首页 > 最新文献

Social cognitive and affective neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
The vocal side of empathy: neural correlates of pain perception in spoken complaints 感同身受的声音:口语抱怨中疼痛感知的神经相关性
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-12 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad075
Maël Mauchand, Jorge L Armony, Marc D Pell
In the extensive neuroimaging literature on empathy for pain, few studies have investigated how this phenomenon may relate to everyday social situations such as spoken interactions. The present study used fMRI to assess how complaints, as vocal expressions of pain, are empathically processed by listeners and how these empathic responses may vary based on speakers’ vocal expression and cultural identity. Twenty-four French participants listened to short utterances describing a painful event, that were either produced in a neutral-sounding or complaining voice by both ingroup (French) and outgroup (French-Canadian) speakers. Results suggest that the perception of suffering from a complaining voice increased activity in the Emotional Voice Areas (EVAs), composed of voice-sensitive temporal regions interacting with prefrontal cortices and the amygdala. The Salience and Theory of Mind networks, associated with affective and cognitive aspects of empathy, also showed prosody-related activity and specifically correlated with behavioral evaluations of suffering by listeners. Complaints produced by in- vs. outgroup speakers elicited sensorimotor and default-mode activity, respectively, suggesting accent-based changes in empathic perspective. These results, while reaffirming the role of key networks in tasks involving empathy, highlight the importance of vocal expression information and social categorization processes when perceiving another’s suffering during social interactions.
在有关对疼痛的移情的大量神经影像文献中,很少有研究探讨这种现象如何与日常社交场合(如口语互动)相关联。本研究利用 fMRI 评估了作为痛苦的声音表达的抱怨是如何被听者共情处理的,以及这些共情反应是如何根据说话者的声音表达和文化身份而变化的。二十四名法国受试者聆听了描述痛苦事件的简短话语,这些话语是由本群体(法语)和外群体(法裔加拿大人)的说话者以中性声音或抱怨的声音发出的。研究结果表明,对抱怨声音的痛苦感知增加了情感声音区(EVAs)的活动,情感声音区由对声音敏感的颞叶区域与前额叶皮层和杏仁核相互作用组成。与移情的情感和认知方面相关的 "显著性 "和 "心智理论 "网络也显示出与前音相关的活动,并与听者对痛苦的行为评估特别相关。同组和异组说话者的抱怨分别引起了感觉运动和默认模式活动,这表明移情视角中基于口音的变化。这些结果再次证实了关键网络在移情任务中的作用,同时强调了在社会交往中感知他人痛苦时,声音表达信息和社会分类过程的重要性。
{"title":"The vocal side of empathy: neural correlates of pain perception in spoken complaints","authors":"Maël Mauchand, Jorge L Armony, Marc D Pell","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad075","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsad075","url":null,"abstract":"In the extensive neuroimaging literature on empathy for pain, few studies have investigated how this phenomenon may relate to everyday social situations such as spoken interactions. The present study used fMRI to assess how complaints, as vocal expressions of pain, are empathically processed by listeners and how these empathic responses may vary based on speakers’ vocal expression and cultural identity. Twenty-four French participants listened to short utterances describing a painful event, that were either produced in a neutral-sounding or complaining voice by both ingroup (French) and outgroup (French-Canadian) speakers. Results suggest that the perception of suffering from a complaining voice increased activity in the Emotional Voice Areas (EVAs), composed of voice-sensitive temporal regions interacting with prefrontal cortices and the amygdala. The Salience and Theory of Mind networks, associated with affective and cognitive aspects of empathy, also showed prosody-related activity and specifically correlated with behavioral evaluations of suffering by listeners. Complaints produced by in- vs. outgroup speakers elicited sensorimotor and default-mode activity, respectively, suggesting accent-based changes in empathic perspective. These results, while reaffirming the role of key networks in tasks involving empathy, highlight the importance of vocal expression information and social categorization processes when perceiving another’s suffering during social interactions.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138690486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does pain hurt more in Spanish? The Neurobiology of Pain among Spanish-English Bilingual Adults 西班牙语中的疼痛更痛?西班牙语-英语双语成年人的疼痛神经生物学
