首页 > 最新文献

Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Gogi (Word Meaning) Aphasia and Its Relation with Semantic Dementia. 词义失语症及其与语义性痴呆的关系。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494950
Atsushi Yamadori

In 1943, Tsuneo Imura, a neuropsychiatrist at Tokyo University, proposed a new aphasic syndrome and designated it as Gogi (word meaning) aphasia. According to Imura, it is characterized by (1) difficulty in comprehending spoken words despite good perception of sound, (2) disorders of expression due to loss of vocabulary and verbal paraphasia, (3) preserved ability of repetition, and (4) selective difficulty in reading and writing kanji (Japanese logographic character) with preserved ability of reading and writing kana (Japanese syllabic character). To be more specific, the difficulty in comprehending spoken words in Gogi aphasia is limited to substantive words as the name of the syndrome indicates, while comprehension of syntax remains unimpaired. Preserved repetition ability cannot be simply regarded as an automatic response, that is, echolalia, because the patient's attitude in repeating was quizzical, picking up the particular word he did not comprehend. Imura vaguely attributed the responsible lesion to the second and third temporal gyri on the left side. Although the general pattern of the syndrome can be placed in the category of transcortical sensory aphasia, its uniqueness as the syndrome still stands out. Another uniqueness of the syndrome is its characteristic pattern of dissociated kanji-kana difficulty in reading and writing. This linguistic symptomatology observed in Gogi aphasia was first introduced to the western academic world on a German neuropsychiatric journal by Panse and Shimoyama in 1955. Ever since, the existence of Gogi aphasia as an independent syndrome among disorders of spoken language has gradually gained international recognition. But whether the pattern of the kanji-kana dissociation described by Imura is an integral part of the syndrome remains unsettled. A recently proposed concept of semantic dementia suggests symptomatic continuation of word meaning loss with non-linguistic semantic memory loss.

1943年,东京大学的神经精神病学家今村恒雄提出了一种新的失语症,并将其命名为高义失语症。据Imura说,它的特点是:(1)尽管有良好的声音感知,但理解口语有困难;(2)由于词汇量的丧失和言语释义造成的表达障碍;(3)保留了重复的能力;(4)保留了阅读和书写假名(日语音节字符)的能力,但在阅读和书写汉字(日语符号字符)方面存在选择性困难。更具体地说,高尔基失语症患者的口语理解困难,正如其名称所示,仅限于实体词,而对句法的理解并未受损。保留的重复能力不能简单地认为是一种自动反应,即模仿,因为患者在重复时的态度是好奇的,捡起他不理解的特定单词。Imura隐隐约约地将病变归因于左侧的第二和第三颞回。虽然该综合征的一般模式可以被置于经皮层感觉失语症的范畴,但其作为综合征的独特性仍然突出。该综合征的另一个独特之处在于其分离的汉字-假名读写困难的典型模式。这种在高氏失语症中观察到的语言症状学在1955年由潘斯和下山在一份德国神经精神病学杂志上首次引入西方学术界。此后,高尔基失语症作为一种独立的言语障碍综合征的存在逐渐得到了国际社会的认可。但是,Imura所描述的假名与汉字的分离模式是否是该综合征的一个组成部分仍未得到解决。最近提出的语义痴呆的概念表明,词义丧失的症状延续与非语言语义记忆丧失。
{"title":"Gogi (Word Meaning) Aphasia and Its Relation with Semantic Dementia.","authors":"Atsushi Yamadori","doi":"10.1159/000494950","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494950","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In 1943, Tsuneo Imura, a neuropsychiatrist at Tokyo University, proposed a new aphasic syndrome and designated it as Gogi (word meaning) aphasia. According to Imura, it is characterized by (1) difficulty in comprehending spoken words despite good perception of sound, (2) disorders of expression due to loss of vocabulary and verbal paraphasia, (3) preserved ability of repetition, and (4) selective difficulty in reading and writing kanji (Japanese logographic character) with preserved ability of reading and writing kana (Japanese syllabic character). To be more specific, the difficulty in comprehending spoken words in Gogi aphasia is limited to substantive words as the name of the syndrome indicates, while comprehension of syntax remains unimpaired. Preserved repetition ability cannot be simply regarded as an automatic response, that is, echolalia, because the patient's attitude in repeating was quizzical, picking up the particular word he did not comprehend. Imura vaguely attributed the responsible lesion to the second and third temporal gyri on the left side. Although the general pattern of the syndrome can be placed in the category of transcortical sensory aphasia, its uniqueness as the syndrome still stands out. Another uniqueness of the syndrome is its characteristic pattern of dissociated kanji-kana difficulty in reading and writing. This linguistic symptomatology observed in Gogi aphasia was first introduced to the western academic world on a German neuropsychiatric journal by Panse and Shimoyama in 1955. Ever since, the existence of Gogi aphasia as an independent syndrome among disorders of spoken language has gradually gained international recognition. But whether the pattern of the kanji-kana dissociation described by Imura is an integral part of the syndrome remains unsettled. A recently proposed concept of semantic dementia suggests symptomatic continuation of word meaning loss with non-linguistic semantic memory loss.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494950","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
History of Subcortical Cognitive Impairment. 皮质下认知障碍病史。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494958
Christopher M Filley

