首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Language Evolution最新文献

英文 中文
The emergence of systematic argument distinctions in artificial sign languages 人工手语中系统论证差异的出现
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2021-06-18 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzab002
Yasamin Motamedi, Kenny Smith, Marieke Schouwstra, Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby
Word order is a key property by which languages indicate the relationship between a predicate and its arguments. However, sign languages use a number of other modality-specific tools in addition to word order such as spatial agreement, which has been likened to verbal agreement in spoken languages, and role shift, where the signer takes on characteristics of propositional agents. In particular, data from emerging sign languages suggest that, though some use of a conventional word order can appear within a few generations, systematic spatial modulation as a grammatical feature takes time to develop. We experimentally examine the emergence of systematic argument marking beyond word order, investigating how artificial gestural systems evolve over generations of participants in the lab. We find that participants converge on different strategies to disambiguate clause arguments, which become more consistent through the use and transmission of gestures; in some cases, this leads to conventionalized iconic spatial contrasts, comparable to those found in natural sign languages. We discuss how our results connect with theoretical issues surrounding the analysis of spatial agreement and role shift in established and newly emerging sign languages, and the possible mechanisms behind its evolution.
语序是语言用来表示谓词及其参数之间关系的关键属性。然而,除了语序之外,手语还使用了许多其他特定于情态的工具,比如空间协议,这被比作口头语言中的口头协议,以及角色转换,在这种情况下,签字人具有命题代理的特征。特别是,来自新兴手语的数据表明,尽管一些传统词序的使用可以在几代人内出现,但系统的空间调制作为一种语法特征需要时间来发展。我们通过实验研究了超越语序的系统论证标记的出现,研究了人工手势系统是如何在实验室的几代参与者中进化的。我们发现参与者倾向于使用不同的策略来消除子句论点的歧义,这些策略通过手势的使用和传递变得更加一致;在某些情况下,这导致了约定俗成的标志性空间对比,与自然手语中的对比相当。我们讨论了我们的研究结果如何与现有和新兴手语中空间一致性和角色转换分析的理论问题联系起来,以及其演变背后的可能机制。
{"title":"The emergence of systematic argument distinctions in artificial sign languages","authors":"Yasamin Motamedi, Kenny Smith, Marieke Schouwstra, Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzab002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzab002","url":null,"abstract":"Word order is a key property by which languages indicate the relationship between a predicate and its arguments. However, sign languages use a number of other modality-specific tools in addition to word order such as spatial agreement, which has been likened to verbal agreement in spoken languages, and role shift, where the signer takes on characteristics of propositional agents. In particular, data from emerging sign languages suggest that, though some use of a conventional word order can appear within a few generations, systematic spatial modulation as a grammatical feature takes time to develop. We experimentally examine the emergence of systematic argument marking beyond word order, investigating how artificial gestural systems evolve over generations of participants in the lab. We find that participants converge on different strategies to disambiguate clause arguments, which become more consistent through the use and transmission of gestures; in some cases, this leads to conventionalized iconic spatial contrasts, comparable to those found in natural sign languages. We discuss how our results connect with theoretical issues surrounding the analysis of spatial agreement and role shift in established and newly emerging sign languages, and the possible mechanisms behind its evolution.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138519804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The emergence of systematic argument distinctions in artificial sign languages 人工手语中系统论证差异的出现
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2021-06-18 DOI: 10.7488/DS/2852
Yasamin Motamedi, Kenny Smith, M. Schouwstra, J. Culbertson, S. Kirby
Word order is a key property by which languages indicate the relationship between a predicate and its arguments. However, sign languages use a number of other modality-specific tools in addition to word order such as spatial agreement, which has been likened to verbal agreement in spoken languages, and role shift, where the signer takes on characteristics of propositional agents. In particular, data from emerging sign languages suggest that, though some use of a conventional word order can appear within a few generations, systematic spatial modulation as a grammatical feature takes time to develop. We experimentally examine the emergence of systematic argument marking beyond word order, investigating how artificial gestural systems evolve over generations of participants in the lab. We find that participants converge on different strategies to disambiguate clause arguments, which become more consistent through the use and transmission of gestures; in some cases, this leads to conventionalized iconic spatial contrasts, comparable to those found in natural sign languages. We discuss how our results connect with theoretical issues surrounding the analysis of spatial agreement and role shift in established and newly emerging sign languages, and the possible mechanisms behind its evolution.