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-12 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad074
Morgan Gianola, Maria Llabre, Elizabeth Losin
We previously found Spanish-English bilingual adults reported higher pain intensity when exposed to painful heat in the language of their stronger cultural orientation. Here, we elucidate brain systems involved in language-driven alterations in pain responses. During separate English- and Spanish-speaking fMRI scanning runs, 39 (21 female) bilingual adults rated painful heat intermixed between culturally evocative images and completed sentence reading tasks. Surveys of cultural identity and language use measured relative preference for US-American vs Hispanic culture (cultural orientation). Participants produced higher intensity ratings in Spanish compared to English. Group-level whole-brain differences in pain-evoked activity between languages emerged in somatosensory, cingulate, precuneus, and cerebellar cortex. Regions of interest associated with semantic, attention, and somatosensory processing showed higher average pain-evoked responses in participants’ culturally preferred language, as did expression of a multivariate pain-predictive pattern. Follow-up moderated mediation analyses showed somatosensory activity mediated language effects on pain intensity, particularly for Hispanic oriented participants. These findings relate to distinct (“meddler”, “spotlight”, and “inducer”) hypotheses about the nature of language effects on perception and cognition. Knowledge of language influences on pain could improve efficacy of culturally sensitive treatment approaches across the diversity of Hispanic adults to mitigate documented health disparities in this population.
我们之前发现,西班牙-英语双语成人在接触到以其较强文化取向的语言表达的疼痛热时,会报告出较高的疼痛强度。在这里,我们阐明了参与语言驱动的疼痛反应改变的大脑系统。在分别进行的英语和西班牙语fMRI扫描过程中,39名(21名女性)双语成年人对夹杂在具有文化唤起力的图像和完成的句子阅读任务之间的热痛进行了评分。对文化认同和语言使用的调查测量了对美国文化和西班牙文化的相对偏好(文化取向)。与英语相比,参与者用西班牙语做出的强度评级更高。在躯体感觉皮层、扣带回皮层、楔前皮层和小脑皮层中,不同语言的疼痛诱发活动存在群体水平的全脑差异。与语义、注意力和躯体感觉处理相关的兴趣区在参与者的文化偏好语言中显示出更高的平均疼痛诱发反应,多元疼痛预测模式的表达也是如此。后续的调节中介分析表明,躯体感觉活动中介了语言对疼痛强度的影响,尤其是对西班牙裔参与者而言。这些发现与关于语言对感知和认知影响的性质的不同假设("中介"、"聚焦 "和 "诱导")有关。了解语言对疼痛的影响可以提高西语裔成年人的文化敏感性治疗方法的疗效,从而减轻该人群中存在的健康差异。
{"title":"Does pain hurt more in Spanish? The Neurobiology of Pain among Spanish-English Bilingual Adults","authors":"Morgan Gianola, Maria Llabre, Elizabeth Losin","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad074","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsad074","url":null,"abstract":"We previously found Spanish-English bilingual adults reported higher pain intensity when exposed to painful heat in the language of their stronger cultural orientation. Here, we elucidate brain systems involved in language-driven alterations in pain responses. During separate English- and Spanish-speaking fMRI scanning runs, 39 (21 female) bilingual adults rated painful heat intermixed between culturally evocative images and completed sentence reading tasks. Surveys of cultural identity and language use measured relative preference for US-American vs Hispanic culture (cultural orientation). Participants produced higher intensity ratings in Spanish compared to English. Group-level whole-brain differences in pain-evoked activity between languages emerged in somatosensory, cingulate, precuneus, and cerebellar cortex. Regions of interest associated with semantic, attention, and somatosensory processing showed higher average pain-evoked responses in participants’ culturally preferred language, as did expression of a multivariate pain-predictive pattern. Follow-up moderated mediation analyses showed somatosensory activity mediated language effects on pain intensity, particularly for Hispanic oriented participants. These findings relate to distinct (“meddler”, “spotlight”, and “inducer”) hypotheses about the nature of language effects on perception and cognition. Knowledge of language influences on pain could improve efficacy of culturally sensitive treatment approaches across the diversity of Hispanic adults to mitigate documented health disparities in this population.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138690254","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Speed of Race 比赛的速度
