首页 > 最新文献

Social cognitive and affective neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
The role of the Somatosensory system in the feeling of emotions: a neurostimulation study 体感系统在情绪感受中的作用:一项神经刺激研究
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae062
Michelle Giraud, Amir-Homayoun Javadi, Carmen Lenatti, John Allen, Luigi Tamè, Elena Nava
Emotional experiences deeply impact our bodily states, such as when we feel ‘anger’, our fists close and our face burns. Recent studies have shown that emotions can be mapped onto specific body areas, suggesting a possible role of the primary somatosensory system (S1) in emotion processing. To date, however, the causal role of S1 in emotion generation remains unclear. To address this question, we applied transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on the S1 at different frequencies (beta, theta and sham) while participants saw emotional stimuli with different degrees of pleasantness and level of arousal. Results showed that modulation of S1 influenced subjective emotional ratings as a function of the frequency applied. While theta and beta-tACS made participants rate the emotional images as more pleasant (higher valence), only theta-tACS lowered the subjective arousal ratings (more calming). Skin conductance responses recorded throughout the experiment confirmed a different arousal for pleasant vs unpleasant stimuli. Our study revealed that S1 has a causal role in the feeling of emotions, adding new insight into the embodied nature of emotions. Importantly, we provided causal evidence that beta and theta frequencies contribute differently to the modulation of two dimensions of emotions - arousal and valence - corroborating the view of a dissociation between these two dimensions of emotions.
情绪体验深深地影响着我们的身体状态,例如当我们感到 "愤怒 "时,我们的拳头会紧握,脸会发烫。最近的研究表明,情绪可以映射到特定的身体区域,这表明初级躯体感觉系统(S1)可能在情绪处理中发挥作用。然而,迄今为止,S1 在情绪产生中的因果作用仍不清楚。为了解决这个问题,我们对 S1 施加了不同频率(β、θ 和假)的经颅交变电流刺激(tACS),同时参与者看到了不同愉悦程度和唤醒水平的情绪刺激。结果表明,对 S1 的调制会影响主观情绪评级,这与所应用的频率有关。虽然θ和β-tACS使参与者对情绪图像的评价更愉快(更高的价值),但只有θ-tACS降低了主观唤醒评价(更平静)。在整个实验过程中记录的皮肤传导反应证实,愉快与不愉快的刺激会引起不同的唤醒。我们的研究揭示了 S1 在情绪感受中的因果作用,为情绪的体现本质增添了新的见解。重要的是,我们提供的因果证据表明,贝塔频率和θ频率对情绪的两个维度--唤醒和情绪价值--的调节作用不同,这证实了情绪的这两个维度之间存在分离的观点。
{"title":"The role of the Somatosensory system in the feeling of emotions: a neurostimulation study","authors":"Michelle Giraud, Amir-Homayoun Javadi, Carmen Lenatti, John Allen, Luigi Tamè, Elena Nava","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae062","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae062","url":null,"abstract":"Emotional experiences deeply impact our bodily states, such as when we feel ‘anger’, our fists close and our face burns. Recent studies have shown that emotions can be mapped onto specific body areas, suggesting a possible role of the primary somatosensory system (S1) in emotion processing. To date, however, the causal role of S1 in emotion generation remains unclear. To address this question, we applied transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on the S1 at different frequencies (beta, theta and sham) while participants saw emotional stimuli with different degrees of pleasantness and level of arousal. Results showed that modulation of S1 influenced subjective emotional ratings as a function of the frequency applied. While theta and beta-tACS made participants rate the emotional images as more pleasant (higher valence), only theta-tACS lowered the subjective arousal ratings (more calming). Skin conductance responses recorded throughout the experiment confirmed a different arousal for pleasant vs unpleasant stimuli. Our study revealed that S1 has a causal role in the feeling of emotions, adding new insight into the embodied nature of emotions. Importantly, we provided causal evidence that beta and theta frequencies contribute differently to the modulation of two dimensions of emotions - arousal and valence - corroborating the view of a dissociation between these two dimensions of emotions.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142267742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increased sensitivity to social hierarchy during social competition versus cooperation 在社会竞争与合作过程中,对社会等级的敏感度提高
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae060
Yaner Su, Sander Martens, André Aleman, Jiali Zhou, Pengfei Xu, Yue-jia Luo, Katharina S Goerlich
Social hierarchy is a typical feature of social organization. The ability to quickly recognize social hierarchy information is crucial for adapting to social contexts. Here, we adopted fast periodic visual stimulation (FPVS) with electroencephalography (EEG) to assess the neural responses to social hierarchy during social competition and cooperation, respectively. Participants first learned hierarchical faces from a competitive game versus a cooperative game. We then sequentially presented the learned hierarchical faces with a specific frequency in a set of faces. Results showed that participants rated the inferior player as lower in the social hierarchy in the cooperative context compared to the competitive context, indicating that social context affects the judgment of others’ rank. Moreover, higher neural responses to high and low-hierarchy faces versus medium-hierarchy faces were observed, suggesting rapid discrimination of social hierarchy from faces. Interestingly, rank-specific neural responses were more pronounced in the competitive context than in the cooperative context, indicating increased sensitivity to social hierarchy during social competition versus social cooperation. This study provides behavioral and neural evidence for rapid, automatic processing of social hierarchy information and for an increased sensitivity to such information in competitive versus cooperative social contexts.