语序是语言用来表示谓词及其参数之间关系的关键属性。然而,除了语序之外,手语还使用了许多其他特定于情态的工具,比如空间协议,这被比作口头语言中的口头协议,以及角色转换,在这种情况下,签字人具有命题代理的特征。特别是,来自新兴手语的数据表明,尽管一些传统词序的使用可以在几代人内出现,但系统的空间调制作为一种语法特征需要时间来发展。我们通过实验研究了超越语序的系统论证标记的出现,研究了人工手势系统是如何在实验室的几代参与者中进化的。我们发现参与者倾向于使用不同的策略来消除子句论点的歧义,这些策略通过手势的使用和传递变得更加一致;在某些情况下,这导致了约定俗成的标志性空间对比,与自然手语中的对比相当。我们讨论了我们的研究结果如何与现有和新兴手语中空间一致性和角色转换分析的理论问题联系起来,以及其演变背后的可能机制。
{"title":"The emergence of systematic argument distinctions in artificial sign languages","authors":"Yasamin Motamedi, Kenny Smith, M. Schouwstra, J. Culbertson, S. Kirby","doi":"10.7488/DS/2852","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.7488/DS/2852","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Word order is a key property by which languages indicate the relationship between a predicate and its arguments. However, sign languages use a number of other modality-specific tools in addition to word order such as spatial agreement, which has been likened to verbal agreement in spoken languages, and role shift, where the signer takes on characteristics of propositional agents. In particular, data from emerging sign languages suggest that, though some use of a conventional word order can appear within a few generations, systematic spatial modulation as a grammatical feature takes time to develop. We experimentally examine the emergence of systematic argument marking beyond word order, investigating how artificial gestural systems evolve over generations of participants in the lab. We find that participants converge on different strategies to disambiguate clause arguments, which become more consistent through the use and transmission of gestures; in some cases, this leads to conventionalized iconic spatial contrasts, comparable to those found in natural sign languages. We discuss how our results connect with theoretical issues surrounding the analysis of spatial agreement and role shift in established and newly emerging sign languages, and the possible mechanisms behind its evolution.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46113860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Cultural evolution leads to vocal iconicity in an experimental iterated learning task 在一项实验性迭代学习任务中,文化进化导致声音象似性
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2021-05-06 DOI: 10.1093/JOLE/LZAB001
Niklas Erben Johansson, J. Carr, S. Kirby
Experimental and cross-linguistic studies have shown that vocal iconicity is prevalent in words that carry meanings related to SIZE and SHAPE. Although these studies demonstrate the importance of vocal iconicity and reveal the cognitive biases underpinning it, there is less work demonstrating how these biases lead to the evolution of a sound symbolic lexicon in the first place. In this study, we show how words can be shaped by cognitive biases through cultural evolution. Using a simple experimental setup resembling the game telephone, we examined how a single word form changed as it was passed from one participant to the next by a process of immediate iterated learning. About 1,500 naı̈ve participants were recruited online and divided into five condition groups. The participants in the CONTROL-group received no information about the meaning of the word they were about to hear, while the participants in the remaining four groups were informed that the word meant either BIG or SMALL (with the meaning being presented in text), or ROUND or POINTY (with the meaning being presented as a picture). The first participant in a transmission chain was presented with a phonetically diverse word and asked to repeat it. Thereafter, the recording of the repeated word was played for the next participant in the same chain. The sounds of the audio recordings were then transcribed and categorized according to six binary sound parameters. By modelling the proportion of vowels or consonants for each sound parameter, the SMALL-condition showed increases of FRONT UNROUNDED vowels and the POINTY-condition increases of ACUTE consonants. The results show that linguistic transmission is sufficient for vocal iconicity to emerge, which demonstrates the role non-arbitrary associations play in the evolution of language.