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-12 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad076
Peter de Lissa, Pauline Schaller, Roberto Caldara
When asked to categorize faces according to ‘race’, people typically categorize other-race faces faster than faces belonging to their own race. This Other Race Categorization Advantage is thought to reflect enhanced sensitivity to early visual signals characteristic of other-race faces, and can manifest within 200 ms of face presentation. However, recent research has highlighted the importance of signal intensity in this effect, where visual-degradation of the face images significantly enhances the effect and exposes a behavioural threshold at very low levels of visual quality where other-race visual signals are able to be perceived while same-race signals are not. The current study investigated the effect of signal intensity in race categorization processes in the brain through electroencephalography and in accuracy/reaction times. While replicating the previously observed enhancement of the other-race categorization advantage, we also found enhanced sensitivity to other-race faces in early P1 peaks, as well as later N170, and N250 peaks. These effects, however, related to the varying levels of signal intensity in the face stimuli, suggesting that race categorization may involve different types of perceptual and neural processes rather than one discrete process. The speed at which race is perceived depends on the intensity of the face signal.
当被要求根据 "种族 "对人脸进行分类时,人们通常会比对自己种族的人脸更快地对其他种族的人脸进行分类。这种 "其他种族分类优势 "被认为反映了人们对其他种族面孔特有的早期视觉信号的敏感性的增强,并可在面孔呈现的 200 毫秒内表现出来。然而,最近的研究强调了信号强度在这一效应中的重要性,人脸图像的视觉降级显著增强了这一效应,并在极低的视觉质量水平上暴露出行为阈值,在这一阈值上,其他种族的视觉信号能够被感知,而同一种族的信号则不能被感知。本研究通过脑电图和准确性/反应时间研究了信号强度对大脑种族分类过程的影响。在复制之前观察到的其他种族分类优势增强的同时,我们还发现在早期 P1 峰以及后期 N170 和 N250 峰中对其他种族面孔的敏感性增强。然而,这些效应与人脸刺激中不同程度的信号强度有关,这表明种族分类可能涉及不同类型的知觉和神经过程,而不是一个独立的过程。种族感知的速度取决于人脸信号的强度。
{"title":"The Speed of Race","authors":"Peter de Lissa, Pauline Schaller, Roberto Caldara","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad076","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsad076","url":null,"abstract":"When asked to categorize faces according to ‘race’, people typically categorize other-race faces faster than faces belonging to their own race. This Other Race Categorization Advantage is thought to reflect enhanced sensitivity to early visual signals characteristic of other-race faces, and can manifest within 200 ms of face presentation. However, recent research has highlighted the importance of signal intensity in this effect, where visual-degradation of the face images significantly enhances the effect and exposes a behavioural threshold at very low levels of visual quality where other-race visual signals are able to be perceived while same-race signals are not. The current study investigated the effect of signal intensity in race categorization processes in the brain through electroencephalography and in accuracy/reaction times. While replicating the previously observed enhancement of the other-race categorization advantage, we also found enhanced sensitivity to other-race faces in early P1 peaks, as well as later N170, and N250 peaks. These effects, however, related to the varying levels of signal intensity in the face stimuli, suggesting that race categorization may involve different types of perceptual and neural processes rather than one discrete process. The speed at which race is perceived depends on the intensity of the face signal.","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138690501","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effector-specific motor simulation supplements core action recognition processes in adverse conditions. 效应器特定的运动模拟补充了不利条件下的核心动作识别过程。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad046
Gilles Vannuscorps, Alfonso Caramazza