社会等级是社会组织的一个典型特征。快速识别社会等级信息的能力对于适应社会环境至关重要。在这里,我们采用快速周期性视觉刺激(FPVS)和脑电图(EEG)分别评估了在社会竞争和合作过程中神经系统对社会等级的反应。受试者首先从竞争游戏与合作游戏中学习了等级面孔。然后,我们在一组面孔中以特定频率依次呈现所学的等级面孔。结果显示,与竞争情境相比,在合作情境中,被试对劣势玩家的社会等级评价较低,这表明社会情境会影响对他人等级的判断。此外,与中等等级的面孔相比,高等级和低等级面孔的神经反应更高,这表明面孔能快速分辨社会等级。有趣的是,在竞争情境中,等级特异性神经反应比在合作情境中更明显,这表明在社会竞争与社会合作中,对社会等级的敏感性增加了。这项研究提供了行为和神经证据,证明了在竞争与合作的社会情境中,社会等级信息的快速、自动处理以及对此类信息的敏感性的提高。
{"title":"Increased sensitivity to social hierarchy during social competition versus cooperation","authors":"Yaner Su, Sander Martens, André Aleman, Jiali Zhou, Pengfei Xu, Yue-jia Luo, Katharina S Goerlich","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae060","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae060","url":null,"abstract":"Social hierarchy is a typical feature of social organization. The ability to quickly recognize social hierarchy information is crucial for adapting to social contexts. Here, we adopted fast periodic visual stimulation (FPVS) with electroencephalography (EEG) to assess the neural responses to social hierarchy during social competition and cooperation, respectively. Participants first learned hierarchical faces from a competitive game versus a cooperative game. We then sequentially presented the learned hierarchical faces with a specific frequency in a set of faces. Results showed that participants rated the inferior player as lower in the social hierarchy in the cooperative context compared to the competitive context, indicating that social context affects the judgment of others’ rank. Moreover, higher neural responses to high and low-hierarchy faces versus medium-hierarchy faces were observed, suggesting rapid discrimination of social hierarchy from faces. Interestingly, rank-specific neural responses were more pronounced in the competitive context than in the cooperative context, indicating increased sensitivity to social hierarchy during social competition versus social cooperation. This study provides behavioral and neural evidence for rapid, automatic processing of social hierarchy information and for an increased sensitivity to such information in competitive versus cooperative social contexts.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142215895","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exposure to Community Violence as a Mechanism Linking Neighborhood Socioeconomic Disadvantage and Neural Responses to Reward 暴露于社区暴力是连接邻里社会经济劣势和奖赏神经反应的机制
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae029
Heidi B Westerman, Gabriela L Suarez, Leah S Richmond-Rakerd, Robin Nusslock, Kelly L Klump, S Alexandra Burt, Luke W Hyde
A growing literature links socioeconomic disadvantage and adversity to brain function, including disruptions in reward processing. Less research has examined exposure to community violence as a specific adversity related to differences in reward-related brain activation, despite the prevalence of community violence exposure for those living in disadvantaged contexts. The current study tested whether exposure to community violence was associated with reward-related ventral striatum activation after accounting for familial factors associated with differences in reward-related activation (e.g., parenting, family income). Moreover, we tested whether exposure to community violence is a mechanism linking socioeconomic disadvantage to reward-related activation in the ventral striatum. We utilized data from 444 adolescent twins sampled from birth records and residing in neighborhoods with above-average levels of poverty. Exposure to community violence was associated with greater reward-related ventral striatum activation, and the association remained after accounting for family-level markers of disadvantage. We identified an indirect pathway in which socioeconomic disadvantage predicted greater reward-related activation via greater exposure to community violence, over and above family-level adversity. These findings highlight the unique impact of community violence exposure on reward processing and provide a mechanism through which socioeconomic disadvantage may shape brain function.
