文 中文
Rushing for "burned" food: Why and how does a group of patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) reach freshly burned areas? 争抢 "烧焦 "的食物:一群帕塔斯猴(Erythrocebus patas)为何以及如何到达新近被烧毁的区域?
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01113-5
Maho Hanzawa, Yoshiki Morimitsu, Erasmus H Owusu, Richard D Suu-Ire, Naofumi Nakagawa

Recently, considerable attention has been paid to animal adaptations to anthropogenic environments, such as foraging in burned areas where plants are promoted to regenerate by anthropogenic burning. However, among primates, reports on the utilization of resources that are available immediately after burning have been limited to a few primate species. In this study, we investigated and compared the activity budgets and food categories of a group of patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) in freshly burned areas by comparing them with those in previously burned areas and unburned areas. We also assessed the proportion of time spent in the freshly burned area before and after the fire: GPS collars were fitted to five of the six adults in the group, and their patterns when they traveled toward freshly burned and unburned feeding areas were compared. Patas monkeys spent more time in freshly burned areas after the fire, and they visited such areas mostly for feeding, particularly on roasted seeds of Cissus populnea. Furthermore, patas monkeys traveled faster and in a more synchronized way toward freshly burned areas. This "apparent goal-directed" travel began at least 1 h before arriving. Results indicate that the group recognized freshly burned areas as valuable, and the monkeys were able to travel in a goal-directed manner to them despite their variable locations. We suggest that smoke from freshly burned areas provides a visual cue with which to orient to the burned areas. Our results also support the notion that some primates are flexible enough to adapt to and benefit from anthropogenic environmental changes.

近来,动物对人为环境的适应受到了广泛关注,例如在人为焚烧促进植物再生的焚烧区觅食。然而,在灵长类动物中,有关焚烧后可立即获得的资源利用情况的报道仅限于少数灵长类物种。在这项研究中,我们调查并比较了一群蝙蝠猴(Erythrocebus patas)在新焚烧地区与之前焚烧地区和未焚烧地区的活动预算和食物类别。我们还评估了火灾前后在新焚烧区逗留的时间比例:我们为猴群中的六只成年猴中的五只安装了 GPS 项圈,并比较了它们前往新烧毁地区和未烧毁地区觅食的模式。火灾发生后,巴塔斯猴在新近被烧毁的地区逗留的时间更长,它们到这些地区主要是为了觅食,尤其是觅食烤熟的杨梅种子。此外,帕塔斯猴在新近被烧毁的区域行进的速度更快,步调也更一致。这种 "明显的目标定向 "旅行至少在到达前 1 小时就开始了。结果表明,猕猴群体认识到新近被烧毁的区域是有价值的,尽管这些区域的位置各不相同,但猕猴能够以目标为导向前往这些区域。我们认为,新近烧毁区域的烟雾提供了一种视觉线索,使猴子能够确定烧毁区域的方向。我们的研究结果还支持这样一种观点,即某些灵长类动物具有足够的灵活性,能够适应人为的环境变化并从中受益。
{"title":"Rushing for \"burned\" food: Why and how does a group of patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) reach freshly burned areas?","authors":"Maho Hanzawa, Yoshiki Morimitsu, Erasmus H Owusu, Richard D Suu-Ire, Naofumi Nakagawa","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01113-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01113-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recently, considerable attention has been paid to animal adaptations to anthropogenic environments, such as foraging in burned areas where plants are promoted to regenerate by anthropogenic burning. However, among primates, reports on the utilization of resources that are available immediately after burning have been limited to a few primate species. In this study, we investigated and compared the activity budgets and food categories of a group of patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) in freshly burned areas by comparing them with those in previously burned areas and unburned areas. We also assessed the proportion of time spent in the freshly burned area before and after the fire: GPS collars were fitted to five of the six adults in the group, and their patterns when they traveled toward freshly burned and unburned feeding areas were compared. Patas monkeys spent more time in freshly burned areas after the fire, and they visited such areas mostly for feeding, particularly on roasted seeds of Cissus populnea. Furthermore, patas monkeys traveled faster and in a more synchronized way toward freshly burned areas. This \"apparent goal-directed\" travel began at least 1 h before arriving. Results indicate that the group recognized freshly burned areas as valuable, and the monkeys were able to travel in a goal-directed manner to them despite their variable locations. We suggest that smoke from freshly burned areas provides a visual cue with which to orient to the burned areas. Our results also support the notion that some primates are flexible enough to adapt to and benefit from anthropogenic environmental changes.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"103-113"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139692712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fragmented forest affects the southern black-horned capuchin (Sapajus nigritus cucullatus) in the Argentinean Atlantic Forest. 森林破碎化对阿根廷大西洋森林中南方黑角卷尾猴(Sapajus nigritus cucullatus)的影响。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-18 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01114-4
Victoria Martinez de Zorzi, Sam Shanee, Luciana Inés Oklander