实验和跨语言研究表明,声乐象似性在具有与大小和形状相关的含义的单词中普遍存在。尽管这些研究证明了声音象似性的重要性,并揭示了支撑它的认知偏见,但很少有研究表明这些偏见最初是如何导致声音符号词典的演变的。在这项研究中,我们展示了认知偏见如何通过文化进化来塑造单词。使用一个类似于游戏电话的简单实验装置,我们研究了一个单词形式在通过即时迭代学习过程从一个参与者传递到下一个参与者时是如何变化的。大约1500名naıve参与者在网上被招募,并被分为五个条件组。对照组的参与者没有收到关于他们即将听到的单词含义的信息,而其余四组的参与者被告知,这个单词的意思是BIG或SMALL(以文本形式呈现),或者ROUND或POINTY(以图片形式呈现)。传输链中的第一个参与者被出示一个发音不同的单词,并被要求重复。然后,为同一链中的下一个参与者播放重复单词的录音。然后根据六个二进制声音参数对录音的声音进行转录和分类。通过对每个声音参数的元音或辅音比例进行建模,SMALL条件显示FRONT UNROUNDED元音增加,POINTY条件显示ACUTE辅音增加。结果表明,语言的传递足以产生声音象似性,这表明非任意联想在语言进化中所起的作用。
{"title":"Cultural evolution leads to vocal iconicity in an experimental iterated learning task","authors":"Niklas Erben Johansson, J. Carr, S. Kirby","doi":"10.1093/JOLE/LZAB001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/JOLE/LZAB001","url":null,"abstract":"Experimental and cross-linguistic studies have shown that vocal iconicity is prevalent in words that carry meanings related to SIZE and SHAPE. Although these studies demonstrate the importance of vocal iconicity and reveal the cognitive biases underpinning it, there is less work demonstrating how these biases lead to the evolution of a sound symbolic lexicon in the first place. In this study, we show how words can be shaped by cognitive biases through cultural evolution. Using a simple experimental setup resembling the game telephone, we examined how a single word form changed as it was passed from one participant to the next by a process of immediate iterated learning. About 1,500 naı̈ve participants were recruited online and divided into five condition groups. The participants in the CONTROL-group received no information about the meaning of the word they were about to hear, while the participants in the remaining four groups were informed that the word meant either BIG or SMALL (with the meaning being presented in text), or ROUND or POINTY (with the meaning being presented as a picture). The first participant in a transmission chain was presented with a phonetically diverse word and asked to repeat it. Thereafter, the recording of the repeated word was played for the next participant in the same chain. The sounds of the audio recordings were then transcribed and categorized according to six binary sound parameters. By modelling the proportion of vowels or consonants for each sound parameter, the SMALL-condition showed increases of FRONT UNROUNDED vowels and the POINTY-condition increases of ACUTE consonants. The results show that linguistic transmission is sufficient for vocal iconicity to emerge, which demonstrates the role non-arbitrary associations play in the evolution of language.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44567270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Constituent order in silent gesture reflects the perspective of the producer 无声手势中的组成秩序反映了生产者的视角
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2021-03-20 DOI: 10.1093/JOLE/LZAA010
Fiona Kirton, S. Kirby, Kenny Smith, J. Culbertson, M. Schouwstra
Understanding the relationship between human cognition and linguistic structure is a central theme in language evolution research. Numerous studies have investigated this question using the silent gesture paradigm in which participants describe events using only gesture and no speech. Research using this paradigm has found that Agent–Patient–Action (APV) is the most commonly produced gesture order, regardless of the producer’s native language. However, studies have uncovered a range of factors that influence ordering preferences. One such factor is salience, which has been suggested as a key determiner of word order. Specifically, humans, who are typically agents, are more salient than inanimate objects, so tend to be mentioned first. In this study, we investigated the role of salience in more detail and asked whether manipulating the salience of a human agent would modulate the tendency to express humans before objects. We found, first, that APV was less common than expected based on previous literature. Secondly, salience influenced the relative ordering of the patient and action, but not the agent and patient. For events involving a non-salient agent, participants typically expressed the patient before the action and vice versa for salient agents. Thirdly, participants typically omitted non-salient agents from their descriptions. We present details of a novel computational solution that infers the orders participants would have produced had they expressed all three constituents on every trial. Our analysis showed that events involving salient agents tended to elicit AVP; those involving a non-salient agent were typically described with APV, modulated by a strong tendency to omit the agent. We argue that these findings provide evidence that the effect of salience is realized through its effect on the perspective from which a producer frames an event.