越来越多的文献将社会经济劣势和逆境与大脑功能(包括奖赏处理过程中的干扰)联系起来。尽管生活在不利环境中的人普遍遭受社区暴力,但将遭受社区暴力作为与奖赏相关的大脑激活差异有关的特定逆境的研究较少。本研究测试了在考虑了与奖赏相关激活差异有关的家庭因素(如养育、家庭收入)后,社区暴力暴露是否与奖赏相关的腹侧纹状体激活有关。此外,我们还检验了暴露于社区暴力是否是社会经济劣势与腹侧纹状体奖赏相关激活的一种关联机制。我们利用了从出生记录中抽取的、居住在贫困水平高于平均水平的社区的 444 对青少年双胞胎的数据。暴露于社区暴力与更大的奖赏相关腹侧纹状体激活有关,在考虑了家庭层面的劣势标记后,这种关联仍然存在。我们发现了一个间接的途径,即社会经济劣势通过更多的社区暴力暴露来预测更高的奖赏相关激活,而不是家庭层面的逆境。这些发现凸显了接触社区暴力对奖赏处理的独特影响,并提供了一种社会经济劣势可能影响大脑功能的机制。
{"title":"Exposure to Community Violence as a Mechanism Linking Neighborhood Socioeconomic Disadvantage and Neural Responses to Reward","authors":"Heidi B Westerman, Gabriela L Suarez, Leah S Richmond-Rakerd, Robin Nusslock, Kelly L Klump, S Alexandra Burt, Luke W Hyde","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae029","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae029","url":null,"abstract":"A growing literature links socioeconomic disadvantage and adversity to brain function, including disruptions in reward processing. Less research has examined exposure to community violence as a specific adversity related to differences in reward-related brain activation, despite the prevalence of community violence exposure for those living in disadvantaged contexts. The current study tested whether exposure to community violence was associated with reward-related ventral striatum activation after accounting for familial factors associated with differences in reward-related activation (e.g., parenting, family income). Moreover, we tested whether exposure to community violence is a mechanism linking socioeconomic disadvantage to reward-related activation in the ventral striatum. We utilized data from 444 adolescent twins sampled from birth records and residing in neighborhoods with above-average levels of poverty. Exposure to community violence was associated with greater reward-related ventral striatum activation, and the association remained after accounting for family-level markers of disadvantage. We identified an indirect pathway in which socioeconomic disadvantage predicted greater reward-related activation via greater exposure to community violence, over and above family-level adversity. These findings highlight the unique impact of community violence exposure on reward processing and provide a mechanism through which socioeconomic disadvantage may shape brain function.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140565036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The SocialVidStim: a video database of positive and negative social evaluation stimuli for use in social cognitive neuroscience paradigms SocialVidStim:用于社会认知神经科学范式的正面和负面社会评价刺激视频数据库
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae024
Laura M Tully, Mary Blendermann, Jeffrey R Fine, Lauren N Zakskorn, Matilda Fritz, Gabriella E Hamlett, Shannon T Lamb, Anna K Moody, Julenne Ng, Narimes Parakul, Bryn M Ritter, Raisa Rahim, Grace Yu, Sandra L Taylor
This paper describes the SocialVidStim—a database of video stimuli available to the scientific community depicting positive and negative social evaluative and neutral statements. The SocialVidStim comprises 53 diverse individuals reflecting the demographic makeup of the USA, ranging from 9 to 41 years old, saying 20–60 positive and 20–60 negative social evaluative statements (e.g. ‘You are a very trustworthy/annoying person’), and 20–60 neutral statements (e.g. ‘The sky is blue’), totaling 5793 videos post-production. The SocialVidStim are designed for use in behavioral and functional magetic resonance imaging paradigms, across developmental stages, in diverse populations. This study describes stimuli development and reports initial validity and reliability data on a subset videos (N = 1890) depicting individuals aged 18–41 years. Raters perceive videos as expected: positive videos elicit positively valenced ratings, negative videos elicit negatively valenced ratings and neutral videos are rated as neutral. Test–retest reliability data demonstrate intraclass correlations in the good-to-excellent range for negative and positive videos and the moderate range for neutral videos. We also report small effects on valence and arousal that should be considered during stimuli selection, including match between rater and actor sex and actor believability. The SocialVidStim is a resource for researchers and we offer suggestions for using the SocialVidStim in future research.