The southern black-horned capuchin, Sapajus nigritus cucullatus, is considered Near Threatened on the IUCN Red List and Vulnerable in Argentina. The species is mainly threatened by habitat loss and fragmentation. The aim of this study was to compare range size, group size, and density in S. n. cucullatus groups between areas of continuous and fragmented habitat in the Atlantic Forest in Argentina. The study was carried out in two areas in northern Misiones province, one continuous and one anthropogenic fragment. Fieldwork was carried out for 5 days each month from November 2019 to March 2020 and from November 2020 to March 2021. SARS-CoV-2 restrictions meant we could not survey in the intervening period. Group counts were made on existing trails and subsequent group follows. We georeferenced encounters and follows to estimate home range sizes. We calculated density based on home range modeling using 100% minimum convex polygons (MCP), and compared these using generalized linear models (GLM). Smaller groups and lower density of S. n. cucullatus were found in continuous forest, with group sizes between 12 and 23 individuals, and density of 0.14 ind/ha, whereas in the fragmented forest, group sizes were between 32 and 36, with density of 0.62 ind/ha (n = 107; zero-inflated negative binomial regression [ZINB], p < 0.05). The higher density in forest fragments may be due to reduced dispersal ability. This work highlights data on species plasticity that could contribute to the development of conservation management strategies for S. n. cucullatus and its habitat.

南方黑角卷尾猴(Sapajus nigritus cucullatus)在《世界自然保护联盟红色名录》中被视为 "近危 "物种,在阿根廷则被视为 "易危 "物种。该物种主要受到栖息地丧失和破碎化的威胁。本研究的目的是比较阿根廷大西洋森林中连续栖息地和破碎化栖息地之间 S. n. cucullatus 群体的分布范围、群体大小和密度。研究在米西奥内斯省北部的两个地区进行,一个是连续生境,一个是人为破碎生境。从 2019 年 11 月到 2020 年 3 月,以及从 2020 年 11 月到 2021 年 3 月,实地考察每月进行 5 天。由于 SARS-CoV-2 的限制,我们无法在此期间进行调查。我们对现有路径和随后跟进的群体进行了计数。我们对相遇和跟随进行了地理坐标定位,以估算家园范围的大小。我们使用 100% 最小凸多边形 (MCP) 根据家园范围模型计算密度,并使用广义线性模型 (GLM) 进行比较。在连续的森林中,S. n. cucullatus的群体较小,密度较低,群体大小在12到23只之间,密度为0.14 ind/ha,而在破碎的森林中,群体大小在32到36只之间,密度为0.62 ind/ha(n = 107;零膨胀负二项回归[ZINB],p = 0.01)。
{"title":"Fragmented forest affects the southern black-horned capuchin (Sapajus nigritus cucullatus) in the Argentinean Atlantic Forest.","authors":"Victoria Martinez de Zorzi, Sam Shanee, Luciana Inés Oklander","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01114-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01114-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The southern black-horned capuchin, Sapajus nigritus cucullatus, is considered Near Threatened on the IUCN Red List and Vulnerable in Argentina. The species is mainly threatened by habitat loss and fragmentation. The aim of this study was to compare range size, group size, and density in S. n. cucullatus groups between areas of continuous and fragmented habitat in the Atlantic Forest in Argentina. The study was carried out in two areas in northern Misiones province, one continuous and one anthropogenic fragment. Fieldwork was carried out for 5 days each month from November 2019 to March 2020 and from November 2020 to March 2021. SARS-CoV-2 restrictions meant we could not survey in the intervening period. Group counts were made on existing trails and subsequent group follows. We georeferenced encounters and follows to estimate home range sizes. We calculated density based on home range modeling using 100% minimum convex polygons (MCP), and compared these using generalized linear models (GLM). Smaller groups and lower density of S. n. cucullatus were found in continuous forest, with group sizes between 12 and 23 individuals, and density of 0.14 ind/ha, whereas in the fragmented forest, group sizes were between 32 and 36, with density of 0.62 ind/ha (n = 107; zero-inflated negative binomial regression [ZINB], p < 0.05). The higher density in forest fragments may be due to reduced dispersal ability. This work highlights data on species plasticity that could contribute to the development of conservation management strategies for S. n. cucullatus and its habitat.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"125-133"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139491809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
May the force be with you: exploring force discrimination in chimpanzees using the force-feedback device 愿力与你同在:利用力反馈装置探索黑猩猩的力辨别能力
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01117-1
Masaki Tomonaga, Yoshihiro Tanaka, Motoyuki Sakai

