首页 > 最新文献

Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties最新文献

英文 中文
Australian adolescent boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD): teacher and teaching factors that assess the efficacy of reducing unwanted behaviours within the classroom environment 澳大利亚患有注意力缺陷/多动障碍(AD/HD)的青春期男孩:评估减少课堂环境中不良行为效果的教师和教学因素
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1393626
K. Gibbs
Abstract Students with AD/HD experience difficulty concentrating in the classroom due to unwanted behaviours. This article focuses on what six Australian adolescent boys have to say about teacher and teaching factors that enabled them to regain focus (if distracted) and concentrate on classroom learning. A multiple, instrumental case-study was used to collect data from the boys using semi-structured individual and focus group interviews as well as school reports across a two year period. Findings suggested that interventions including frequent short breaks, humour by the teacher and teachers who operate in a well-structured classroom and who know their subject matter well, were beneficial in reducing unwanted behaviours and resulted in less negative events, thereby making schooling a more positive experience.
摘要患有AD/HD的学生由于不良行为而难以在课堂上集中注意力。这篇文章的重点是六名澳大利亚青春期男孩对教师和教学因素的看法,这些因素使他们能够重新集中注意力(如果分心的话)并专注于课堂学习。使用多个工具性案例研究,通过半结构化的个人和焦点小组访谈以及两年的学校报告,从男孩身上收集数据。研究结果表明,干预措施,包括频繁的短暂休息、教师和在结构良好的课堂上操作、对自己的主题很了解的教师的幽默,有助于减少不必要的行为,减少负面事件,从而使上学成为一种更积极的体验。
{"title":"Australian adolescent boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD): teacher and teaching factors that assess the efficacy of reducing unwanted behaviours within the classroom environment","authors":"K. Gibbs","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1393626","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1393626","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Students with AD/HD experience difficulty concentrating in the classroom due to unwanted behaviours. This article focuses on what six Australian adolescent boys have to say about teacher and teaching factors that enabled them to regain focus (if distracted) and concentrate on classroom learning. A multiple, instrumental case-study was used to collect data from the boys using semi-structured individual and focus group interviews as well as school reports across a two year period. Findings suggested that interventions including frequent short breaks, humour by the teacher and teachers who operate in a well-structured classroom and who know their subject matter well, were beneficial in reducing unwanted behaviours and resulted in less negative events, thereby making schooling a more positive experience.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"23 1","pages":"53 - 65"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1393626","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44771237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
A model of assessment and intervention for Non-Verbal Learning Disability (NVLD) in the Australian education system: an educational and developmental psychologist perspective 澳大利亚教育系统中非语言学习障碍(NVLD)的评估和干预模型:教育和发展心理学家的视角
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2018.1467936
Celia Brenchley, S. Costello
Abstract Non-Verbal Learning Disabilities (NVLD) have a relatively rare incidence, estimated to be approximately 1.7% of all learning disabilities. Symptoms of the disorder are perceptual, social and emotional. These symptoms differ according to the developmental age, with 85% of cases being diagnosed in secondary school when education becomes more complex. In Australia the intricate arrangements between funding for intervention within the school and the requirements from the assessment authority in each state for special provision mean that a cohesive model is required for school professionals to guide education for NVLD students. This is particularly important to enable access to tertiary education. A flow-chart model of assessment and intervention for the Australian education system is demonstrated, which draws on two case studies (“Katie” currently attending university and “Jamie” currently in year 8) with the provision of Australian and International research and literature to validate the model.