The representation of cognitive function in the cerebral cortex has a long and cherished history, but much evidence also supports a critical role of subcortical structures in the operations of cognition. The idea of subcortical dementia, first proposed in 1932 and substantially expanded in the 1970s, is the most prominent formulation intended to capture the phenomenology of cognitive impairment attributable to subcortical involvement. Despite criticism highlighting its imprecision, subcortical dementia has endured as a useful general concept assisting the classification of dementia syndromes based on the primary site(s) of neuropathology. As neuroscientific knowledge expanded with the advent of modern structural and functional neuroimaging, a more detailed understanding of the contributions of specific subcortical regions emerged, such that the cognitive affiliations of the basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum, brainstem, and white matter are all better defined. Important advances have been made by the study of both neurodegenerative diseases and focal lesions. Today, the complex admixture of cortical and subcortical foundations of cognition is increasingly well appreciated, and has been conceptually organized within the broadly inclusive notion of distributed neural networks. These networks are thought to integrate cortical and subcortical gray and white matter structures throughout the brain into functional neuronal ensembles subserving various domains of cognition. In this light, specific disorders of subcortical regions produce cognitive sequelae that can be usefully analyzed within the context of networks that involve key cortical regions as well. The study of subcortical contributions to cognition has been highly informative in expanding neurobehavioral thinking to include regions beyond the cerebral cortex, adding nuance and sophistication to the conceptualization of brain-behavior relationships.

认知功能在大脑皮层中的表现有着悠久而珍贵的历史,但许多证据也支持皮层下结构在认知运作中的关键作用。皮层下痴呆的概念于1932年首次提出,并在20世纪70年代得到了实质性的扩展,是旨在捕捉可归因于皮层下受累的认知障碍现象学的最突出的构想。尽管批评强调其不精确,皮质下痴呆一直是一个有用的一般概念,有助于根据神经病理的原发部位对痴呆综合征进行分类。随着现代结构和功能神经影像学的出现,神经科学知识得到了扩展,对特定皮层下区域的贡献有了更详细的了解,从而对基底节区、丘脑、小脑、脑干和白质的认知联系有了更好的定义。神经退行性疾病和局灶性病变的研究取得了重要进展。今天,认知的皮层和皮层下基础的复杂混合物越来越受到重视,并在概念上组织在分布式神经网络的广泛包容的概念中。这些网络被认为将整个大脑的皮层和皮层下灰质和白质结构整合为功能神经元集合,服务于不同的认知领域。从这个角度来看,皮层下区域的特定疾病产生认知后遗症,可以在涉及关键皮层区域的网络背景下有效地分析。皮层下对认知的贡献的研究在将神经行为思维扩展到大脑皮层以外的区域方面提供了大量信息,为大脑-行为关系的概念化增加了细微差别和复杂性。
{"title":"History of Subcortical Cognitive Impairment.","authors":"Christopher M Filley","doi":"10.1159/000494958","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494958","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The representation of cognitive function in the cerebral cortex has a long and cherished history, but much evidence also supports a critical role of subcortical structures in the operations of cognition. The idea of subcortical dementia, first proposed in 1932 and substantially expanded in the 1970s, is the most prominent formulation intended to capture the phenomenology of cognitive impairment attributable to subcortical involvement. Despite criticism highlighting its imprecision, subcortical dementia has endured as a useful general concept assisting the classification of dementia syndromes based on the primary site(s) of neuropathology. As neuroscientific knowledge expanded with the advent of modern structural and functional neuroimaging, a more detailed understanding of the contributions of specific subcortical regions emerged, such that the cognitive affiliations of the basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum, brainstem, and white matter are all better defined. Important advances have been made by the study of both neurodegenerative diseases and focal lesions. Today, the complex admixture of cortical and subcortical foundations of cognition is increasingly well appreciated, and has been conceptually organized within the broadly inclusive notion of distributed neural networks. These networks are thought to integrate cortical and subcortical gray and white matter structures throughout the brain into functional neuronal ensembles subserving various domains of cognition. In this light, specific disorders of subcortical regions produce cognitive sequelae that can be usefully analyzed within the context of networks that involve key cortical regions as well. The study of subcortical contributions to cognition has been highly informative in expanding neurobehavioral thinking to include regions beyond the cerebral cortex, adding nuance and sophistication to the conceptualization of brain-behavior relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494958","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Neurology versus Psychiatry? Hallucinations, Delusions, and Confabulations. 神经病学还是精神病学?幻觉、妄想和虚构。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494960
Antonio Carota, Julien Bogousslavsky