While force-feedback devices have been developed in areas such as virtual reality, there have been very few comparative cognitive studies in nonhuman animals using these devices. In addition, although cross-modal perception between vision and touch has been actively studied in nonhuman primates for several decades, there have been no studies of their active haptic perception. In this study, we attempted to train force discrimination in chimpanzees using a force-feedback device modified from a trackball. Chimpanzees were given different levels of force feedback (8.0 vs. 0.5 N) when moving the on-screen cursor to the target area by manipulating the trackball and were required to select one of two choice stimuli based on the force cue. The experiment was conducted using a trial-block procedure in which the same force stimulus was presented for a fixed number of trials, and the force stimulus was changed between blocks. The block size was progressively reduced from ten trials. Four chimpanzees were trained, but none reached the learning criterion (80% or more correct responses under the condition that the force stimuli were presented randomly). However, a detailed analysis of the chimpanzees’ performance before and after the trial-block switching revealed that their choice behavior could not be explained by a simple win–stay/lose–shift strategy, suggesting that the switching of the force stimuli affected the chimpanzees’ choice behavior. It was also found that the chimpanzees performed better when switching from small to large force stimuli than when switching from large to small force stimuli. Although none of the chimpanzees in this study acquired force discrimination, future studies using such force-feedback devices will provide new insights for understanding haptic cognition in nonhuman primates from a comparative cognitive perspective.

虽然在虚拟现实等领域已经开发出了力反馈设备,但在非人类动物中使用这些设备进行的比较认知研究却寥寥无几。此外,尽管几十年来人们一直在积极研究非人灵长类动物视觉和触觉之间的跨模态感知,但还没有关于它们主动触觉感知的研究。在这项研究中,我们尝试使用一个由轨迹球改装而成的力反馈装置来训练黑猩猩的力分辨能力。当黑猩猩通过操纵轨迹球将屏幕上的光标移动到目标区域时,会得到不同程度的力反馈(8.0 N 与 0.5 N),黑猩猩需要根据力的提示从两个选择刺激中选择一个。实验采用试验-区块程序进行,即在固定次数的试验中呈现相同的力刺激,并在区块之间更换力刺激。试验块的大小从十次试验开始逐渐减少。四只黑猩猩接受了训练,但没有一只达到学习标准(在力刺激随机呈现的条件下,正确率达到 80% 或以上)。然而,对黑猩猩在试块切换前后的表现进行详细分析后发现,它们的选择行为无法用简单的 "赢-留-输 "策略来解释,这表明力刺激的切换影响了黑猩猩的选择行为。研究还发现,黑猩猩在从小力刺激切换到大力刺激时的表现要好于从大力刺激切换到小力刺激时的表现。虽然本研究中没有一只黑猩猩获得了力的辨别能力,但未来使用这种力反馈装置进行的研究将为从比较认知的角度理解非人灵长类的触觉认知提供新的见解。
{"title":"May the force be with you: exploring force discrimination in chimpanzees using the force-feedback device","authors":"Masaki Tomonaga, Yoshihiro Tanaka, Motoyuki Sakai","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01117-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-023-01117-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While force-feedback devices have been developed in areas such as virtual reality, there have been very few comparative cognitive studies in nonhuman animals using these devices. In addition, although cross-modal perception between vision and touch has been actively studied in nonhuman primates for several decades, there have been no studies of their active haptic perception. In this study, we attempted to train force discrimination in chimpanzees using a force-feedback device modified from a trackball. Chimpanzees were given different levels of force feedback (8.0 vs. 0.5 N) when moving the on-screen cursor to the target area by manipulating the trackball and were required to select one of two choice stimuli based on the force cue. The experiment was conducted using a trial-block procedure in which the same force stimulus was presented for a fixed number of trials, and the force stimulus was changed between blocks. The block size was progressively reduced from ten trials. Four chimpanzees were trained, but none reached the learning criterion (80% or more correct responses under the condition that the force stimuli were presented randomly). However, a detailed analysis of the chimpanzees’ performance before and after the trial-block switching revealed that their choice behavior could not be explained by a simple win–stay/lose–shift strategy, suggesting that the switching of the force stimuli affected the chimpanzees’ choice behavior. It was also found that the chimpanzees performed better when switching from small to large force stimuli than when switching from large to small force stimuli. Although none of the chimpanzees in this study acquired force discrimination, future studies using such force-feedback devices will provide new insights for understanding haptic cognition in nonhuman primates from a comparative cognitive perspective.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"59 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139508754","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Food preferences and nutrient composition in captive Southern brown howler monkeys, Alouatta guariba clamitans 人工饲养的南方褐吼猴(Alouatta guariba clamitans)的食物偏好和营养成分
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01109-1
Pamela Silveira, Ícaro William Valler, Zelinda Maria Braga Hirano, Aline Naíssa Dada, Matthias Laska, Laura Teresa Hernandez Salazar