理解人类认知与语言结构之间的关系是语言进化研究的中心主题。许多研究使用无声手势范式来调查这个问题,在无声手势范式中,参与者只使用手势而不使用言语来描述事件。使用这种范式的研究发现,无论生产者的母语如何,代理-患者-动作(APV)都是最常见的手势顺序。然而,研究发现了一系列影响订购偏好的因素。其中一个因素是显著性,它被认为是语序的关键决定因素。具体来说,人类通常是代理人,比无生命物体更突出,因此往往首先被提及。在这项研究中,我们更详细地调查了显著性的作用,并询问操纵人类主体的显著性是否会调节在物体之前表达人类的倾向。首先,我们发现APV的发病率低于先前文献的预期。其次,显著性影响患者和行动的相对顺序,但不影响代理人和患者。对于涉及非显著因素的事件,参与者通常在行动前表达患者,而显著因素则相反。第三,参与者通常会在描述中省略不突出的主体。我们介绍了一种新的计算解决方案的细节,该解决方案推断了参与者在每次试验中表达所有三种成分时会产生的顺序。我们的分析表明,涉及显著因素的事件往往会引发AVP;那些涉及非显著试剂的试剂通常用APV来描述,APV由省略试剂的强烈趋势来调节。我们认为,这些发现提供了证据,证明显著性的影响是通过其对生产者构建事件的视角的影响来实现的。
{"title":"Constituent order in silent gesture reflects the perspective of the producer","authors":"Fiona Kirton, S. Kirby, Kenny Smith, J. Culbertson, M. Schouwstra","doi":"10.1093/JOLE/LZAA010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/JOLE/LZAA010","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Understanding the relationship between human cognition and linguistic structure is a central theme in language evolution research. Numerous studies have investigated this question using the silent gesture paradigm in which participants describe events using only gesture and no speech. Research using this paradigm has found that Agent–Patient–Action (APV) is the most commonly produced gesture order, regardless of the producer’s native language. However, studies have uncovered a range of factors that influence ordering preferences. One such factor is salience, which has been suggested as a key determiner of word order. Specifically, humans, who are typically agents, are more salient than inanimate objects, so tend to be mentioned first. In this study, we investigated the role of salience in more detail and asked whether manipulating the salience of a human agent would modulate the tendency to express humans before objects. We found, first, that APV was less common than expected based on previous literature. Secondly, salience influenced the relative ordering of the patient and action, but not the agent and patient. For events involving a non-salient agent, participants typically expressed the patient before the action and vice versa for salient agents. Thirdly, participants typically omitted non-salient agents from their descriptions. We present details of a novel computational solution that infers the orders participants would have produced had they expressed all three constituents on every trial. Our analysis showed that events involving salient agents tended to elicit AVP; those involving a non-salient agent were typically described with APV, modulated by a strong tendency to omit the agent. We argue that these findings provide evidence that the effect of salience is realized through its effect on the perspective from which a producer frames an event.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2021-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/JOLE/LZAA010","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42219944","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Flexibility in wild infant chimpanzee vocal behavior 野生幼年黑猩猩发声行为的灵活性
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-12-02 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa009
G. Dezecache, K. Zuberbühler, Marina Davila-Ross, Christoph D. Dahl
How did human language evolve from earlier forms of communication? One way to address this question is to compare prelinguistic human vocal behavior with nonhuman primate calls. An important finding has been that, prior to speech and from early on, human infant vocal behavior exhibits functional flexibility, or the capacity to produce sounds that are not tied to one specific function. This is reflected in human infants’ use of single categories of protophones (precursors of speech sounds) in various affective circumstances, such that a given call type can occur in and express positive, neutral, or negative affective states, depending on the occasion. Nonhuman primate vocal behavior, in contrast, is seen as comparably inflexible, with different call types tied to specific functions and sometimes to specific affective states (e.g. screams mostly occur in negative circumstances). As a first step toward addressing this claim, we examined the vocal behavior of six wild infant chimpanzees during their first year of life. We found that the most common vocal signal, grunts, occurred in a range of contexts that were deemed positive, neutral, and negative. Using automated feature extraction and supervised learning algorithms, we also found acoustic variants of grunts produced in the affective contexts, suggesting gradation within this vocal category. In contrast, the second most common call type of infant chimpanzees, the whimpers, was produced in only one affective context, in line with standard models of nonhuman primate vocal behavior. Insofar as our affective categorization reflects infants’ true affective state, our results suggest that the most common chimpanzee vocalization, the grunt is not affectively bound. Affective decoupling is a prerequisite for chimpanzee grunts (and other vocal categories) to be deemed ‘functionally flexible’. If later confirmed to be a functionally flexible vocal type, this would indicate that the evolution of this foundational vocal capability occurred before the split between the Homo and Pan lineages.