Hallucinations, delusions, and confabulations are common symptoms between neurology and psychiatry. The neurological diseases manifesting with such symptoms (dementia, epilepsy, Korsakoff's disease, brain tumors, Parkinson's disease, migraine, right hemisphere stroke and others) would be the key to understand their biological mechanisms, while the cognitive sciences, neuropharmacology and functional neuroimaging would be the tools of such researches. It is possible to understand the perceptive rules of the mind and the mechanisms of the human consciousness based on these symptoms. However, hallucinations and delusions manifest with extraordinary vehemence with psychiatric disorders such as psychosis and schizophrenia, with which there is no evidence of brain lesions. Furthermore, they are subjective symptoms, and they do not have biological markers. Hence, they are prone to high inter-individual variability and depend on other variables (such as education, history of trauma), and are therefore difficult to reduce to unequivocal constructs. Causative mechanisms are probably multiple. For understanding these symptoms, a common framework between neurology and psychiatry is still missing. The psychopathology of French alienists over the 19th century, of S. Freud, and of Henry Ey over the 20th century gave way, in the second half of the 20th century, to the adoption of the DSM and neurosciences, to pursue a pure neurological perspective. However, although psychodynamic models seem nowadays (in a technological era) less influential, detailed clinical evaluations focusing on emotional-cognitive paradigms are probably the only way to lead to new neurobiological researches.