Studies of food preferences in captive primates have so far mainly been restricted to frugivorous species. It was therefore the aim of the present study to assess the occurrence of spontaneous food preferences in a mainly folivorous primate, the captive Southern brown howler monkey, and to analyze whether these preferences correlate with nutrient composition. Using a two-alternative choice test, we presented ten male and five female adult Alouatta guariba clamitans with all possible binary combinations of ten types of food that are part of their diet in captivity and recorded their choice behavior. We found the howler monkeys to display the following rank order of preference: banana > mango > watermelon > papaya > beetroot > apple > pear > orange > cucumber > tomato. This preference ranking significantly and positively correlated with the total carbohydrate content and with the sucrose content of the food items. We also found significant positive correlations between the food preference ranking and the content of the minerals copper and magnesium. Male and female howler monkeys did not differ significantly in their food preference rankings. These results suggest this howler monkeys under human care are not opportunistic, but selective feeders with regard to maximizing their net gain of energy as only the content of carbohydrates, but not the contents of total energy, proteins, or lipids significantly correlated with the displayed food preferences. Thus, the food preferences of this primate are similar to those reported in several species of frugivorous primates tested with cultivated fruits and vegetables.

迄今为止,对圈养灵长类动物食物偏好的研究主要局限于节食物种。因此,本研究的目的是评估圈养南方褐喉猴这种以食叶类为主的灵长类动物的自发食物偏好,并分析这些偏好是否与营养成分有关。我们采用两选一的选择测试方法,向十只雄性和五只雌性成年褐喉猴提供了十种食物的所有可能的二元组合,这些食物是它们圈养食物的一部分,并记录了它们的选择行为。我们发现吼猴的偏好顺序如下:香蕉;芒果;西瓜;木瓜;甜菜根;苹果;梨;橘子;黄瓜;番茄。这种偏好等级与食物的总碳水化合物含量和蔗糖含量呈明显的正相关。我们还发现,食物偏好等级与矿物质铜和镁的含量之间存在明显的正相关。雄性和雌性吼猴在食物偏好排名上没有明显差异。这些结果表明,人类照料下的吼猴并不是机会主义者,而是有选择性地摄取食物,以最大限度地获得能量,因为只有碳水化合物的含量,而不是总能量、蛋白质或脂类的含量与吼猴的食物偏好有明显的相关性。因此,这种灵长类动物的食物偏好与用栽培水果和蔬菜进行测试的几种节食灵长类动物的食物偏好相似。
{"title":"Food preferences and nutrient composition in captive Southern brown howler monkeys, Alouatta guariba clamitans","authors":"Pamela Silveira, Ícaro William Valler, Zelinda Maria Braga Hirano, Aline Naíssa Dada, Matthias Laska, Laura Teresa Hernandez Salazar","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01109-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-023-01109-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies of food preferences in captive primates have so far mainly been restricted to frugivorous species. It was therefore the aim of the present study to assess the occurrence of spontaneous food preferences in a mainly folivorous primate, the captive Southern brown howler monkey, and to analyze whether these preferences correlate with nutrient composition. Using a two-alternative choice test, we presented ten male and five female adult <i>Alouatta guariba clamitans</i> with all possible binary combinations of ten types of food that are part of their diet in captivity and recorded their choice behavior. We found the howler monkeys to display the following rank order of preference: banana &gt; mango &gt; watermelon &gt; papaya &gt; beetroot &gt; apple &gt; pear &gt; orange &gt; cucumber &gt; tomato. This preference ranking significantly and positively correlated with the total carbohydrate content and with the sucrose content of the food items. We also found significant positive correlations between the food preference ranking and the content of the minerals copper and magnesium. Male and female howler monkeys did not differ significantly in their food preference rankings. These results suggest this howler monkeys under human care are not opportunistic, but selective feeders with regard to maximizing their net gain of energy as only the content of carbohydrates, but not the contents of total energy, proteins, or lipids significantly correlated with the displayed food preferences. Thus, the food preferences of this primate are similar to those reported in several species of frugivorous primates tested with cultivated fruits and vegetables.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":"9 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139083429","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Migration of a harem in search of a new leader following the loss of the former adult male leader from a one-male unit of geladas (Theropithecus gelada). 在一只雄性的geladas(Theropithecus gelada)中失去前成年男性首领后,为寻找新首领而进行的后宫迁徙。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01100-w
Zewdu Kifle