人类语言是如何从早期的交流形式演变而来的?解决这个问题的一种方法是将语言前人类的发声行为与非人类灵长类动物的叫声进行比较。一个重要的发现是,在说话之前和从早期开始,人类婴儿的发声行为表现出功能灵活性,或产生与特定功能无关的声音的能力。这反映在人类婴儿在各种情感环境中使用单一类别的原音(语音的前兆),因此特定的呼叫类型可以出现在积极、中性或消极的情感状态中,并根据具体情况表达。相比之下,非人类灵长类动物的发声行为被认为是相对不灵活的,不同的叫声类型与特定的功能有关,有时与特定的情感状态有关(例如,尖叫大多发生在负面环境中)。作为解决这一问题的第一步,我们研究了六只野生幼年黑猩猩在出生第一年的发声行为。我们发现,最常见的声音信号,呼噜声,发生在一系列被认为是积极的、中性的和消极的环境中。使用自动特征提取和监督学习算法,我们还发现了在情感环境中产生的咕哝声的声学变体,这表明在这个声音类别中是分级的。相比之下,幼年黑猩猩第二常见的叫声类型,呜咽,只在一种情感环境中产生,符合非人类灵长类动物发声行为的标准模型。就我们的情感分类反映婴儿的真实情感状态而言,我们的研究结果表明,黑猩猩最常见的发声方式,呼噜声并没有被情感束缚。情感脱钩是黑猩猩咕哝声(和其他发声类别)被视为“功能灵活”的先决条件。如果后来被证实是一种功能灵活的发声类型,这将表明这种基本发声能力的进化发生在人和泛谱系分裂之前。
{"title":"Flexibility in wild infant chimpanzee vocal behavior","authors":"G. Dezecache, K. Zuberbühler, Marina Davila-Ross, Christoph D. Dahl","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa009","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 How did human language evolve from earlier forms of communication? One way to address this question is to compare prelinguistic human vocal behavior with nonhuman primate calls. An important finding has been that, prior to speech and from early on, human infant vocal behavior exhibits functional flexibility, or the capacity to produce sounds that are not tied to one specific function. This is reflected in human infants’ use of single categories of protophones (precursors of speech sounds) in various affective circumstances, such that a given call type can occur in and express positive, neutral, or negative affective states, depending on the occasion. Nonhuman primate vocal behavior, in contrast, is seen as comparably inflexible, with different call types tied to specific functions and sometimes to specific affective states (e.g. screams mostly occur in negative circumstances). As a first step toward addressing this claim, we examined the vocal behavior of six wild infant chimpanzees during their first year of life. We found that the most common vocal signal, grunts, occurred in a range of contexts that were deemed positive, neutral, and negative. Using automated feature extraction and supervised learning algorithms, we also found acoustic variants of grunts produced in the affective contexts, suggesting gradation within this vocal category. In contrast, the second most common call type of infant chimpanzees, the whimpers, was produced in only one affective context, in line with standard models of nonhuman primate vocal behavior. Insofar as our affective categorization reflects infants’ true affective state, our results suggest that the most common chimpanzee vocalization, the grunt is not affectively bound. Affective decoupling is a prerequisite for chimpanzee grunts (and other vocal categories) to be deemed ‘functionally flexible’. If later confirmed to be a functionally flexible vocal type, this would indicate that the evolution of this foundational vocal capability occurred before the split between the Homo and Pan lineages.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/jole/lzaa009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43609400","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
How vocal temporal parameters develop: a comparative study between humans and songbirds, two distantly related vocal learners 声乐时间参数是如何发展的:人类和鸣禽这两种亲缘关系遥远的声乐学习者之间的比较研究
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-11-28 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa008
M. Takahasi, K. Okanoya, R. Mazuka
Human infants acquire motor patterns for speech during the first several years of their lives. Sequential vocalizations such as human speech are complex behaviors, and the ability to learn new vocalizations is limited to only a few animal species. Vocalizations are generated through the coordination of three types of organs: namely, vocal, respiratory, and articulatory organs. Moreover, sophisticated temporal respiratory control might be necessary for sequential vocalization involving human speech. However, it remains unknown how coordination develops in human infants and if this developmental process is shared with other vocal learners. To answer these questions, we analyzed temporal parameters of sequential vocalizations during the first year in human infants and compared these developmental changes to song development in the Bengalese finch, another vocal learner. In human infants, early cry was also analyzed as an innate sequential vocalization. The following three temporal parameters of sequential vocalizations were measured: note duration (ND), inter-onset interval, and inter-note interval (INI). The results showed that both human infants and Bengalese finches had longer INIs than ND in the early phase. Gradually, the INI and ND converged to a similar range throughout development. While ND increased until 6 months of age in infants, the INI decreased up to 60 days posthatching in finches. Regarding infant cry, ND and INI were within similar ranges, but the INI was more stable in length than ND. In sequential vocalizations, temporal parameters developed early with subsequent articulatory stabilization in both vocal learners. However, this developmental change was accomplished in a species-specific manner. These findings could provide important insights into our understanding of the evolution of vocal learning.