幻觉、妄想和虚构是神经病学和精神病学之间的常见症状。表现出这些症状的神经系统疾病(痴呆、癫痫、Korsakoff病、脑肿瘤、帕金森病、偏头痛、右半球中风等)将是了解其生物学机制的关键,而认知科学、神经药理学和功能神经影像学将是这些研究的工具。基于这些症状,有可能理解心灵的感知规则和人类意识的机制。然而,幻觉和妄想在精神疾病如精神病和精神分裂症中表现得异常强烈,没有证据表明有脑损伤。此外,它们是主观症状,没有生物学标记。因此,它们往往具有高度的个体间可变性,并取决于其他变量(如教育程度、创伤史),因此难以简化为明确的构念。致病机制可能有多种。为了理解这些症状,神经病学和精神病学之间仍然缺乏一个共同的框架。19世纪法国异己主义者的精神病理学,弗洛伊德和亨利·伊在20世纪的精神病理学,在20世纪下半叶,让位给了DSM和神经科学,以追求纯粹的神经学视角。然而,尽管心理动力学模型现在(在技术时代)似乎没有那么有影响力,但专注于情感-认知范式的详细临床评估可能是导致新的神经生物学研究的唯一途径。
{"title":"Neurology versus Psychiatry? Hallucinations, Delusions, and Confabulations.","authors":"Antonio Carota,&nbsp;Julien Bogousslavsky","doi":"10.1159/000494960","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494960","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Hallucinations, delusions, and confabulations are common symptoms between neurology and psychiatry. The neurological diseases manifesting with such symptoms (dementia, epilepsy, Korsakoff's disease, brain tumors, Parkinson's disease, migraine, right hemisphere stroke and others) would be the key to understand their biological mechanisms, while the cognitive sciences, neuropharmacology and functional neuroimaging would be the tools of such researches. It is possible to understand the perceptive rules of the mind and the mechanisms of the human consciousness based on these symptoms. However, hallucinations and delusions manifest with extraordinary vehemence with psychiatric disorders such as psychosis and schizophrenia, with which there is no evidence of brain lesions. Furthermore, they are subjective symptoms, and they do not have biological markers. Hence, they are prone to high inter-individual variability and depend on other variables (such as education, history of trauma), and are therefore difficult to reduce to unequivocal constructs. Causative mechanisms are probably multiple. For understanding these symptoms, a common framework between neurology and psychiatry is still missing. The psychopathology of French alienists over the 19th century, of S. Freud, and of Henry Ey over the 20th century gave way, in the second half of the 20th century, to the adoption of the DSM and neurosciences, to pursue a pure neurological perspective. However, although psychodynamic models seem nowadays (in a technological era) less influential, detailed clinical evaluations focusing on emotional-cognitive paradigms are probably the only way to lead to new neurobiological researches.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494960","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37350116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
History of Anosognosia. 病感失认症病史。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494954
Guido Gainotti
Even if Babinski (1914) is usually considered as the discoverer of anosognosia, other authors before him contributed to the development of this construct. Von Monakow (1885) and Dejerine and Vialet (1893) gave the first descriptions of patients with cortical blindness who were unaware of their disability, but did not distinguish this unawareness from the rest of the clinical description. Anton (1999) described patients with cortical deafness and cortical blindness, considering these defects of awareness as a symptom independent from the neurological dysfunction. He conceptualized them as a phenomenon in its own right and tried to link this unawareness of a disability with specific neuro-anatomical changes. Finally, Babinski (1914) coined the term "anosognosia" to designate the clinical entity conceptualized by Anton (1899) and extended this concept from the unawareness of cortical deafness and blindness to the unawareness of hemiplegia. The choice of the term "anosognosia" to denote the observed phenomenon was important, because referring to "lack of knowledge of the disease" (anosognosia), he not only emphasized the separation between "lack of knowledge" and "disease, " but also suggested a general use of this term, because disease can refer to many other disabilities besides hemiplegia. Further investigations have shown that: (a) brain-damaged patients may be unaware of different kinds of disabilities; (b) anosognosia can be selective, in that an affected person with multiple impairments may be unaware of only one handicap, while appearing fully aware of any others; and (c) lack of acknowledgment of a disease may not necessarily be due to a defective awareness, but must sometimes be considered as an extreme but understable pattern of adaptation to stress. For this condition, the term "Denial of Illness" seems preferable to that of anosognosia. Anosognosia must perhaps be viewed as a multifaceted phenomenon, resulting from both cognitive and motivational factors.
即使巴宾斯基(Babinski, 1914)通常被认为是病感失认症的发现者,在他之前的其他作者也为这一概念的发展做出了贡献。Von Monakow(1885)和Dejerine and Vialet(1893)首次对皮质性失明患者进行了描述,他们不知道自己的残疾,但没有将这种不知道与其他临床描述区分开来。Anton(1999)描述了皮质性耳聋和皮质性失明的患者,认为这些意识缺陷是独立于神经功能障碍的症状。他将其视为一种现象,并试图将这种对残疾的无知与特定的神经解剖学变化联系起来。最后,Babinski(1914)创造了“病感失认症”一词来指代Anton(1899)提出的临床实体概念,并将这一概念从皮质性耳聋和失明的无意识扩展到偏瘫的无意识。选择“病感失认”一词来表示观察到的现象是很重要的,因为在提到“对疾病缺乏了解”(病感失认)时,他不仅强调了“缺乏了解”和“疾病”之间的区别,而且还建议普遍使用这个术语,因为疾病可以指除偏瘫以外的许多其他残疾。进一步的调查显示:(a)脑损伤患者可能不知道不同种类的残疾;(b)病感失认症可以是选择性的,因为患有多种障碍的患者可能只意识不到一种障碍,而似乎完全意识到其他任何障碍;(c)对疾病缺乏认识不一定是由于认识有缺陷,但有时必须被视为对压力的极端但不稳定的适应模式。对于这种情况,术语“否认疾病”似乎比病感失认更合适。病感失认症可能是一个多方面的现象,由认知和动机因素共同导致。
{"title":"History of Anosognosia.","authors":"Guido Gainotti","doi":"10.1159/000494954","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494954","url":null,"abstract":"Even if Babinski (1914) is usually considered as the discoverer of anosognosia, other authors before him contributed to the development of this construct. Von Monakow (1885) and Dejerine and Vialet (1893) gave the first descriptions of patients with cortical blindness who were unaware of their disability, but did not distinguish this unawareness from the rest of the clinical description. Anton (1999) described patients with cortical deafness and cortical blindness, considering these defects of awareness as a symptom independent from the neurological dysfunction. He conceptualized them as a phenomenon in its own right and tried to link this unawareness of a disability with specific neuro-anatomical changes. Finally, Babinski (1914) coined the term \"anosognosia\" to designate the clinical entity conceptualized by Anton (1899) and extended this concept from the unawareness of cortical deafness and blindness to the unawareness of hemiplegia. The choice of the term \"anosognosia\" to denote the observed phenomenon was important, because referring to \"lack of knowledge of the disease\" (anosognosia), he not only emphasized the separation between \"lack of knowledge\" and \"disease, \" but also suggested a general use of this term, because disease can refer to many other disabilities besides hemiplegia. Further investigations have shown that: (a) brain-damaged patients may be unaware of different kinds of disabilities; (b) anosognosia can be selective, in that an affected person with multiple impairments may be unaware of only one handicap, while appearing fully aware of any others; and (c) lack of acknowledgment of a disease may not necessarily be due to a defective awareness, but must sometimes be considered as an extreme but understable pattern of adaptation to stress. For this condition, the term \"Denial of Illness\" seems preferable to that of anosognosia. Anosognosia must perhaps be viewed as a multifaceted phenomenon, resulting from both cognitive and motivational factors.","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494954","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
History of "Frontal" Syndromes and Executive Dysfunction. “额叶”综合征和执行功能障碍的病史。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494957
Jonathan DeRight