摘要非语言学习障碍(NVLD)的发生率相对较低,估计约占所有学习障碍的1.7%。这种疾病的症状包括感知、社交和情绪。这些症状因发育年龄而异,85%的病例是在教育变得更加复杂的中学时诊断出来的。在澳大利亚,学校内部干预的资金和各州评估机构对特殊规定的要求之间的复杂安排意味着学校专业人员需要一个连贯的模式来指导NVLD学生的教育。这对于提供接受高等教育的机会尤为重要。展示了澳大利亚教育系统的评估和干预流程图模型,该模型借鉴了两个案例研究(“Katie”目前正在上大学,“Jamie”目前在读8年级),并提供了澳大利亚和国际研究和文献来验证该模型。
{"title":"A model of assessment and intervention for Non-Verbal Learning Disability (NVLD) in the Australian education system: an educational and developmental psychologist perspective","authors":"Celia Brenchley, S. Costello","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2018.1467936","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2018.1467936","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Non-Verbal Learning Disabilities (NVLD) have a relatively rare incidence, estimated to be approximately 1.7% of all learning disabilities. Symptoms of the disorder are perceptual, social and emotional. These symptoms differ according to the developmental age, with 85% of cases being diagnosed in secondary school when education becomes more complex. In Australia the intricate arrangements between funding for intervention within the school and the requirements from the assessment authority in each state for special provision mean that a cohesive model is required for school professionals to guide education for NVLD students. This is particularly important to enable access to tertiary education. A flow-chart model of assessment and intervention for the Australian education system is demonstrated, which draws on two case studies (“Katie” currently attending university and “Jamie” currently in year 8) with the provision of Australian and International research and literature to validate the model.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"23 1","pages":"67 - 86"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2018.1467936","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41690414","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Shared book reading behaviours of children with Down Syndrome before and after participation in the MultiLit reading tutor program: an exploratory study 唐氏综合症儿童在参加MultiLit阅读辅导计划前后的共同阅读行为:一项探索性研究
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1367948
Lisa Lim, J. Arciuli, Natalie Munro
Abstract This exploratory study compares the shared book reading behaviours of five school aged children with DS (aged 11 years 6 months to 15 years 6 months) before and after participation in an intervention which included selected components of the MultiLit Reading Tutor Program. The program was delivered 1:1 to participants each week over a 12 week period. Analysis of the average performance across the group revealed that the proportion of reading errors relative to the number of words read from preintervention to postintervention were significantly reduced. Significant improvement was also seen in shared book reading fluency following intervention. Individual case study data is also presented. Postintervention, reading errors per minute were reduced for two participants (P4 and P5). Reading dysfluencies per minute decreased for two participants (P1 and P5) while all participants improved in shared book reading fluency. Preliminary results suggest that children with DS can generalise skills taught in the MultiLit Reading Tutor Program to shared book reading, although variability regarding changes in literacy abilities postintervention was observed.
摘要本探索性研究比较了5名学龄DS儿童(11岁6个月至15岁6个月)在参与包括MultiLit reading Tutor Program部分内容的干预前后的共同书籍阅读行为。在12周的时间里,该项目每周以1:1的方式向参与者提供。对整个小组平均表现的分析显示,从干预前到干预后,与阅读单词数量相关的阅读错误比例显著降低。干预后,共享阅读流畅性也有显著改善。个别案例研究数据也提出。干预后,两名参与者(P4和P5)的每分钟阅读误差减少。两名参与者(P1和P5)的每分钟阅读不流畅度有所下降,而所有参与者的共享书籍阅读流畅度都有所提高。初步结果表明,尽管观察到干预后读写能力的变化,但患有DS的儿童可以将在MultiLit阅读导师计划中教授的技能推广到共享书籍阅读。
{"title":"Shared book reading behaviours of children with Down Syndrome before and after participation in the MultiLit reading tutor program: an exploratory study","authors":"Lisa Lim, J. Arciuli, Natalie Munro","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1367948","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1367948","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This exploratory study compares the shared book reading behaviours of five school aged children with DS (aged 11 years 6 months to 15 years 6 months) before and after participation in an intervention which included selected components of the MultiLit Reading Tutor Program. The program was delivered 1:1 to participants each week over a 12 week period. Analysis of the average performance across the group revealed that the proportion of reading errors relative to the number of words read from preintervention to postintervention were significantly reduced. Significant improvement was also seen in shared book reading fluency following intervention. Individual case study data is also presented. Postintervention, reading errors per minute were reduced for two participants (P4 and P5). Reading dysfluencies per minute decreased for two participants (P1 and P5) while all participants improved in shared book reading fluency. Preliminary results suggest that children with DS can generalise skills taught in the MultiLit Reading Tutor Program to shared book reading, although variability regarding changes in literacy abilities postintervention was observed.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"23 1","pages":"31 - 51"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1367948","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43877213","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Teachers’ use of phonics, knowledge of language constructs, and preferred word identification prompts in relation to beginning readers* 教师对语音的使用、语言结构的知识以及与初读者相关的首选单词识别提示*
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2018-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2018.1467937
J. Chapman, Keith T. Greaney, Alison Arrow, W. Tunmer
Abstract New Zealand’s approach to literacy instruction is predominantly whole language. Explicit code-orientated literacy instruction is not favoured, however, most teachers are believed to include phonics in their literacy lessons. No study has been reported on phonics use in New Zealand schools. Survey responses on the use of phonics instruction from 666 primary school teachers were analysed. We also assessed knowledge of the basic language constructs related to early reading success with 55 teachers participating in a professional development program on literacy teaching. A word identification prompt task based on six common word error scenarios experienced by beginning readers was also administered. Results of the phonics survey revealed that 90% of teachers indicated they used phonics in their literacy instruction. Knowledge of basic language constructs was variable: phonological and phonemic knowledge were generally good, but understanding of phonic and morphological constructs was relatively weak. Only 40% of initial word identification prompts were focussed on word-level information; the remainder (60%) were based on context or were non-specific. The implications of these findings for beginning readers are discussed.