本文介绍了 SocialVidStim--一个可供科学界使用的视频刺激数据库,其中描述了正面和负面的社会评价和中性语句。SocialVidStim 由 53 名反映美国人口构成的不同个体组成,年龄从 9 岁到 41 岁不等,他们分别说了 20-60 句正面和 20-60 句负面的社会评价语句(如 "你是一个非常值得信赖/讨厌的人"),以及 20-60 句中性语句(如 "天空是蓝色的"),共制作了 5793 个视频。SocialVidStim 设计用于不同人群不同发育阶段的行为和功能磁共振成像范例。本研究介绍了刺激物的开发,并报告了描述 18-41 岁个体的子集视频(N = 1890)的初步有效性和可靠性数据。评分者对视频的感知符合预期:积极的视频会引起积极的评价,消极的视频会引起消极的评价,中性的视频会被评为中性。测试-再测可靠性数据表明,负面和正面视频的类内相关性在良好-优秀范围内,中性视频的类内相关性在中等范围内。我们还报告了在选择刺激物时应考虑的对情绪和唤醒的微小影响,包括评分者和演员性别之间的匹配以及演员的可信度。SocialVidStim 是研究人员的一种资源,我们为在未来研究中使用 SocialVidStim 提出了建议。
{"title":"The SocialVidStim: a video database of positive and negative social evaluation stimuli for use in social cognitive neuroscience paradigms","authors":"Laura M Tully, Mary Blendermann, Jeffrey R Fine, Lauren N Zakskorn, Matilda Fritz, Gabriella E Hamlett, Shannon T Lamb, Anna K Moody, Julenne Ng, Narimes Parakul, Bryn M Ritter, Raisa Rahim, Grace Yu, Sandra L Taylor","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae024","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae024","url":null,"abstract":"This paper describes the SocialVidStim—a database of video stimuli available to the scientific community depicting positive and negative social evaluative and neutral statements. The SocialVidStim comprises 53 diverse individuals reflecting the demographic makeup of the USA, ranging from 9 to 41 years old, saying 20–60 positive and 20–60 negative social evaluative statements (e.g. ‘You are a very trustworthy/annoying person’), and 20–60 neutral statements (e.g. ‘The sky is blue’), totaling 5793 videos post-production. The SocialVidStim are designed for use in behavioral and functional magetic resonance imaging paradigms, across developmental stages, in diverse populations. This study describes stimuli development and reports initial validity and reliability data on a subset videos (N = 1890) depicting individuals aged 18–41 years. Raters perceive videos as expected: positive videos elicit positively valenced ratings, negative videos elicit negatively valenced ratings and neutral videos are rated as neutral. Test–retest reliability data demonstrate intraclass correlations in the good-to-excellent range for negative and positive videos and the moderate range for neutral videos. We also report small effects on valence and arousal that should be considered during stimuli selection, including match between rater and actor sex and actor believability. The SocialVidStim is a resource for researchers and we offer suggestions for using the SocialVidStim in future research.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140564912","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disrupted cognitive and affective empathy network interactions in autistic children viewing social animation 自闭症儿童观看社交动画时的认知和情感共鸣网络互动受到干扰
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae028
Xinrong Guo, Chuanyong Xu, Jierong Chen, Zhiliu Wu, Shumeng Hou, Zhen Wei
Empathy can be divided into two core components, cognitive empathy (CE) and affective empathy (AE), mediated by distinct neural networks. Deficient empathy is a central feature of autism spectrum conditions (ASC), but it is unclear if this deficit results from disruption solely within empathy networks or from disrupted functional integration between cognitive and affective empathy networks. To address this issue, we measured functional connectivity (FC) patterns both within and between empathy networks in autistic children (4–8 years, n = 31) and matched typically developing (TD) children (n = 26) using near-infrared spectroscopy during presentation of an animated story evoking CE and AE. Empathy and social communication ability were also assessed using the Empathy Quotient/Systemizing Quotient (EQ/SQ) and Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), respectively. The results showed that the FC in the AE network of autistic children did not differ from the TD group across conditions; however, ASC group showed weaker FC in the CE network under the CE condition and weaker FC between networks when processing AE information, the latter of which was negatively correlated with EQ scores in ASC. The empathy defect in ASC may involve abnormal integration of CE and AE network activities under AE condition.
移情可分为认知移情(CE)和情感移情(AE)两个核心部分,由不同的神经网络介导。共情缺失是自闭症谱系病(ASC)的一个核心特征,但目前还不清楚这种缺失是仅仅由于共情网络内部的紊乱还是认知共情网络和情感共情网络之间的功能整合紊乱所致。为了解决这个问题,我们使用近红外光谱仪测量了自闭症儿童(4-8 岁,n = 31)和匹配的典型发育(TD)儿童(n = 26)在播放唤起共情和情感共鸣的动画故事时共情网络内部和之间的功能连接(FC)模式。此外,还分别使用移情商数/系统化商数(EQ/SQ)和社会反应量表(SRS)对移情和社会交流能力进行了评估。结果显示,自闭症儿童AE网络中的FC在不同条件下与TD组没有差异;但ASC组在CE条件下CE网络中的FC较弱,在处理AE信息时网络间的FC也较弱,后者与ASC的EQ评分呈负相关。ASC的移情缺陷可能与AE条件下CE和AE网络活动的异常整合有关。