Elements of "frontal" syndromes and executive dysfunction have been pondered by humans since ancient times, perhaps because executive dysfunction often threatens the very characteristics that make us human. This chapter provides a historical account of scientific advancements related to frontal lobe functioning and how the term has transformed over time. From ancient Greek philosophy to early neuroscientific animal studies to the default mode network, knowledge about the neural underpinnings of executive functioning has blossomed, almost so broadly that the behemoth term comprising broad neuropsychological functions may struggle to be provide specificity on its own without further clarification.

自古以来,人类就一直在思考“额叶”综合症和执行功能障碍的因素,也许是因为执行功能障碍经常威胁到使我们成为人类的特征。本章提供了与额叶功能相关的科学进步的历史记录,以及该术语如何随着时间的推移而变化。从古希腊哲学到早期的神经科学动物研究,再到默认模式网络,关于执行功能的神经基础的知识已经开花结果,几乎是如此广泛,以至于这个包含广泛神经心理学功能的庞然大物可能很难在没有进一步澄清的情况下单独提供特异性。
{"title":"History of \"Frontal\" Syndromes and Executive Dysfunction.","authors":"Jonathan DeRight","doi":"10.1159/000494957","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494957","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Elements of \"frontal\" syndromes and executive dysfunction have been pondered by humans since ancient times, perhaps because executive dysfunction often threatens the very characteristics that make us human. This chapter provides a historical account of scientific advancements related to frontal lobe functioning and how the term has transformed over time. From ancient Greek philosophy to early neuroscientific animal studies to the default mode network, knowledge about the neural underpinnings of executive functioning has blossomed, almost so broadly that the behemoth term comprising broad neuropsychological functions may struggle to be provide specificity on its own without further clarification.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494957","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347196","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
The Discovery of Cerebral Specialization. 大脑专业化的发现。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494938
Lauren Julius Harris

Of the main principles of human neuropsychology, the best known may be cerebral specialization: the left and right hemispheres play different roles in language and other higher-order functions. This chapter discusses when and how and by whom the differences were found. It begins with an account of Gall's cortical localization theory, which set the stage. It then describes the discoveries themselves, reviews how the differences were explained, and concludes with a summary of further developments.

在人类神经心理学的主要原理中,最有名的可能是大脑的专业化:左右半球在语言和其他高级功能中扮演不同的角色。本章讨论了何时、如何以及由谁发现这些差异。它以加尔的大脑皮层定位理论开始,这个理论奠定了基础。然后描述这些发现本身,回顾如何解释这些差异,最后总结进一步的发展。
{"title":"The Discovery of Cerebral Specialization.","authors":"Lauren Julius Harris","doi":"10.1159/000494938","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494938","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Of the main principles of human neuropsychology, the best known may be cerebral specialization: the left and right hemispheres play different roles in language and other higher-order functions. This chapter discusses when and how and by whom the differences were found. It begins with an account of Gall's cortical localization theory, which set the stage. It then describes the discoveries themselves, reviews how the differences were explained, and concludes with a summary of further developments.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494938","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Hemineglect and Attentional Dysfunction. 半忽视和注意力障碍。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494956
Karen G Langer, Bartlomiej Piechowski-Jozwiak, Julien Bogousslavsky