人类婴儿在其生命的最初几年中获得了说话的运动模式。人类语音等连续发声是一种复杂的行为,学习新发声的能力仅限于少数动物物种。发声是通过三种器官的协调产生的:即发声器官、呼吸器官和发音器官。此外,复杂的时间呼吸控制对于涉及人类语音的顺序发声可能是必要的。然而,目前尚不清楚人类婴儿的协调能力是如何发展的,以及这种发展过程是否与其他声乐学习者共享。为了回答这些问题,我们分析了人类婴儿第一年连续发声的时间参数,并将这些发育变化与另一位声乐学习者孟加拉雀的歌曲发育进行了比较。在人类婴儿中,早期啼哭也被分析为一种天生的顺序发声。测量了顺序发声的以下三个时间参数:音符持续时间(ND)、发作间隔和音符间隔(INI)。结果表明,在早期阶段,人类婴儿和孟加拉雀的INIs都比ND长。在整个开发过程中,INI和ND逐渐收敛到相似的范围。ND增加至6 在婴儿的几个月大时,在给雀交配后60天,INI下降。关于婴儿哭闹,ND和INI在相似的范围内,但INI的长度比ND更稳定。在连续发声中,时间参数在早期发展,随后两个声乐学习者的发音稳定。然而,这种发育变化是以特定物种的方式完成的。这些发现可以为我们理解声乐学习的演变提供重要的见解。
{"title":"How vocal temporal parameters develop: a comparative study between humans and songbirds, two distantly related vocal learners","authors":"M. Takahasi, K. Okanoya, R. Mazuka","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa008","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 Human infants acquire motor patterns for speech during the first several years of their lives. Sequential vocalizations such as human speech are complex behaviors, and the ability to learn new vocalizations is limited to only a few animal species. Vocalizations are generated through the coordination of three types of organs: namely, vocal, respiratory, and articulatory organs. Moreover, sophisticated temporal respiratory control might be necessary for sequential vocalization involving human speech. However, it remains unknown how coordination develops in human infants and if this developmental process is shared with other vocal learners. To answer these questions, we analyzed temporal parameters of sequential vocalizations during the first year in human infants and compared these developmental changes to song development in the Bengalese finch, another vocal learner. In human infants, early cry was also analyzed as an innate sequential vocalization. The following three temporal parameters of sequential vocalizations were measured: note duration (ND), inter-onset interval, and inter-note interval (INI). The results showed that both human infants and Bengalese finches had longer INIs than ND in the early phase. Gradually, the INI and ND converged to a similar range throughout development. While ND increased until 6 months of age in infants, the INI decreased up to 60 days posthatching in finches. Regarding infant cry, ND and INI were within similar ranges, but the INI was more stable in length than ND. In sequential vocalizations, temporal parameters developed early with subsequent articulatory stabilization in both vocal learners. However, this developmental change was accomplished in a species-specific manner. These findings could provide important insights into our understanding of the evolution of vocal learning.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/jole/lzaa008","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49666785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
When the tune shapes morphology: The origins of vocatives 当曲调形成形态:祈使词的起源
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-09-01 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa007
Sóskuthy M, Roettger T.
Abstract
Many languages use pitch to express pragmatic meaning (henceforth ‘tune’). This requires segmental carriers with rich harmonic structure and high periodic energy, making vowels the optimal carriers of the tune. Tunes can be phonetically impoverished when there is a shortage of vowels, endangering the recovery of their function. This biases sound systems towards the optimisation of tune transmission by processes such as the insertion of vowels. Vocative constructions—used to attract and maintain the addressee’s attention—are often characterised by specific tunes. Many languages additionally mark vocatives morphologically. In this article, we argue that one potential pathway for the emergence of vocative morphemes is the morphological re-analysis of tune-driven phonetic variation that helps to carry pitch patterns. Looking at a corpus of 101 languages, we compare vocatives to structural case markers in terms of their phonological make-up. We find that vocatives are often characterised by additional prosodic modulation (vowel lengthening, stress shift, tone change) and contain substantially fewer consonants, supporting our hypothesis that the acoustic properties of tunes interact with segmental features and can shape the emergence of morphological markers. This fits with the view that the efficient transmission of information is a driving force in the evolution of languages, but also highlights the importance of defining ‘information’ broadly to include pragmatic, social, and affectual components alongside propositional meaning.