{"title":"Disrupted cognitive and affective empathy network interactions in autistic children viewing social animation","authors":"Xinrong Guo, Chuanyong Xu, Jierong Chen, Zhiliu Wu, Shumeng Hou, Zhen Wei","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae028","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae028","url":null,"abstract":"Empathy can be divided into two core components, cognitive empathy (CE) and affective empathy (AE), mediated by distinct neural networks. Deficient empathy is a central feature of autism spectrum conditions (ASC), but it is unclear if this deficit results from disruption solely within empathy networks or from disrupted functional integration between cognitive and affective empathy networks. To address this issue, we measured functional connectivity (FC) patterns both within and between empathy networks in autistic children (4–8 years, n = 31) and matched typically developing (TD) children (n = 26) using near-infrared spectroscopy during presentation of an animated story evoking CE and AE. Empathy and social communication ability were also assessed using the Empathy Quotient/Systemizing Quotient (EQ/SQ) and Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), respectively. The results showed that the FC in the AE network of autistic children did not differ from the TD group across conditions; however, ASC group showed weaker FC in the CE network under the CE condition and weaker FC between networks when processing AE information, the latter of which was negatively correlated with EQ scores in ASC. The empathy defect in ASC may involve abnormal integration of CE and AE network activities under AE condition.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140565220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mothers’ and Fathers’ Neural Responses Toward Gender-Stereotype Violations by their Own Children 母亲和父亲对自己子女违反性别陈规定型观念的神经反应
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae025
Christel M Portengen, Anneloes L van Baar, Joyce J Endendijk
Gender stereotypes facilitate people’s processing of social information by providing assumptions about expected behaviors and preferences. When gendered expectations are violated, people often respond negatively, both on a behavioral and neural level. Little is known about the impact of family kinship on the behavioral and neural reactions to gender-stereotype violations. Therefore, we examined whether parents show different responses when gender stereotypes are violated by their own children versus unknown children. The sample comprised 74 Dutch families with a father (Mage = 37.54), mother (Mage = 35.83), son and daughter aged 3-6 years. Electro-encephalography (EEG) measurements were obtained while parents viewed pictures of their own and unknown children paired with toy or problem behavior words that violated or confirmed gender stereotypes. In half of the trials, parents evaluated the appropriateness of toy-gender and behavior-gender combinations. Parents showed stronger LPP amplitudes toward gender stereotype-violating behaviors by own children compared to unknown children. Moreover, parents’ P1 responses toward gender stereotype-violating child behaviors were stronger for boys than for girls, and for parents who evaluated gender-stereotype violations as less appropriate than gender-stereotype confirmations. The findings indicated that gender-stereotype violations by parents’ own children are particularly salient and viewed as less appropriate than gender-stereotype confirmations.
性别刻板印象通过提供对预期行为和偏好的假设,促进人们对社会信息的处理。当性别期望被违背时,人们往往会在行为和神经层面上做出负面反应。关于家庭亲缘关系对违反性别陈规定型的行为和神经反应的影响,人们知之甚少。因此,我们研究了当自己的孩子和不认识的孩子违反性别刻板印象时,父母是否会表现出不同的反应。样本包括 74 个荷兰家庭,父亲(Mage = 37.54)、母亲(Mage = 35.83)、儿子和女儿的年龄均为 3-6 岁。父母在观看自己孩子和未知孩子的照片时,脑电图(EEG)测量结果与违反或证实性别刻板印象的玩具或问题行为词语相匹配。在一半的试验中,家长评估了玩具-性别和行为-性别组合的适当性。与未知儿童相比,家长对自己孩子违反性别刻板印象的行为表现出更强的 LPP 振幅。此外,对于违反性别刻板印象的儿童行为,男孩家长的 P1 反应强于女孩家长,而对于违反性别刻板印象的行为,家长的 P1 反应强于确认性别刻板印象的行为。研究结果表明,与确认性别陈规定型观念相比,父母自己的孩子违反性别陈规定型观念的行为尤为突出,而且被认为不那么恰当。
{"title":"Mothers’ and Fathers’ Neural Responses Toward Gender-Stereotype Violations by their Own Children","authors":"Christel M Portengen, Anneloes L van Baar, Joyce J Endendijk","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae025","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae025","url":null,"abstract":"Gender stereotypes facilitate people’s processing of social information by providing assumptions about expected behaviors and preferences. When gendered expectations are violated, people often respond negatively, both on a behavioral and neural level. Little is known about the impact of family kinship on the behavioral and neural reactions to gender-stereotype violations. Therefore, we examined whether parents show different responses when gender stereotypes are violated by their own children versus unknown children. The sample comprised 74 Dutch families with a father (Mage = 37.54), mother (Mage = 35.83), son and daughter aged 3-6 years. Electro-encephalography (EEG) measurements were obtained while parents viewed pictures of their own and unknown children paired with toy or problem behavior words that violated or confirmed gender stereotypes. In half of the trials, parents evaluated the appropriateness of toy-gender and behavior-gender combinations. Parents showed stronger LPP amplitudes toward gender stereotype-violating behaviors by own children compared to unknown children. Moreover, parents’ P1 responses toward gender stereotype-violating child behaviors were stronger for boys than for girls, and for parents who evaluated gender-stereotype violations as less appropriate than gender-stereotype confirmations. The findings indicated that gender-stereotype violations by parents’ own children are particularly salient and viewed as less appropriate than gender-stereotype confirmations.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140564788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Practical Guide to EEG Hyperscanning in Joint Action Research: From Motivation to Implementation 联合行动研究中的脑电图超扫描实用指南》:从动机到实施