摘要新西兰的识字教学方法主要是全语言教学。明确的以代码为导向的识字教学并不受欢迎,然而,大多数教师被认为在识字课中包括语音。没有关于新西兰学校使用语音的研究报告。分析了666名小学教师对语音教学使用情况的调查结果。我们还评估了与早期阅读成功相关的基本语言结构知识,55名教师参与了识字教学专业发展计划。基于初读者经历的六种常见单词错误场景,还进行了单词识别提示任务。语音调查结果显示,90%的教师表示他们在识字教学中使用了语音。基本语言结构的知识是可变的:语音和音位知识总体上很好,但对语音和形态结构的理解相对较弱。只有40%的初始单词识别提示集中在单词级别的信息上;其余的(60%)是基于上下文或是非特异性的。讨论了这些发现对初读者的启示。
{"title":"Teachers’ use of phonics, knowledge of language constructs, and preferred word identification prompts in relation to beginning readers*","authors":"J. Chapman, Keith T. Greaney, Alison Arrow, W. Tunmer","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2018.1467937","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2018.1467937","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract New Zealand’s approach to literacy instruction is predominantly whole language. Explicit code-orientated literacy instruction is not favoured, however, most teachers are believed to include phonics in their literacy lessons. No study has been reported on phonics use in New Zealand schools. Survey responses on the use of phonics instruction from 666 primary school teachers were analysed. We also assessed knowledge of the basic language constructs related to early reading success with 55 teachers participating in a professional development program on literacy teaching. A word identification prompt task based on six common word error scenarios experienced by beginning readers was also administered. Results of the phonics survey revealed that 90% of teachers indicated they used phonics in their literacy instruction. Knowledge of basic language constructs was variable: phonological and phonemic knowledge were generally good, but understanding of phonic and morphological constructs was relatively weak. Only 40% of initial word identification prompts were focussed on word-level information; the remainder (60%) were based on context or were non-specific. The implications of these findings for beginning readers are discussed.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"23 1","pages":"104 - 87"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2018-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2018.1467937","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48299710","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Video self-modelling as an intervention for remediating dysgraphia in children with autism spectrum disorders 视频自我建模作为治疗自闭症谱系障碍儿童书写障碍的干预措施
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1397525
Geri Harris, S. G. Little, Angeleque Akin-Little
Abstract A severe deficit in handwriting is known as dysgraphia, a problem frequently associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Video self-modelling (VSM) has been proven effective for children with ASD in the strengthening of social skills, verbalizations, and daily living skills. Because VSM has demonstrated success in the acquisition of many types of skills, it may prove similarly effective for remediating dysgraphia in children with ASD. Utilizing a single-subject design with three 7–8 year old children diagnosed with ASD, this study examined VSM as a treatment for improving handwriting legibility and proficiency. All participants’ legibility data showed a large effect sizes and high Percentages of non-overlapping data from baseline to treatment and maintenance phases, indicating that VSM is an effective treatment for improving and maintaining handwriting legibility in children with ASD. Results are discussed in terms of applicability of VSM as an intervention with academic skills deficits in children with ASD.