【摘要】许多语言用音高来表达语用意义(即“调”)。这就需要谐波结构丰富、周期能量高的音段载波,使元音成为曲调的最佳载波。当缺少元音时,曲调可能会出现语音贫乏,危及其功能的恢复。这使得声音系统倾向于通过插入元音等过程来优化音调传输。感召结构——用来吸引和保持收件人的注意力——通常以特定的曲调为特征。许多语言还在词形上标注了祈使词。在这篇文章中,我们认为一个潜在的途径是出现的音调驱动的语音变化的形态学重新分析,有助于携带音高模式。我们对101种语言的语料库进行了比较,从音系组成方面比较了呼动词和结构格标记。我们发现,感叹词通常以额外的韵律调节(元音延长、重音转移、音调变化)为特征,并且包含较少的辅音,这支持了我们的假设,即曲调的声学特性与片段特征相互作用,可以塑造形态标记的出现。这与信息的有效传递是语言进化的驱动力这一观点相吻合,但也强调了广泛定义“信息”的重要性,即除了命题意义外,还包括语用、社会和情感成分。
{"title":"When the tune shapes morphology: The origins of vocatives","authors":"Sóskuthy M, Roettger T.","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa007","url":null,"abstract":"<span><div>Abstract</div>Many languages use pitch to express pragmatic meaning (henceforth ‘tune’). This requires segmental carriers with rich harmonic structure and high periodic energy, making vowels the optimal carriers of the tune. Tunes can be phonetically impoverished when there is a shortage of vowels, endangering the recovery of their function. This biases sound systems towards the optimisation of tune transmission by processes such as the insertion of vowels. Vocative constructions—used to attract and maintain the addressee’s attention—are often characterised by specific tunes. Many languages additionally mark vocatives morphologically. In this article, we argue that one potential pathway for the emergence of vocative morphemes is the morphological re-analysis of tune-driven phonetic variation that helps to carry pitch patterns. Looking at a corpus of 101 languages, we compare vocatives to structural case markers in terms of their phonological make-up. We find that vocatives are often characterised by additional prosodic modulation (vowel lengthening, stress shift, tone change) and contain substantially fewer consonants, supporting our hypothesis that the acoustic properties of tunes interact with segmental features and can shape the emergence of morphological markers. This fits with the view that the efficient transmission of information is a driving force in the evolution of languages, but also highlights the importance of defining ‘information’ broadly to include pragmatic, social, and affectual components alongside propositional meaning.</span>","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138519802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Biting into evolution of language 语言的进化
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-08-01 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa003
M A C (Riny) Huybregts
{"title":"Biting into evolution of language","authors":"M A C (Riny) Huybregts","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa003","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/jole/lzaa003","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42370986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pantomime as the original human-specific communicative system 哑剧作为人类特有的原始交际系统
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-08-01 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa006
J. Zlatev, Przemysław Żywiczyński, Sławomir Wacewicz
We propose reframing one of the key questions in the field of language evolution as what was the original human-specific communicative system? With the help of cognitive semiotics, first we clarify the difference between signals, which characterize animal communication, and signs, which do not replace but complement signals in human communication. We claim that the evolution of bodily mimesis allowed for the use of signs, and the social-cognitive skills needed to support them to emerge in hominin evolution. Neither signs nor signals operate single-handedly, but as part of semiotic systems. Communicative systems can be either monosemiotic or polysemiotic—the former consisting of a single semiotic system and the latter, of several. Our proposal is that pantomime, as the original human-specific communicative system, should be characterized as polysemiotic: dominated by gesture but also including vocalization, facial expression, and possibly the rudiments of depiction. Given that pantomimic gestures must have been maximally similar to bodily actions, we characterize them as typically (1) dominated by iconicity, (2) of the primary kind, (3) involving the whole body, (4) performed from a first-person perspective, (5) concerning peripersonal space, and (6) using the Enacting mode of representation.
我们建议将语言进化领域的一个关键问题重新定义为最初的人类特定交际系统是什么?借助认知符号学,我们首先阐明了信号和符号之间的区别,信号是动物交流的特征,符号在人类交流中不是替代而是补充信号。我们声称,模仿身体的进化允许使用手势,以及支持手势在人类进化中出现所需的社会认知技能。符号和信号都不是单独运作的,而是符号系统的一部分。交际系统可以是单符号系统,也可以是多义系统——前者由单个符号系统组成,后者由几个符号系统组成。我们的建议是,哑剧作为人类特有的原始交际系统,应该被描述为多义现象:以手势为主,但也包括发声、面部表情,可能还有描绘的雏形。鉴于哑剧手势必须与身体动作最大程度地相似,我们将其描述为典型的(1)受象似性支配,(2)主要类型,(3)涉及整个身体,(4)从第一人称的角度进行,(5)涉及个人周围空间,以及(6)使用再现模式。
{"title":"Pantomime as the original human-specific communicative system","authors":"J. Zlatev, Przemysław Żywiczyński, Sławomir Wacewicz","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa006","url":null,"abstract":"We propose reframing one of the key questions in the field of language evolution as what was the original human-specific communicative system? With the help of cognitive semiotics, first we clarify the difference between signals, which characterize animal communication, and signs, which do not replace but complement signals in human communication. We claim that the evolution of bodily mimesis allowed for the use of signs, and the social-cognitive skills needed to support them to emerge in hominin evolution. Neither signs nor signals operate single-handedly, but as part of semiotic systems. Communicative systems can be either monosemiotic or polysemiotic—the former consisting of a single semiotic system and the latter, of several. Our proposal is that pantomime, as the original human-specific communicative system, should be characterized as polysemiotic: dominated by gesture but also including vocalization, facial expression, and possibly the rudiments of depiction. Given that pantomimic gestures must have been maximally similar to bodily actions, we characterize them as typically (1) dominated by iconicity, (2) of the primary kind, (3) involving the whole body, (4) performed from a first-person perspective, (5) concerning peripersonal space, and (6) using the Enacting mode of representation.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/jole/lzaa006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49600002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 41