Observing other people acting activates imitative motor plans in the observer. Whether, and if so when and how, such 'effector-specific motor simulation' contributes to action recognition remains unclear. We report that individuals born without upper limbs (IDs)-who cannot covertly imitate upper-limb movements-are significantly less accurate at recognizing degraded (but not intact) upper-limb than lower-limb actions (i.e. point-light animations). This finding emphasizes the need to reframe the current controversy regarding the role of effector-specific motor simulation in action recognition: instead of focusing on the dichotomy between motor and non-motor theories, the field would benefit from new hypotheses specifying when and how effector-specific motor simulation may supplement core action recognition processes to accommodate the full variety of action stimuli that humans can recognize.

观察其他人的行为会激活观察者的模仿运动计划。这种“效应器特异性运动模拟”是否有助于动作识别,如果有,何时以及如何,目前尚不清楚。我们报告说,出生时没有上肢(ID)的人——他们不能秘密模仿上肢运动——在识别退化(但不是完整的)上肢方面的准确性明显低于下肢动作(即点光源动画)。这一发现强调了重新定义当前关于效应器特异性运动模拟在动作识别中的作用的争议的必要性:与其关注运动和非运动理论之间的二分法,该领域将受益于新的假设,这些假设规定了效应器特异性运动模拟何时以及如何补充核心动作识别过程,以适应人类可以识别的各种各样的动作刺激。
{"title":"Effector-specific motor simulation supplements core action recognition processes in adverse conditions.","authors":"Gilles Vannuscorps, Alfonso Caramazza","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad046","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad046","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Observing other people acting activates imitative motor plans in the observer. Whether, and if so when and how, such 'effector-specific motor simulation' contributes to action recognition remains unclear. We report that individuals born without upper limbs (IDs)-who cannot covertly imitate upper-limb movements-are significantly less accurate at recognizing degraded (but not intact) upper-limb than lower-limb actions (i.e. point-light animations). This finding emphasizes the need to reframe the current controversy regarding the role of effector-specific motor simulation in action recognition: instead of focusing on the dichotomy between motor and non-motor theories, the field would benefit from new hypotheses specifying when and how effector-specific motor simulation may supplement core action recognition processes to accommodate the full variety of action stimuli that humans can recognize.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10576201/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10542250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mothers exhibit higher neural activity in gaining rewards for their children than for themselves. 母亲在为孩子获得奖励方面表现出比为自己获得奖励更高的神经活动。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-06 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad048
Yan Zhang, Yachao Rong, Ping Wei

Are people willing to exert greater effort to obtain rewards for their children than they are for themselves? Although previous studies have demonstrated that social distance influences neural responses to altruistic reward processing, the distinction between winning rewards for oneself and winning them for one's child is unclear. In the present study, a group of 31 mothers performed a monetary incentive delay task in which cue-induced reward anticipations of winning a reward for themselves, their children and donation to a charity program were manipulated trial-wise, followed by performance-contingent feedback. Behaviorally, the anticipation of winning a reward for their children accelerated participants' responses. Importantly, the electroencephalogram results revealed that across the reward anticipation and consumption phases, the child condition elicited comparable or higher brain responses of participants than the self condition did. The source localization results showed that participants' reward anticipations for their children were associated with more activation in the social brain regions, compared to winning a reward for themselves or a charity donation. Overall, these findings advance our understanding of the neural mechanisms of altruistic reward processing and suggest that the priority of winning a reward for one's child may transcend the limits of the self-advantage effect in reward processing.

人们是否愿意为孩子付出比自己更大的努力来获得奖励?尽管之前的研究表明,社交距离会影响神经对利他主义奖励处理的反应,但为自己赢得奖励和为孩子赢得奖励之间的区别尚不清楚。在本研究中,一组由31名母亲组成的小组进行了一项金钱激励延迟任务,在该任务中,对线索诱导的为自己、孩子赢得奖励和向慈善项目捐款的奖励预期进行了试验性操纵,然后进行绩效相关反馈。在行为上,对为孩子赢得奖励的预期加速了参与者的反应。重要的是,脑电图结果显示,在奖励预期和消费阶段,儿童状况引发的参与者大脑反应与自我状况相当或更高。来源定位结果显示,与为自己或慈善捐款赢得奖励相比,参与者对孩子的奖励预期与社交大脑区域的更多激活有关。总的来说,这些发现促进了我们对利他主义奖励处理的神经机制的理解,并表明为孩子赢得奖励的优先级可能超过奖励处理中自我优势效应的限制。
{"title":"Mothers exhibit higher neural activity in gaining rewards for their children than for themselves.","authors":"Yan Zhang, Yachao Rong, Ping Wei","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad048","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad048","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Are people willing to exert greater effort to obtain rewards for their children than they are for themselves? Although previous studies have demonstrated that social distance influences neural responses to altruistic reward processing, the distinction between winning rewards for oneself and winning them for one's child is unclear. In the present study, a group of 31 mothers performed a monetary incentive delay task in which cue-induced reward anticipations of winning a reward for themselves, their children and donation to a charity program were manipulated trial-wise, followed by performance-contingent feedback. Behaviorally, the anticipation of winning a reward for their children accelerated participants' responses. Importantly, the electroencephalogram results revealed that across the reward anticipation and consumption phases, the child condition elicited comparable or higher brain responses of participants than the self condition did. The source localization results showed that participants' reward anticipations for their children were associated with more activation in the social brain regions, compared to winning a reward for themselves or a charity donation. Overall, these findings advance our understanding of the neural mechanisms of altruistic reward processing and suggest that the priority of winning a reward for one's child may transcend the limits of the self-advantage effect in reward processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10558201/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10227403","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Similarity in functional connectome architecture predicts teenage grit. 功能连接体结构的相似性预测青少年的毅力。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad047
Sujin Park, Daeun Park, M Justin Kim