Tracing the history of neglect is intriguing, as diverse terminologies have been used to characterize a multi-factorial disorder with rather startling manifestations. In part, heterogeneous terms may have hinted at distinct subtypes. Thus, different variants of hemi-inattention and neglect relate conceptually, but may be functionally dissociable. Patients with neglect, acting as if the world-space they perceive is full, do not phenomenally experience the omissions or absences so patently obvious to an observer. From the late 19th century, hemi-inattention was described according to its prominent manifestations, visual, bodily or spatial. Since then, diverse terms including imperception, inattention, unilateral visual inattention, unilateral spatial agnosia, and neglect, among others, reflected proposed underlying mechanisms. Major theories presented to account for this curious, even astonishing, neurological disorder, included disruption of body-scheme, perceptual rivalry and extinction, forgetting or amnesia for half the body, and highly nuanced models of distribution of directed spatial attention, and of disrupted perceptual processes. Unlike neurological counterparts, already designated as hemi-syndromes by the first part of the 20th century, not until about 1970 did neglect become so broadly recognized as a syndrome. Earlier, commonalities were identified, features conceptually clustered, and then subtypes were distinguished. Neglect was designated as an overarching term for a class of disorder with distinct subtypes, including visual, motor, extrapersonal, bodily or personal, other somatosensory, and representational. Specificity for modality, chronology, material, and symptom severity was noted. Remarkable clinical, neuropsychological, and behavioral manifestations of hemi-inattention and neglect may involve varying proposed mechanisms of higher cognitive functions, all within a spectrum of clinical disorder. Concepts of connectivity and interaction, neural networks, and functional integration enhance understanding of dysfunction, recovery, and compensation in neglect and inattention. Focus on distinct manifestations clustered under the umbrella of neglect offers a vantage point for examining historical trends in approach to the phenomenon.

追踪忽视的历史是有趣的,因为不同的术语被用来描述一种具有相当惊人表现的多因素疾病。在某种程度上,异质术语可能暗示了不同的亚型。因此,半注意力不集中和忽视的不同变体在概念上是相关的,但在功能上可能是可分离的。被忽视的病人,表现得好像他们感知到的世界空间是满的,不会明显地体验到对观察者来说如此明显的遗漏或缺失。从19世纪后期开始,半注意力不集中被描述为根据其突出的表现,视觉,身体或空间。从那时起,各种各样的术语,包括不知觉、不注意、单侧视觉不注意、单侧空间失认和忽视等,反映了提出的潜在机制。解释这种奇怪甚至令人震惊的神经紊乱的主要理论包括身体结构的破坏、知觉竞争和消失、半个身体的遗忘或健忘症,以及定向空间注意力分布和知觉过程中断的高度细致的模型。与20世纪上半叶已经被指定为半综合征的神经系统疾病不同,直到1970年左右,忽视才被广泛认为是一种综合征。在此之前,识别共性,概念聚类特征,然后区分亚型。忽视被指定为具有不同亚型的一类障碍的总体术语,包括视觉,运动,超个人,身体或个人,其他躯体感觉和代表性。特异性的形式,时间,材料和症状的严重程度被注意。半注意力不集中和忽视的显著临床、神经心理学和行为表现可能涉及不同的高级认知功能机制,所有这些都在临床障碍的范围内。连通性和相互作用、神经网络和功能整合的概念增强了对忽视和注意力不集中的功能障碍、恢复和补偿的理解。关注在忽视的保护伞下聚集的不同表现,为研究这一现象的历史趋势提供了有利条件。
{"title":"Hemineglect and Attentional Dysfunction.","authors":"Karen G Langer,&nbsp;Bartlomiej Piechowski-Jozwiak,&nbsp;Julien Bogousslavsky","doi":"10.1159/000494956","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494956","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Tracing the history of neglect is intriguing, as diverse terminologies have been used to characterize a multi-factorial disorder with rather startling manifestations. In part, heterogeneous terms may have hinted at distinct subtypes. Thus, different variants of hemi-inattention and neglect relate conceptually, but may be functionally dissociable. Patients with neglect, acting as if the world-space they perceive is full, do not phenomenally experience the omissions or absences so patently obvious to an observer. From the late 19th century, hemi-inattention was described according to its prominent manifestations, visual, bodily or spatial. Since then, diverse terms including imperception, inattention, unilateral visual inattention, unilateral spatial agnosia, and neglect, among others, reflected proposed underlying mechanisms. Major theories presented to account for this curious, even astonishing, neurological disorder, included disruption of body-scheme, perceptual rivalry and extinction, forgetting or amnesia for half the body, and highly nuanced models of distribution of directed spatial attention, and of disrupted perceptual processes. Unlike neurological counterparts, already designated as hemi-syndromes by the first part of the 20th century, not until about 1970 did neglect become so broadly recognized as a syndrome. Earlier, commonalities were identified, features conceptually clustered, and then subtypes were distinguished. Neglect was designated as an overarching term for a class of disorder with distinct subtypes, including visual, motor, extrapersonal, bodily or personal, other somatosensory, and representational. Specificity for modality, chronology, material, and symptom severity was noted. Remarkable clinical, neuropsychological, and behavioral manifestations of hemi-inattention and neglect may involve varying proposed mechanisms of higher cognitive functions, all within a spectrum of clinical disorder. Concepts of connectivity and interaction, neural networks, and functional integration enhance understanding of dysfunction, recovery, and compensation in neglect and inattention. Focus on distinct manifestations clustered under the umbrella of neglect offers a vantage point for examining historical trends in approach to the phenomenon.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494956","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347172","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
History of Amusia. 失语症的历史。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494955
Mitsuru Kawamura, Michael W Miller