Geladas (Theropithecus gelada) live in social groups consisting of one-male units (sometimes referred to as "harems"), bands, and all-male units. Takeover by a new male affects the reproductive success and mating strategies of the individuals in a harem. However, to the best of my knowledge, there is no information available on the fate of the females in a one-male unit whose leader dies or disappears and is not replaced by another adult male. In 2017, I observed the migration of a male-less unit into the home range of my study group at Kosheme, in Wollo, Ethiopia. The unit consisted of 14 individuals. I observed the harem leader of my study group desert his unit and join the new unit, which appeared to peacefully accept him as their leader. These observations are in agreement with information from local residents, who told me that if the leader male of a unit dies, the male-less group (the "survivors") will soon migrate in search of a new adult male leader. To the best of my knowledge, this is the first documented case of the migration of a gelada harem in search of an adult male leader after the loss of the unit's former leader. This report contributes toward a better understanding of the reproductive behavior of geladas in particular, and of primates in general.

盖拉达人(Theropithecus gelada)生活在由一个男性单元(有时被称为“后宫”)、乐队和所有男性单元组成的社会群体中。新雄性的接管会影响后宫中个体的繁殖成功和交配策略。然而,据我所知,没有关于一个男性单位中女性命运的信息,该单位的领导人死亡或失踪,没有被另一名成年男性取代。2017年,我在埃塞俄比亚沃洛的科舍姆观察到一个没有男性的单位迁移到我的研究小组的家中。该单位由14人组成。我观察到我学习小组的后宫首领抛弃了他的部队,加入了新的部队,新的部队似乎平静地接受了他作为他们的首领。这些观察结果与当地居民的信息一致,他们告诉我,如果一个单位的男性领导死亡,那么没有男性的群体(“幸存者”)将很快迁移,寻找新的成年男性领导。据我所知,这是第一个有记录的案例,在该部队失去前领导人后,格拉达后宫为了寻找成年男性领导人而迁移。这份报告有助于更好地了解geladas的繁殖行为,尤其是灵长类动物的繁殖行为。
{"title":"Migration of a harem in search of a new leader following the loss of the former adult male leader from a one-male unit of geladas (Theropithecus gelada).","authors":"Zewdu Kifle","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01100-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01100-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Geladas (Theropithecus gelada) live in social groups consisting of one-male units (sometimes referred to as \"harems\"), bands, and all-male units. Takeover by a new male affects the reproductive success and mating strategies of the individuals in a harem. However, to the best of my knowledge, there is no information available on the fate of the females in a one-male unit whose leader dies or disappears and is not replaced by another adult male. In 2017, I observed the migration of a male-less unit into the home range of my study group at Kosheme, in Wollo, Ethiopia. The unit consisted of 14 individuals. I observed the harem leader of my study group desert his unit and join the new unit, which appeared to peacefully accept him as their leader. These observations are in agreement with information from local residents, who told me that if the leader male of a unit dies, the male-less group (the \"survivors\") will soon migrate in search of a new adult male leader. To the best of my knowledge, this is the first documented case of the migration of a gelada harem in search of an adult male leader after the loss of the unit's former leader. This report contributes toward a better understanding of the reproductive behavior of geladas in particular, and of primates in general.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"21-24"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50162642","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
News and Perspectives: Words matter in primatology. 新闻与展望:词语在灵长类动物学中很重要。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01104-6
Michelle Bezanson, Liliana Cortés-Ortiz, Júlio César Bicca-Marques, Ramesh Boonratana, Susana Carvalho, Marina Cords, Stella de la Torre, Catherine Hobaiter, Tatyana Humle, Patrícia Izar, Jessica W Lynch, Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Joanna M Setchell, Gladys Kalema Zikusoka, Karen B Strier

Postings on social media on Twitter (now X), BioAnthropology News (Facebook), and other venues, as well as recent publications in prominent journals, show that primatologists, ecologists, and other researchers are questioning the terms "Old World" and "New World" due to their colonial implications and history. The terms are offensive if they result in erasing Indigenous voices and history, ignoring the fact that Indigenous peoples were in the Americas long before European colonization. Language use is not without context, but alternative terminology is not always obvious and available. In this perspective, we share opinions expressed by an international group of primatologists who considered questions about the use of these terms, whether primatologists should adjust language use, and how to move forward. The diversity of opinions provides insight into how conventional terms used in primatological research and conservation may impact our effectiveness in these domains.