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae026
Anna Zamm, Janeen D Loehr, Cordula Vesper, Ivana Konvalinka, Simon L Kappel, Ole A Heggli, Peter Vuust, Peter E Keller
Developments in cognitive neuroscience have led to the emergence of hyperscanning, the simultaneous measurement of brain activity from multiple people. Hyperscanning is useful for investigating social cognition, including joint action, because of its ability to capture neural processes that occur within and between people as they coordinate actions toward a shared goal. Here, we provide a practical guide for researchers considering using hyperscanning to study joint action and seeking to avoid frequently raised concerns from hyperscanning skeptics. We focus specifically on EEG hyperscanning, which is widely available and optimally suited for capturing fine-grained temporal dynamics of action coordination. Our guidelines cover questions that are likely to arise when planning a hyperscanning project, ranging from whether hyperscanning is appropriate for answering one’s research questions to considerations for study design, dependent variable selection, data analysis, and visualization. By following clear guidelines that facilitate careful consideration of the theoretical implications of research design choices and other methodological decisions, joint action researchers can mitigate interpretability issues and maximize the benefits of hyperscanning paradigms.
认知神经科学的发展带动了超扫描技术的出现,即同时测量多人的大脑活动。由于超扫描能够捕捉人与人之间为实现共同目标而协调行动时发生的神经过程,因此对研究包括联合行动在内的社会认知非常有用。在此,我们将为考虑使用超扫描研究联合行动的研究人员提供实用指南,以避免超扫描怀疑论者经常提出的顾虑。我们特别关注脑电图超扫描,因为它可以广泛使用,而且非常适合捕捉行动协调的细粒度时间动态。我们的指南涵盖了在规划超级扫描项目时可能出现的问题,从超级扫描是否适合回答研究问题到研究设计、因变量选择、数据分析和可视化等方面的考虑。通过遵循明确的指导方针,仔细考虑研究设计选择和其他方法决策的理论意义,联合行动研究人员可以减少可解释性问题,最大限度地发挥超扫描范式的优势。
{"title":"A Practical Guide to EEG Hyperscanning in Joint Action Research: From Motivation to Implementation","authors":"Anna Zamm, Janeen D Loehr, Cordula Vesper, Ivana Konvalinka, Simon L Kappel, Ole A Heggli, Peter Vuust, Peter E Keller","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae026","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae026","url":null,"abstract":"Developments in cognitive neuroscience have led to the emergence of hyperscanning, the simultaneous measurement of brain activity from multiple people. Hyperscanning is useful for investigating social cognition, including joint action, because of its ability to capture neural processes that occur within and between people as they coordinate actions toward a shared goal. Here, we provide a practical guide for researchers considering using hyperscanning to study joint action and seeking to avoid frequently raised concerns from hyperscanning skeptics. We focus specifically on EEG hyperscanning, which is widely available and optimally suited for capturing fine-grained temporal dynamics of action coordination. Our guidelines cover questions that are likely to arise when planning a hyperscanning project, ranging from whether hyperscanning is appropriate for answering one’s research questions to considerations for study design, dependent variable selection, data analysis, and visualization. By following clear guidelines that facilitate careful consideration of the theoretical implications of research design choices and other methodological decisions, joint action researchers can mitigate interpretability issues and maximize the benefits of hyperscanning paradigms.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140565102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Support and Fear-inhibition: An Examination of Underlying Neural Mechanisms 社会支持与恐惧抑制:对潜在神经机制的研究
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsae002
E.A Hornstein, C.J Leschak, M.H Parrish, K.E Byrne-Haltom, M.S Fanselow, M.G Craske, N.I Eisenberger
Recent work has demonstrated that reminders of those we are closest to have a unique combination of effects on fear learning and represent a new category of fear inhibitors, termed prepared fear suppressors. Notably, social-support-figure images have been shown to resist becoming associated with fear, suppress conditional-fear-responding, and lead to long-term fear reduction. Due to the novelty of this category, understanding the underlying neural mechanisms that support these unique abilities of social-support-reminders has yet to be investigated. Here, we examined the neural correlates that enable social-support-reminders to resist becoming associated with fear during a retardation-of-acquisition test. We found that social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images) were less readily associated with fear, replicating prior work, and that this effect was associated with decreased amygdala activity and increased ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) activity for social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images), suggesting that social-support-engagement of the VMPFC and consequent inhibition of the amygdala may contribute to unique their inhibitory effects. Connectivity analyses supported this interpretation, showing greater connectivity between the VMPFC and left amygdala for social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images).