Grit is a personality trait that encapsulates the tendency to persevere and maintain consistent interest for long-term goals. While prior studies found that grit predicts positive behavioral outcomes, there is a paucity of work providing explanatory evidence from a neurodevelopmental perspective. Based on previous research suggesting the utility of the functional connectome (FC) as a developmental measure, we tested the idea that individual differences in grit might be, in part, rooted in brain development in adolescence and emerging adulthood (N = 64, 11-19 years of age). Our analysis showed that grit was associated with connectome stability across conditions and connectome similarity across individuals. Notably, inter-subject representational similarity analysis revealed that teenagers who were grittier shared similar FC architecture with each other, more so than those with lower grit. Our findings suggest that individuals with high levels of grit are more likely to exhibit a converging pattern of whole-brain functional connectivity, which may underpin subsequent beneficial behavioral outcomes.

Grit是一种性格特征,它概括了坚持下去并对长期目标保持一致兴趣的倾向。虽然先前的研究发现勇气可以预测积极的行为结果,但从神经发育的角度提供解释性证据的工作很少。基于先前的研究表明功能连接体(FC)作为一种发育指标的效用,我们测试了个体毅力的差异可能部分源于青春期和成年期的大脑发育(N = 64,11-19 年龄)。我们的分析表明,砂砾与不同条件下连接体的稳定性和个体间连接体的相似性有关。值得注意的是,受试者之间的表征相似性分析显示,更坚韧的青少年彼此共享相似的FC架构,比那些更坚韧的人更相似。我们的研究结果表明,具有高毅力的个体更有可能表现出全脑功能连接的趋同模式,这可能是后续有益行为结果的基础。
{"title":"Similarity in functional connectome architecture predicts teenage grit.","authors":"Sujin Park,&nbsp;Daeun Park,&nbsp;M Justin Kim","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad047","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad047","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Grit is a personality trait that encapsulates the tendency to persevere and maintain consistent interest for long-term goals. While prior studies found that grit predicts positive behavioral outcomes, there is a paucity of work providing explanatory evidence from a neurodevelopmental perspective. Based on previous research suggesting the utility of the functional connectome (FC) as a developmental measure, we tested the idea that individual differences in grit might be, in part, rooted in brain development in adolescence and emerging adulthood (N = 64, 11-19 years of age). Our analysis showed that grit was associated with connectome stability across conditions and connectome similarity across individuals. Notably, inter-subject representational similarity analysis revealed that teenagers who were grittier shared similar FC architecture with each other, more so than those with lower grit. Our findings suggest that individuals with high levels of grit are more likely to exhibit a converging pattern of whole-brain functional connectivity, which may underpin subsequent beneficial behavioral outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/49/e5/nsad047.PMC10549957.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10214803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Openness to Experience is associated with neural and performance measures of memory in older adults. 对经验的开放性与老年人记忆的神经和表现测量有关。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-27 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad041
Christopher Stolz, Ariane Bulla, Joram Soch, Björn H Schott, Anni Richter

Age-related decline in episodic memory performance is a well-replicated finding across numerous studies. Recent studies focusing on aging and individual differences found that the Big Five personality trait Openness to Experience (hereafter: Openness) is associated with better episodic memory performance in older adults, but the associated neural mechanisms are largely unclear. Here, we investigated the relationship between Openness and memory network function in a sample of 352 participants (143 older adults, 50-80 years; 209 young adults, 18-35 years). Participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a visual memory encoding task. Functional memory brain-network integrity was assessed using the similarity of activations during memory encoding (SAME) scores, which reflect the similarity of a participant's memory network activity compared to prototypical fMRI activity patterns of young adults. Openness was assessed using the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Older vs young adults showed lower memory performance and higher deviation of fMRI activity patterns (i.e. lower SAME scores). Specifically in older adults, high Openness was associated with better memory performance, and mediation analysis showed that this relationship was partially mediated by higher SAME scores. Our results suggest that trait Openness may constitute a protective factor in cognitive aging by better preservation of the brain's memory network.