在Twitter(现在是X)、生物人类学新闻(Facebook)和其他社交媒体上的帖子,以及最近在著名期刊上发表的文章表明,灵长类动物学家、生态学家和其他研究人员正在质疑“旧世界”和“新世界”这两个词,因为它们的殖民含义和历史。如果这些词抹杀了原住民的声音和历史,忽略了原住民早在欧洲殖民之前就生活在美洲的事实,那么这些词就是冒犯性的。语言的使用并非没有上下文,但替代术语并不总是显而易见和可用的。从这个角度来看,我们分享了一个国际灵长类动物学家小组所表达的观点,他们考虑了这些术语的使用问题,灵长类动物学家是否应该调整语言的使用,以及如何向前发展。意见的多样性提供了对灵长类动物学研究和保护中使用的传统术语如何影响我们在这些领域的有效性的见解。
{"title":"News and Perspectives: Words matter in primatology.","authors":"Michelle Bezanson, Liliana Cortés-Ortiz, Júlio César Bicca-Marques, Ramesh Boonratana, Susana Carvalho, Marina Cords, Stella de la Torre, Catherine Hobaiter, Tatyana Humle, Patrícia Izar, Jessica W Lynch, Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Joanna M Setchell, Gladys Kalema Zikusoka, Karen B Strier","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01104-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01104-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Postings on social media on Twitter (now X), BioAnthropology News (Facebook), and other venues, as well as recent publications in prominent journals, show that primatologists, ecologists, and other researchers are questioning the terms \"Old World\" and \"New World\" due to their colonial implications and history. The terms are offensive if they result in erasing Indigenous voices and history, ignoring the fact that Indigenous peoples were in the Americas long before European colonization. Language use is not without context, but alternative terminology is not always obvious and available. In this perspective, we share opinions expressed by an international group of primatologists who considered questions about the use of these terms, whether primatologists should adjust language use, and how to move forward. The diversity of opinions provides insight into how conventional terms used in primatological research and conservation may impact our effectiveness in these domains.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"33-39"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10796633/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138462189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Opportunistic meat-eating by urban folivorous-frugivorous monkeys. 城市食叶食草猴的机会主义肉食行为。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01098-1
Isadora Alves de Lima, Júlio César Bicca-Marques

The consumption of vertebrate tissues and eggs (hereinafter "meat") is relatively common among some primates that are highly frugivorous or eclectic omnivores, but rare or absent in those that are highly folivorous. The Neotropical howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.) belong in the latter group. Here we report the consumption of meat by free-ranging urban black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya) and discuss the potential role of the consumed meat as a source of energy, protein, or micronutrients. We studied three groups of howler monkeys (comprising four to seven individuals), living in city squares (0.6, 1.5, and 1.9 ha) in south Brazil, from July 2022 to May 2023 (65 days; 797 h of observations). All of the study groups were spontaneously supplemented daily by people with variable amounts and types of food provided. Meat was only offered in the two larger squares. The groups' diets included leaves (42-49% scan sampling feeding records), fruit (3-20%), and flowers (2-5%) from 13 to 20 plant species, and considerable amounts of supplemented food (27-50%). We recorded 33 individual events of ingestion of supplemented cooked meat, three individual events of dove egg predation, and three bird nest inspections without egg consumption. All members of the two groups in the larger squares, except an infant male, ingested meat at least once. Meat accounted for 1% of total scan feeding records of both groups with access to this supplement. We conclude that whereas the opportunistic consumption of meat probably contributed only minor amounts of energy and protein to the study subjects, it may have benefitted them with micronutrients that are scarce in plant foods.