We live in a world surrounded by sound. Throughout life, we are exposed to music: from lullabies and songs taught at school to instrumental music both heard and played for pleasure. Every nation, along with its own language, has unique forms of music and dance. "Music knows no boundaries," as the saying goes. Just as language impairment is known as "aphasia," impairment of the perception of music is called "amusia." In this article, we will first classify the types of amusia. This will be followed by an introduction to the classical research of Salomon Eberhard Henschen (1847-1930), and to a discussion of higher auditory functions in which we highlight cases of amusia encountered in a person and through the literature.

我们生活在一个被声音包围的世界。纵观一生,我们都接触到音乐:从学校教的摇篮曲和歌曲,到为娱乐而听和演奏的器乐。每个民族都有自己的语言和独特的音乐和舞蹈形式。俗话说:“音乐没有边界。”正如语言障碍被称为“失语症”一样,音乐感知障碍被称为“失音症”。在这篇文章中,我们将首先对失音症的类型进行分类。接下来将介绍所罗门·埃伯哈德·亨申(1847-1930)的经典研究,并讨论高级听觉功能,其中我们将重点介绍在个人和文献中遇到的失音症病例。
{"title":"History of Amusia.","authors":"Mitsuru Kawamura,&nbsp;Michael W Miller","doi":"10.1159/000494955","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We live in a world surrounded by sound. Throughout life, we are exposed to music: from lullabies and songs taught at school to instrumental music both heard and played for pleasure. Every nation, along with its own language, has unique forms of music and dance. \"Music knows no boundaries,\" as the saying goes. Just as language impairment is known as \"aphasia,\" impairment of the perception of music is called \"amusia.\" In this article, we will first classify the types of amusia. This will be followed by an introduction to the classical research of Salomon Eberhard Henschen (1847-1930), and to a discussion of higher auditory functions in which we highlight cases of amusia encountered in a person and through the literature.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494955","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347194","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Kanji (Morphogram) and Kana (Phonogram) Problem in Japanese Alexia and Agraphia. 日语失读和失写症中的汉字(形)和假名(音)问题。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494952
Yasuhisa Sakurai

The kanji and kana (or kanji vs. kana) problem in the Japanese language denotes the dissociation between kanji (morphograms) and kana (phonograms) in reading/comprehension and writing. Since paragraphia of kana in a patient with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis was first reported in 1893, kanji-kana dissociation has been the central topic in Japanese aphasiology. Recent advancements in lesion-to-symptom analyses and functional imaging studies have identified some areas whose damage causes dissociative disturbances of reading or writing between kanji and kana. That is, (1) angular alexia with agraphia causes kanji agraphia; alexia of kana with an angular gyrus lesion is the result of a damage to the middle occipital gyrus; (2) alexia with agraphia for kanji is caused by a posterior inferior temporal cortex (mid-fusiform/inferior temporal gyri; visual word form area) lesion, whereas pure agraphia for kanji is caused by a posterior middle temporal gyrus lesion; and (3) pure alexia, particularly for kanji, results from a mid-fusiform gyrus lesion (Brodmann's Area [BA] 37), whereas pure alexia for kana results from a posterior fusiform/inferior occipital gyri lesion (BA 18/19).