在许多研究中,与年龄相关的情景记忆表现下降是一个很好的复制发现。最近专注于衰老和个体差异的研究发现,五大人格特征对经验的开放性(以下简称:开放性)与老年人更好的情景记忆表现有关,但相关的神经机制在很大程度上尚不清楚。在这里,我们调查了352名参与者(143名老年人,50-80岁 年;209名18-35岁的年轻人 年)。参与者在视觉记忆编码任务中接受了功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)。使用记忆编码过程中激活的相似性(SAME)评分来评估功能记忆脑网络的完整性,该评分反映了参与者的记忆网络活动与年轻人典型的fMRI活动模式的相似性。开放性使用近地天体五因素清单进行评估。老年人与年轻人相比,记忆力较差,功能磁共振成像活动模式偏差较大(即SAME评分较低)。特别是在老年人中,高开放性与更好的记忆表现有关,中介分析表明,这种关系部分是由较高的SAME分数介导的。我们的研究结果表明,开放性特征可能通过更好地保存大脑的记忆网络,构成认知衰老的保护因素。
{"title":"Openness to Experience is associated with neural and performance measures of memory in older adults.","authors":"Christopher Stolz, Ariane Bulla, Joram Soch, Björn H Schott, Anni Richter","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad041","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad041","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Age-related decline in episodic memory performance is a well-replicated finding across numerous studies. Recent studies focusing on aging and individual differences found that the Big Five personality trait Openness to Experience (hereafter: Openness) is associated with better episodic memory performance in older adults, but the associated neural mechanisms are largely unclear. Here, we investigated the relationship between Openness and memory network function in a sample of 352 participants (143 older adults, 50-80 years; 209 young adults, 18-35 years). Participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a visual memory encoding task. Functional memory brain-network integrity was assessed using the similarity of activations during memory encoding (SAME) scores, which reflect the similarity of a participant's memory network activity compared to prototypical fMRI activity patterns of young adults. Openness was assessed using the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Older vs young adults showed lower memory performance and higher deviation of fMRI activity patterns (i.e. lower SAME scores). Specifically in older adults, high Openness was associated with better memory performance, and mediation analysis showed that this relationship was partially mediated by higher SAME scores. Our results suggest that trait Openness may constitute a protective factor in cognitive aging by better preservation of the brain's memory network.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10533339/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10073428","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Partner similarity and social cognitive traits predict social interaction success among strangers. 伴侣相似性和社会认知特征预测陌生人之间社交互动的成功。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad045
Sarah L Dziura, Aditi Hosangadi, Deena Shariq, Junaid S Merchant, Elizabeth Redcay

Social interactions are a ubiquitous part of engaging in the world around us, and determining what makes an interaction successful is necessary for social well-being. This study examined the separate contributions of individual social cognitive ability and partner similarity to social interaction success among strangers, measured by a cooperative communication task and self-reported interaction quality. Sixty participants engaged in a 1-h virtual social interaction with an unfamiliar partner (a laboratory confederate) including a 30-min cooperative 'mind-reading' game and then completed several individual tasks and surveys. They then underwent a separate functional MRI session in which they passively viewed video clips that varied in content. The neural responses to these videos were correlated with those of their confederate interaction partners to yield a measure of pairwise neural similarity. We found that trait empathy (assessed by the interpersonal reactivity index) and neural similarity to partner both predicted communication success in the mind-reading game. In contrast, perceived similarity to partner and (to a much lesser extent) trait mind-reading motivation predicted self-reported interaction quality. These results highlight the importance of sharing perspectives in successful communication as well as differences between neurobiological similarity and perceived similarity in supporting different types of interaction success.