Dispersion, communication, and alignment: an experimental study of the emergence of structure in combinatorial phonology 分散、交流和对齐:组合音系中结构出现的实验研究
IF 2.6 Pub Date : 2020-08-01 DOI: 10.1093/jole/lzaa004
Gareth Roberts, R. Clark
Languages exhibit structure at a number of levels, including at the level of phonology, the system of meaningless combinatorial units from which words are constructed. Phonological systems typically exhibit greater dispersion than would be expected by chance. Several theoretical models have been proposed to account for this, and a common theme is that such organization emerges as a result of the competing forces acting on production and perception. Fundamentally, this implies a cultural evolutionary explanation, by which emergent organization is an adaptive response to the pressures of communicative interaction. This process is hard to investigate empirically using natural-language data. We therefore designed an experimental task in which pairs of participants play a communicative game using a novel medium in which varying the position of one’s finger on a trackpad produced different colors. This task allowed us to manipulate the alignment of pressures acting on production and perception. Here we used it to investigate (a) whether above-chance levels of dispersion would emerge in the resulting systems, (b) whether dispersion would correlate with communicative success, and (c) how systems would differ if the pressures acting on perception were misaligned with pressures acting on production (and which would take precedence). We found that above-chance levels of dispersion emerged when pressures were aligned, but that the primary driver of communicative success was the alignment of production and perception pressures rather than dispersion itself. When they were misaligned, participants both found the task harder and (driven by perceptual demands) created systems with lower levels of dispersion.
语言在多个层面上表现出结构,包括音韵学层面,音韵学是一个由无意义的组合单元组成的系统,单词就是从这个系统中构建出来的。语音系统通常表现出比偶然预期的更大的分散性。已经提出了几个理论模型来解释这一点,一个共同的主题是,这种组织是作用于生产和感知的竞争力量的结果。从根本上说,这意味着一种文化进化的解释,即新兴组织是对交际互动压力的适应性反应。这个过程很难用自然语言数据进行实证研究。因此,我们设计了一项实验任务,两对参与者使用一种新颖的媒介进行交流游戏,在这种媒介中,改变手指在触控板上的位置会产生不同的颜色。这项任务使我们能够操纵作用于生产和感知的压力的一致性。在这里,我们用它来研究(a)在产生的系统中是否会出现高于偶然水平的分散,(b)分散是否与沟通成功相关,以及(c)如果作用在感知上的压力与作用在生产上的压力不一致,系统会有什么不同(哪个优先)。我们发现,当压力一致时,会出现高于机会的分散水平,但沟通成功的主要驱动力是生产和感知压力的一致性,而不是分散本身。当他们错位时,参与者都发现任务更难完成,并且(在感知需求的驱动下)创建了分散度较低的系统。
{"title":"Dispersion, communication, and alignment: an experimental study of the emergence of structure in combinatorial phonology","authors":"Gareth Roberts, R. Clark","doi":"10.1093/jole/lzaa004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/jole/lzaa004","url":null,"abstract":"Languages exhibit structure at a number of levels, including at the level of phonology, the system of meaningless combinatorial units from which words are constructed. Phonological systems typically exhibit greater dispersion than would be expected by chance. Several theoretical models have been proposed to account for this, and a common theme is that such organization emerges as a result of the competing forces acting on production and perception. Fundamentally, this implies a cultural evolutionary explanation, by which emergent organization is an adaptive response to the pressures of communicative interaction. This process is hard to investigate empirically using natural-language data. We therefore designed an experimental task in which pairs of participants play a communicative game using a novel medium in which varying the position of one’s finger on a trackpad produced different colors. This task allowed us to manipulate the alignment of pressures acting on production and perception. Here we used it to investigate (a) whether above-chance levels of dispersion would emerge in the resulting systems, (b) whether dispersion would correlate with communicative success, and (c) how systems would differ if the pressures acting on perception were misaligned with pressures acting on production (and which would take precedence). We found that above-chance levels of dispersion emerged when pressures were aligned, but that the primary driver of communicative success was the alignment of production and perception pressures rather than dispersion itself. When they were misaligned, participants both found the task harder and (driven by perceptual demands) created systems with lower levels of dispersion.","PeriodicalId":37118,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Language Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2020-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/jole/lzaa004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47466007","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Journal of Language Evolution
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1