脊椎动物组织和蛋(以下简称“肉”)在一些高度食草或兼收并蓄的杂食性灵长类动物中相对常见,但在高度食叶的灵长类动物身上很少或不存在。新热带吼猴(Alouatta spp.)属于后一类。在这里,我们报道了自由放养的城市黑和金吼猴(Alouatta caraya)的肉类消费情况,并讨论了所消费的肉类作为能量、蛋白质或微量营养素来源的潜在作用。我们研究了2022年7月至2023年5月(65天;797小时的观察)期间生活在巴西南部城市广场(0.6、1.5和1.9公顷)的三组吼猴(包括4至7只)。所有研究组每天都由提供不同数量和类型食物的人自发补充。肉只在两个较大的广场上供应。这些组的饮食包括13至20种植物的叶子(42-49%的扫描采样喂养记录)、水果(3-20%)和花朵(2-5%),以及大量的补充食物(27-50%)。我们记录了33起摄入补充熟肉的个体事件,3起捕食鸽子蛋的个体事件和3起未食用鸽子蛋的鸟巢检查。在较大的正方形中,除一名男婴外,两组成员都至少摄入一次肉。在获得这种补充剂的两组中,肉类占扫描喂养记录总数的1%。我们得出的结论是,尽管机会主义的肉类消费可能只为研究对象提供了少量的能量和蛋白质,但它可能使他们受益于植物性食品中稀缺的微量营养素。
{"title":"Opportunistic meat-eating by urban folivorous-frugivorous monkeys.","authors":"Isadora Alves de Lima, Júlio César Bicca-Marques","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01098-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01098-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The consumption of vertebrate tissues and eggs (hereinafter \"meat\") is relatively common among some primates that are highly frugivorous or eclectic omnivores, but rare or absent in those that are highly folivorous. The Neotropical howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.) belong in the latter group. Here we report the consumption of meat by free-ranging urban black and gold howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya) and discuss the potential role of the consumed meat as a source of energy, protein, or micronutrients. We studied three groups of howler monkeys (comprising four to seven individuals), living in city squares (0.6, 1.5, and 1.9 ha) in south Brazil, from July 2022 to May 2023 (65 days; 797 h of observations). All of the study groups were spontaneously supplemented daily by people with variable amounts and types of food provided. Meat was only offered in the two larger squares. The groups' diets included leaves (42-49% scan sampling feeding records), fruit (3-20%), and flowers (2-5%) from 13 to 20 plant species, and considerable amounts of supplemented food (27-50%). We recorded 33 individual events of ingestion of supplemented cooked meat, three individual events of dove egg predation, and three bird nest inspections without egg consumption. All members of the two groups in the larger squares, except an infant male, ingested meat at least once. Meat accounted for 1% of total scan feeding records of both groups with access to this supplement. We conclude that whereas the opportunistic consumption of meat probably contributed only minor amounts of energy and protein to the study subjects, it may have benefitted them with micronutrients that are scarce in plant foods.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"25-32"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49681520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Through cross-disciplinary collaboration. 通过跨学科合作。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-10 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01115-3
Shoji Kawamura
{"title":"Through cross-disciplinary collaboration.","authors":"Shoji Kawamura","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01115-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01115-3","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"7-13"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139404112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct links between resource availability and activity budget better reveal ecological patterns of endangered Coimbra-Filho's titi monkey. 资源可用性和活动预算之间的直接联系更好地揭示了濒危的Coimbra Filho’s titi猴的生态模式。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01095-4
João Pedro Souza-Alves, Renato R Hilário, Isadora P Fontes, William Wayt Thomas, Maria Regina de Vasconcellos Barbosa

Many primatological studies do not assess direct indexes of food availability to make inferences about behavioral strategies. We related the diet and behavior of a group of Callicebus coimbrai in northeastern Brazil to fruit availability indexes and compared this pattern between seasons (direct and indirect assessment of food availability) to assess whether direct and indirect approaches detect similar ecological patterns. We monitored the study group for 33 months (5 days/month) via scan sampling. The monthly availability of fruits and new leaves was recorded in phenological transects. Fruit availability varied across years based on fruit prevalence, and timing and duration of the abundant seasons. We did not find evidence of a time-minimizing strategy, since C. coimbrai did not change its activity levels according to food availability. However, the negative relationship between foraging and fruit availability indicates that C. coimbrai can compensate for the lower fruit availability by increasing the search for alternative food sources. Monthly fruit consumption was positively correlated to fruit availability and negatively related to the consumption of other food items. However, the behavioral and feeding profiles did not vary between seasons and were not related to rainfall levels. Primate studies should directly relate behavioral and feeding profiles to fruit availability indices, thus avoiding using seasons as proxies of food availability.