社交互动是我们周围世界中无处不在的一部分,决定是什么使互动成功对社会福祉是必要的。本研究通过合作沟通任务和自我报告的互动质量来衡量个人社交认知能力和伴侣相似性对陌生人社交成功的单独贡献。60名参与者与一位陌生的伴侣(实验室联盟)进行了1小时的虚拟社交互动,包括30分钟的合作“读心术”游戏,然后完成了几项个人任务和调查。然后,他们接受了一次单独的功能性MRI检查,被动地观看内容各异的视频片段。对这些视频的神经反应与他们的联盟互动伙伴的反应相关联,以产生成对神经相似性的测量。我们发现,特质移情(通过人际反应指数评估)和与伴侣的神经相似性都可以预测读心游戏中的沟通成功。相比之下,感知到的与伴侣的相似性和(在较小程度上)特质读心动机预测了自我报告的互动质量。这些结果强调了分享观点在成功沟通中的重要性,以及神经生物学相似性和感知相似性在支持不同类型的互动成功方面的差异。
{"title":"Partner similarity and social cognitive traits predict social interaction success among strangers.","authors":"Sarah L Dziura,&nbsp;Aditi Hosangadi,&nbsp;Deena Shariq,&nbsp;Junaid S Merchant,&nbsp;Elizabeth Redcay","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad045","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad045","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social interactions are a ubiquitous part of engaging in the world around us, and determining what makes an interaction successful is necessary for social well-being. This study examined the separate contributions of individual social cognitive ability and partner similarity to social interaction success among strangers, measured by a cooperative communication task and self-reported interaction quality. Sixty participants engaged in a 1-h virtual social interaction with an unfamiliar partner (a laboratory confederate) including a 30-min cooperative 'mind-reading' game and then completed several individual tasks and surveys. They then underwent a separate functional MRI session in which they passively viewed video clips that varied in content. The neural responses to these videos were correlated with those of their confederate interaction partners to yield a measure of pairwise neural similarity. We found that trait empathy (assessed by the interpersonal reactivity index) and neural similarity to partner both predicted communication success in the mind-reading game. In contrast, perceived similarity to partner and (to a much lesser extent) trait mind-reading motivation predicted self-reported interaction quality. These results highlight the importance of sharing perspectives in successful communication as well as differences between neurobiological similarity and perceived similarity in supporting different types of interaction success.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10516339/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10214435","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of induced optimism on early pain processing: indication by contact heat evoked potentials (CHEPs) and the sympathetic skin response (SSR). 诱导乐观对早期疼痛处理的影响:接触热诱发电位(CHEPs)和交感皮肤反应(SSR)的指示。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-19 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad042
Johanna Basten-Günther, Laura Jutz, Madelon L Peters, Janosch A Priebe, Stefan Lautenbacher

Situationally induced optimism has been shown to influence several components of experimental pain. The aim of the present study was to enlarge these findings for the first time to the earliest components of the pain response by measuring contact heat evoked potentials (CHEPs) and the sympathetic skin response (SSR). Forty-seven healthy participants underwent two blocks of phasic thermal stimulation. CHEPs, the SSR and self-report pain ratings were recorded. Between the blocks of stimulation, the 'Best Possible Self' imagery and writing task was performed to induce situational optimism. The optimism manipulation was successful in increasing state optimism. It did, however, neither affect pain-evoked potentials nor the SSR nor self-report pain ratings. These results suggest that optimism does not alter early responses to pain. The higher-level cognitive processes involved in optimistic thinking might only act on later stages of pain processing. Therefore, more research is needed targeting different time frames of stimulus processing and response measures for early and late pain processing in parallel.

情境引发的乐观情绪已被证明会影响实验性疼痛的几个组成部分。本研究的目的是通过测量接触热诱发电位(CHEP)和交感皮肤反应(SSR),首次将这些发现扩展到疼痛反应的最早成分。47名健康参与者接受了两组阶段性热刺激。记录CHEP、SSR和自我报告的疼痛评分。在刺激块之间,进行“最佳自我”意象和写作任务,以诱导情境乐观。乐观情绪的操纵成功地增加了国家的乐观情绪。然而,它既没有影响疼痛诱发电位,也没有影响SSR,也没有自我报告的疼痛评级。这些结果表明,乐观主义不会改变早期对疼痛的反应。乐观思维中涉及的更高层次的认知过程可能只作用于疼痛处理的后期阶段。因此,需要针对不同的刺激处理时间框架和早期和晚期疼痛处理的反应措施进行更多的研究。
{"title":"The effect of induced optimism on early pain processing: indication by contact heat evoked potentials (CHEPs) and the sympathetic skin response (SSR).","authors":"Johanna Basten-Günther,&nbsp;Laura Jutz,&nbsp;Madelon L Peters,&nbsp;Janosch A Priebe,&nbsp;Stefan Lautenbacher","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad042","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad042","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Situationally induced optimism has been shown to influence several components of experimental pain. The aim of the present study was to enlarge these findings for the first time to the earliest components of the pain response by measuring contact heat evoked potentials (CHEPs) and the sympathetic skin response (SSR). Forty-seven healthy participants underwent two blocks of phasic thermal stimulation. CHEPs, the SSR and self-report pain ratings were recorded. Between the blocks of stimulation, the 'Best Possible Self' imagery and writing task was performed to induce situational optimism. The optimism manipulation was successful in increasing state optimism. It did, however, neither affect pain-evoked potentials nor the SSR nor self-report pain ratings. These results suggest that optimism does not alter early responses to pain. The higher-level cognitive processes involved in optimistic thinking might only act on later stages of pain processing. Therefore, more research is needed targeting different time frames of stimulus processing and response measures for early and late pain processing in parallel.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10508319/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10652343","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relation of resting brain signal variability to cognitive and socioemotional measures in an adult lifespan sample. 成年人寿命样本中静息脑信号变异性与认知和社会情绪测量的关系。
IF 4.2 2区 医学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-18 DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsad044
Cheryl L Grady, Jenny R Rieck, Giulia Baracchini, Brennan DeSouza