最近的研究表明,与我们最亲近的人的提醒对恐惧学习具有独特的综合效应,是一种新的恐惧抑制剂,被称为有准备的恐惧抑制剂。值得注意的是,社会支持形象已被证明不会与恐惧联系在一起,能抑制条件性恐惧反应,并导致长期的恐惧减少。由于这一类别的新颖性,人们对支持社会支持-提醒的这些独特能力的潜在神经机制的了解还有待研究。在这里,我们研究了使社会支持记忆在习得迟缓测试中不与恐惧相关联的神经相关因素。我们发现,社会支持图像(与陌生人图像相比)更不容易与恐惧联系在一起,这与之前的研究结果相同,而且这种效应与社会支持图像(与陌生人图像相比)的杏仁核活动减少和腹侧前额叶皮层(VMPFC)活动增加有关,这表明社会支持图像与 VMPFC 的联系以及由此产生的对杏仁核的抑制可能有助于产生独特的抑制作用。连通性分析也支持这一解释,分析结果显示,对于社会支持图像(与陌生人图像相比),VMPFC 和左侧杏仁核之间的连通性更高。
{"title":"Social Support and Fear-inhibition: An Examination of Underlying Neural Mechanisms","authors":"E.A Hornstein, C.J Leschak, M.H Parrish, K.E Byrne-Haltom, M.S Fanselow, M.G Craske, N.I Eisenberger","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsae002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsae002","url":null,"abstract":"Recent work has demonstrated that reminders of those we are closest to have a unique combination of effects on fear learning and represent a new category of fear inhibitors, termed prepared fear suppressors. Notably, social-support-figure images have been shown to resist becoming associated with fear, suppress conditional-fear-responding, and lead to long-term fear reduction. Due to the novelty of this category, understanding the underlying neural mechanisms that support these unique abilities of social-support-reminders has yet to be investigated. Here, we examined the neural correlates that enable social-support-reminders to resist becoming associated with fear during a retardation-of-acquisition test. We found that social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images) were less readily associated with fear, replicating prior work, and that this effect was associated with decreased amygdala activity and increased ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) activity for social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images), suggesting that social-support-engagement of the VMPFC and consequent inhibition of the amygdala may contribute to unique their inhibitory effects. Connectivity analyses supported this interpretation, showing greater connectivity between the VMPFC and left amygdala for social-support-figure-images (vs. stranger-images).","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139459122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No Changes in Triple Network Engagement Following (Combined) Noradrenergic and Glucocorticoid Stimulation in Healthy Men 对健康男性进行去甲肾上腺素能和糖皮质激素(联合)刺激后,三重网络参与没有变化
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad073
Renée Lipka, Catarina Rosada, Sophie Metz, Julian Hellmann-Regen, Hauke Heekeren, Katja Wingenfeld
Successful recovery from stress is integral for adaptive responding to the environment. At a cellular level, this involves (slow genomic) actions of cortisol, which alter or reverse rapid effects of noradrenaline and cortisol associated with acute stress. At the network scale, stress recovery is less well understood but assumed to involve changes within salience-, executive control-, and default mode networks. To date, few studies have investigated this phase and directly tested these assumptions. Here we present results from a double-blind, placebo-controlled, between-groups paradigm (N =165 healthy males) administering 10 mg oral yohimbine and/or 10 mg oral hydrocortisone two hours prior to resting state scanning. We found no changes in within network connectivity of the three networks, both after single and combined drug administration. We further report the results of Bayesian parameter inference to provide evidence for the null hypothesis. Our results contrast with previous findings, which may be attributable to systematic differences between paradigms, highlighting the need to isolate paradigm-specific effects from those related to stress.