日语中的汉字和假名(或汉字与假名)问题表明,在阅读/理解和写作中,汉字(词形)和假名(音)之间存在分离。自1893年首次报道肌萎缩性侧索硬化症患者假名分段以来,假名与假名的分离一直是日本失语症的中心话题。近年来在病征分析和功能成像研究方面的进展已经确定了一些区域的损伤导致读写汉字和假名之间的分离性障碍。即(1)角型失读伴失写症引起汉字失写;假名失读伴角回损伤是枕中回损伤的结果;(2)失读症伴汉字失写症是由后颞下皮层(中梭状回/颞下回)引起的;视觉词形区损伤,而单纯的汉字失写症是由后颞中回损伤引起的;(3)纯粹的失读症,特别是汉字,是由梭状回中部损伤引起的(Brodmann's Area [BA] 37),而假名的纯粹失读症是由梭状回后部/枕下回损伤引起的(BA 18/19)。
{"title":"Kanji (Morphogram) and Kana (Phonogram) Problem in Japanese Alexia and Agraphia.","authors":"Yasuhisa Sakurai","doi":"10.1159/000494952","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494952","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The kanji and kana (or kanji vs. kana) problem in the Japanese language denotes the dissociation between kanji (morphograms) and kana (phonograms) in reading/comprehension and writing. Since paragraphia of kana in a patient with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis was first reported in 1893, kanji-kana dissociation has been the central topic in Japanese aphasiology. Recent advancements in lesion-to-symptom analyses and functional imaging studies have identified some areas whose damage causes dissociative disturbances of reading or writing between kanji and kana. That is, (1) angular alexia with agraphia causes kanji agraphia; alexia of kana with an angular gyrus lesion is the result of a damage to the middle occipital gyrus; (2) alexia with agraphia for kanji is caused by a posterior inferior temporal cortex (mid-fusiform/inferior temporal gyri; visual word form area) lesion, whereas pure agraphia for kanji is caused by a posterior middle temporal gyrus lesion; and (3) pure alexia, particularly for kanji, results from a mid-fusiform gyrus lesion (Brodmann's Area [BA] 37), whereas pure alexia for kana results from a posterior fusiform/inferior occipital gyri lesion (BA 18/19).</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494952","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37347195","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Historical Pathway from Description of Cognitive Recovery to Formal Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 从认知恢复描述到正式神经心理康复的历史路径。
Q3 Medicine Pub Date : 2019-01-01 Epub Date: 2019-04-30 DOI: 10.1159/000494962
Eloi Magnin, Ilham Ryff, Baptiste Brun, Pierre Decavel, Sebastien Hague, Thierry Moulin

Neuropsychological rehabilitation is one of the subspecialties of neuropsychology, along with neuropsychological assessment, cognitive process descriptions, and anatomo-functional correlation, but it is still frequently underrecognized, even from a historical point of view. In this chronological review, we propose following some of the historical descriptions of cognitive recovery, and the suggested procedures and therapies to improve this recovery from mythological periods and the antiquity to recent contemporary periods and the birth of formal neuropsychological rehabilitation in neurological and psychiatric conditions.

神经心理康复是神经心理学的一个亚专业,与神经心理学评估、认知过程描述和解剖功能相关性一起,但即使从历史的角度来看,它仍然经常被低估。在这篇按时间顺序的回顾中,我们提出了一些关于认知恢复的历史描述,以及从神话时期和古代到近代的建议程序和治疗方法,以及在神经和精神疾病中正式的神经心理康复的诞生。
{"title":"Historical Pathway from Description of Cognitive Recovery to Formal Neuropsychological Rehabilitation.","authors":"Eloi Magnin,&nbsp;Ilham Ryff,&nbsp;Baptiste Brun,&nbsp;Pierre Decavel,&nbsp;Sebastien Hague,&nbsp;Thierry Moulin","doi":"10.1159/000494962","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000494962","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Neuropsychological rehabilitation is one of the subspecialties of neuropsychology, along with neuropsychological assessment, cognitive process descriptions, and anatomo-functional correlation, but it is still frequently underrecognized, even from a historical point of view. In this chronological review, we propose following some of the historical descriptions of cognitive recovery, and the suggested procedures and therapies to improve this recovery from mythological periods and the antiquity to recent contemporary periods and the birth of formal neuropsychological rehabilitation in neurological and psychiatric conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":35285,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000494962","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37350115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1