Temporal variability of the fMRI-derived blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal during cognitive tasks shows important associations with individual differences in age and performance. Less is known about relations between spontaneous BOLD variability measured at rest and relatively stable cognitive measures, such as IQ or socioemotional function. Here, we examined associations among resting BOLD variability, cognitive/socioemotional scores from the NIH Toolbox and optimal time of day for alertness (chronotype) in a sample of 157 adults from 20 to 86 years of age. To investigate individual differences in these associations independently of age, we regressed age out from both behavioral and BOLD variability scores. We hypothesized that greater BOLD variability would be related to higher fluid cognition scores, more positive scores on socioemotional scales and a morningness chronotype. Consistent with this idea, we found positive correlations between resting BOLD variability, positive socioemotional scores (e.g. self-efficacy) and morning chronotype, as well as negative correlations between variability and negative emotional scores (e.g. loneliness). Unexpectedly, we found negative correlations between BOLD variability and fluid cognition. These results suggest that greater resting brain signal variability facilitates optimal socioemotional function and characterizes those with morning-type circadian rhythms, but individuals with greater fluid cognition may be more likely to show less temporal variability in spontaneous measures of BOLD activity.

功能磁共振成像衍生的血氧水平依赖性(BOLD)信号在认知任务中的时间变异性与年龄和表现的个体差异有重要联系。关于休息时测量的自发BOLD变异性与相对稳定的认知测量(如智商或社会情绪功能)之间的关系,人们知之甚少。在这里,我们对157名20岁至86岁的成年人样本进行了静息BOLD变异性、NIH工具箱中的认知/社会情绪评分和最佳警觉性时间(时间型)之间的相关性研究 年龄。为了独立于年龄研究这些关联的个体差异,我们从行为和BOLD变异性得分中回归了年龄。我们假设,更大的BOLD变异性与更高的流体认知得分、更积极的社会情绪量表得分和晨间时间型有关。与这一观点一致,我们发现静息BOLD变异性、积极的社会情绪得分(如自我效能感)和早晨时间类型之间存在正相关,变异性和消极情绪得分(例如孤独感)之间存在负相关。出乎意料的是,我们发现BOLD变异性和流体认知之间存在负相关。这些结果表明,更大的静息脑信号变异性有助于优化社会情绪功能,并具有早晨型昼夜节律的特征,但具有更高流体认知能力的个体可能更有可能在BOLD活动的自发测量中表现出更少的时间变异性。
{"title":"Relation of resting brain signal variability to cognitive and socioemotional measures in an adult lifespan sample.","authors":"Cheryl L Grady,&nbsp;Jenny R Rieck,&nbsp;Giulia Baracchini,&nbsp;Brennan DeSouza","doi":"10.1093/scan/nsad044","DOIUrl":"10.1093/scan/nsad044","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Temporal variability of the fMRI-derived blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal during cognitive tasks shows important associations with individual differences in age and performance. Less is known about relations between spontaneous BOLD variability measured at rest and relatively stable cognitive measures, such as IQ or socioemotional function. Here, we examined associations among resting BOLD variability, cognitive/socioemotional scores from the NIH Toolbox and optimal time of day for alertness (chronotype) in a sample of 157 adults from 20 to 86 years of age. To investigate individual differences in these associations independently of age, we regressed age out from both behavioral and BOLD variability scores. We hypothesized that greater BOLD variability would be related to higher fluid cognition scores, more positive scores on socioemotional scales and a morningness chronotype. Consistent with this idea, we found positive correlations between resting BOLD variability, positive socioemotional scores (e.g. self-efficacy) and morning chronotype, as well as negative correlations between variability and negative emotional scores (e.g. loneliness). Unexpectedly, we found negative correlations between BOLD variability and fluid cognition. These results suggest that greater resting brain signal variability facilitates optimal socioemotional function and characterizes those with morning-type circadian rhythms, but individuals with greater fluid cognition may be more likely to show less temporal variability in spontaneous measures of BOLD activity.</p>","PeriodicalId":21789,"journal":{"name":"Social cognitive and affective neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.2,"publicationDate":"2023-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10508322/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10670368","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social cognitive and affective neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1