成功从压力中恢复是对环境做出适应性反应的必要条件。在细胞层面,这涉及皮质醇的(缓慢基因组)作用,它改变或逆转了与急性压力相关的去甲肾上腺素和皮质醇的快速效应。在网络层面,人们对压力恢复的了解较少,但认为它涉及显著性网络、执行控制网络和默认模式网络的变化。迄今为止,很少有研究对这一阶段进行调查并直接测试这些假设。在此,我们展示了一项双盲、安慰剂对照、组间范例研究(N = 165 名健康男性)的结果,研究人员在静息状态扫描前两小时口服了 10 毫克育亨宾和/或 10 毫克氢化可的松。我们发现,无论是单次用药还是联合用药,三个网络内部的连通性都没有发生变化。我们进一步报告了贝叶斯参数推断的结果,为零假设提供了证据。我们的研究结果与之前的研究结果形成了鲜明对比,这可能是由于不同范式之间的系统性差异造成的,突出了将特定范式效应与应激相关效应分离开来的必要性。
{"title":"No Changes in Triple Network Engagement Following (Combined) Noradrenergic and Glucocorticoid Stimulation in Healthy Men","authors":"Renée Lipka, Catarina Rosada, Sophie Metz, Julian Hellmann-Regen, Hauke Heekeren, Katja Wingenfeld","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad073","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsad073","url":null,"abstract":"Successful recovery from stress is integral for adaptive responding to the environment. At a cellular level, this involves (slow genomic) actions of cortisol, which alter or reverse rapid effects of noradrenaline and cortisol associated with acute stress. At the network scale, stress recovery is less well understood but assumed to involve changes within salience-, executive control-, and default mode networks. To date, few studies have investigated this phase and directly tested these assumptions. Here we present results from a double-blind, placebo-controlled, between-groups paradigm (N =165 healthy males) administering 10 mg oral yohimbine and/or 10 mg oral hydrocortisone two hours prior to resting state scanning. We found no changes in within network connectivity of the three networks, both after single and combined drug administration. We further report the results of Bayesian parameter inference to provide evidence for the null hypothesis. Our results contrast with previous findings, which may be attributable to systematic differences between paradigms, highlighting the need to isolate paradigm-specific effects from those related to stress.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138825368","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental Emotionality is Related to Preschool Children’s Neural Responses to Emotional Faces 父母的情绪与学龄前儿童对情绪面孔的神经反应有关
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad078
Ruohan Xia, Megan J Heise, Lindsay C Bowman
The ability to accurately decode others’ facial expressions is essential for successful social interaction. Previous theories suggest that aspects of parental emotionality—the frequency, persistence, and intensity of parents’ own emotions—can influence children’s emotion perception. Through a combination of mechanisms, parental emotionality may shape how children’s brains specialize to respond to emotional expressions, but empirical data is lacking. The present study provides a direct empirical test of the relation between the intensity, persistence, and frequency of parents’ own emotions and children’s neural responses to perceiving emotional expressions. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded as typically developing 3 to 5-year-old children (final Ns = 59 and 50) passively viewed faces expressing different emotional valences (happy, angry, fearful) at full and reduced intensity (100% intense expression, 40% intense expression). We examined relations between parental emotionality (as self-reported on the Nock et al., 2008 Emotion Reactivity Scale) and children’s mean amplitude ERP N170 and Nc responses. Findings demonstrate a clear relation between parental emotionality and children’s neural responses (in the N170 mean amplitude and latency) to emotional expressions and suggest that parents may influence children’s emotion-processing neural circuitry.
准确解读他人面部表情的能力对于成功的社会交往至关重要。以往的理论认为,父母情绪化的各个方面--父母自身情绪的频率、持续性和强度--会影响儿童的情绪感知。通过多种机制的结合,父母的情绪化可能会影响儿童大脑对情绪表达的反应,但目前还缺乏实证数据。本研究对父母自身情绪的强度、持续性和频率与儿童感知情绪表达的神经反应之间的关系进行了直接的实证检验。当发育正常的 3 至 5 岁儿童(最终人数分别为 59 和 50)被动地观看表达不同情绪价值(快乐、愤怒、恐惧)的脸部表情时,我们记录了事件相关电位(ERPs)的全部强度和减弱强度(100% 强烈表达、40% 强烈表达)。我们研究了父母的情绪性(根据诺克等人 2008 年的情绪反应量表自我报告)与儿童ERP N170 和 Nc 反应平均振幅之间的关系。研究结果表明,父母的情绪性与儿童对情绪表达的神经反应(N170 平均振幅和潜伏期)之间存在明显的关系,并表明父母可能会影响儿童的情绪处理神经回路。
{"title":"Parental Emotionality is Related to Preschool Children’s Neural Responses to Emotional Faces","authors":"Ruohan Xia, Megan J Heise, Lindsay C Bowman","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad078","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsad078","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to accurately decode others’ facial expressions is essential for successful social interaction. Previous theories suggest that aspects of parental emotionality—the frequency, persistence, and intensity of parents’ own emotions—can influence children’s emotion perception. Through a combination of mechanisms, parental emotionality may shape how children’s brains specialize to respond to emotional expressions, but empirical data is lacking. The present study provides a direct empirical test of the relation between the intensity, persistence, and frequency of parents’ own emotions and children’s neural responses to perceiving emotional expressions. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded as typically developing 3 to 5-year-old children (final Ns = 59 and 50) passively viewed faces expressing different emotional valences (happy, angry, fearful) at full and reduced intensity (100% intense expression, 40% intense expression). We examined relations between parental emotionality (as self-reported on the Nock et al., 2008 Emotion Reactivity Scale) and children’s mean amplitude ERP N170 and Nc responses. Findings demonstrate a clear relation between parental emotionality and children’s neural responses (in the N170 mean amplitude and latency) to emotional expressions and suggest that parents may influence children’s emotion-processing neural circuitry.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138826929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social cognitive and affective neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1