许多灵长类动物学研究并没有评估食物可获得性的直接指标来推断行为策略。我们将巴西东北部一组柯布丽蝇的饮食和行为与水果可用性指数联系起来,并在不同季节之间比较这种模式(对食物可用性的直接和间接评估),以评估直接和间接方法是否检测到类似的生态模式。我们通过扫描采样对研究组进行了33个月(5天/月)的监测。在酚样带中记录了果实和新叶的月可利用性。不同年份的水果供应量因水果流行率、丰季的时间和持续时间而异。我们没有发现时间最小化策略的证据,因为C.coimbrai不会根据食物供应情况改变其活性水平。然而,觅食与果实可利用性之间的负相关关系表明,C.coimbrai可以通过增加对替代食物来源的搜索来弥补较低的果实可利用率。每月水果消费量与水果供应量呈正相关,与其他食品的消费量呈负相关。然而,行为和进食特征在不同季节之间没有变化,也与降雨量水平无关。灵长类动物的研究应该将行为和进食特征与水果可用性指数直接联系起来,从而避免将季节作为食物可用性的指标。
{"title":"Direct links between resource availability and activity budget better reveal ecological patterns of endangered Coimbra-Filho's titi monkey.","authors":"João Pedro Souza-Alves, Renato R Hilário, Isadora P Fontes, William Wayt Thomas, Maria Regina de Vasconcellos Barbosa","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01095-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01095-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many primatological studies do not assess direct indexes of food availability to make inferences about behavioral strategies. We related the diet and behavior of a group of Callicebus coimbrai in northeastern Brazil to fruit availability indexes and compared this pattern between seasons (direct and indirect assessment of food availability) to assess whether direct and indirect approaches detect similar ecological patterns. We monitored the study group for 33 months (5 days/month) via scan sampling. The monthly availability of fruits and new leaves was recorded in phenological transects. Fruit availability varied across years based on fruit prevalence, and timing and duration of the abundant seasons. We did not find evidence of a time-minimizing strategy, since C. coimbrai did not change its activity levels according to food availability. However, the negative relationship between foraging and fruit availability indicates that C. coimbrai can compensate for the lower fruit availability by increasing the search for alternative food sources. Monthly fruit consumption was positively correlated to fruit availability and negatively related to the consumption of other food items. However, the behavioral and feeding profiles did not vary between seasons and were not related to rainfall levels. Primate studies should directly relate behavioral and feeding profiles to fruit availability indices, thus avoiding using seasons as proxies of food availability.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"49-59"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41150192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A case of suspected chimpanzee scavenging in the Issa Valley, Tanzania. 坦桑尼亚伊萨山谷的一个疑似黑猩猩拾荒的案例。
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 ZOOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10329-023-01099-0
Sam A Baker, Fiona A Stewart, Alex K Piel

Like humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are well known for their vertebrate and invertebrate hunting, but they rarely scavenge. In contrast, while hunting and meat consumption became increasingly important during the evolution of the genus Homo, scavenging meat and marrow from carcasses of large mammals was also likely to be an important component of their subsistence strategies. Here, we describe a confrontational scavenging interaction between an adult male chimpanzee from the Issa Valley and a crowned eagle (Stephanoaetus coronatus), which resulted in the chimpanzee capturing and consuming the carcass of a juvenile bushbuck (Tragelaphus scriptus). We describe the interaction and contextualize this with previous scavenging observations from chimpanzees.

和人类一样,黑猩猩(Pan troglodytes)以捕食脊椎动物和无脊椎动物而闻名,但它们很少觅食。相比之下,尽管狩猎和肉类消费在人属进化过程中变得越来越重要,但从大型哺乳动物尸体上采集肉类和骨髓也可能是它们生存策略的重要组成部分。在这里,我们描述了一只来自伊萨山谷的成年雄性黑猩猩和一只冠鹰(Stephanoaetus coronatus)之间的对抗性觅食互动,这导致黑猩猩捕获并吃掉了一只幼年丛林鹿的尸体(Tragelaphus scriptus)。我们描述了这种相互作用,并将其与黑猩猩先前的拾荒观察联系起来。
{"title":"A case of suspected chimpanzee scavenging in the Issa Valley, Tanzania.","authors":"Sam A Baker, Fiona A Stewart, Alex K Piel","doi":"10.1007/s10329-023-01099-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10329-023-01099-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Like humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are well known for their vertebrate and invertebrate hunting, but they rarely scavenge. In contrast, while hunting and meat consumption became increasingly important during the evolution of the genus Homo, scavenging meat and marrow from carcasses of large mammals was also likely to be an important component of their subsistence strategies. Here, we describe a confrontational scavenging interaction between an adult male chimpanzee from the Issa Valley and a crowned eagle (Stephanoaetus coronatus), which resulted in the chimpanzee capturing and consuming the carcass of a juvenile bushbuck (Tragelaphus scriptus). We describe the interaction and contextualize this with previous scavenging observations from chimpanzees.</p>","PeriodicalId":20468,"journal":{"name":"Primates","volume":" ","pages":"41-48"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71413632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Primates
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1