摘要严重的书写缺陷被称为书写困难,这是一个经常与自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)有关的问题。视频自我建模(VSM)已被证明对ASD儿童在加强社交技能、语言表达和日常生活技能方面有效。由于VSM在获得多种技能方面取得了成功,因此它可能对治疗ASD儿童的书写障碍同样有效。本研究利用三名被诊断为ASD的7-8岁儿童的单受试者设计,检验了VSM作为改善笔迹易读性和熟练度的一种治疗方法。所有参与者的易读性数据显示,从基线到治疗和维持阶段,效果大小大,非重叠数据百分比高,表明VSM是改善和维持ASD儿童笔迹易读性的有效治疗方法。结果讨论了VSM作为ASD儿童学习技能缺陷干预措施的适用性。
{"title":"Video self-modelling as an intervention for remediating dysgraphia in children with autism spectrum disorders","authors":"Geri Harris, S. G. Little, Angeleque Akin-Little","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1397525","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1397525","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract A severe deficit in handwriting is known as dysgraphia, a problem frequently associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Video self-modelling (VSM) has been proven effective for children with ASD in the strengthening of social skills, verbalizations, and daily living skills. Because VSM has demonstrated success in the acquisition of many types of skills, it may prove similarly effective for remediating dysgraphia in children with ASD. Utilizing a single-subject design with three 7–8 year old children diagnosed with ASD, this study examined VSM as a treatment for improving handwriting legibility and proficiency. All participants’ legibility data showed a large effect sizes and high Percentages of non-overlapping data from baseline to treatment and maintenance phases, indicating that VSM is an effective treatment for improving and maintaining handwriting legibility in children with ASD. Results are discussed in terms of applicability of VSM as an intervention with academic skills deficits in children with ASD.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"153 - 170"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1397525","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49265837","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Does learning the alphabet in kindergarten give children a head start in the first year of school? A comparison of children’s reading progress in two first grade classes in state and Montessori schools in Switzerland 在幼儿园学习字母表是否能让孩子们在入学的第一年有一个良好的开端?瑞士国立学校和蒙特梭利学校两个一年级班级儿童阅读进展的比较
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1399913
Judy Elben, T. Nicholson
Abstract The main purpose of this study was to examine whether the age at which children start to learn to read affects their later progress. The study was conducted in Zürich, Switzerland, and compared a first grade class in a local school with two first grade classes in a Montessori school. It was found that although the Montessori children had an advantage over the local children in alphabet knowledge at entry to Grade 1, this was not translated into a significant advantage at the end of Grade 1 in either phonemic awareness or reading ability. Further analysis revealed that pretest alphabet knowledge for the whole group was significantly related to progress. In addition, scatterplots showed that some children started school with high levels of alphabet knowledge but did not make progress, indicating that alphabet knowledge is necessary for literacy progress but not sufficient.
摘要本研究的主要目的是探讨儿童开始学习阅读的年龄是否会影响他们以后的学习进度。这项研究是在瑞士z里奇进行的,并将当地一所学校的一个一年级班级与蒙特梭利学校的两个一年级班级进行了比较。研究发现,虽然蒙台梭利儿童在一年级入学时在字母知识方面比当地儿童有优势,但这并没有转化为一年级结束时在音位意识或阅读能力方面的显著优势。进一步的分析表明,整个小组的预试字母知识与进步显著相关。此外,散点图显示,一些孩子开始上学时具有较高的字母知识水平,但没有取得进展,这表明字母知识是识字进步的必要条件,但不是充分条件。
{"title":"Does learning the alphabet in kindergarten give children a head start in the first year of school? A comparison of children’s reading progress in two first grade classes in state and Montessori schools in Switzerland","authors":"Judy Elben, T. Nicholson","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1399913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1399913","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The main purpose of this study was to examine whether the age at which children start to learn to read affects their later progress. The study was conducted in Zürich, Switzerland, and compared a first grade class in a local school with two first grade classes in a Montessori school. It was found that although the Montessori children had an advantage over the local children in alphabet knowledge at entry to Grade 1, this was not translated into a significant advantage at the end of Grade 1 in either phonemic awareness or reading ability. Further analysis revealed that pretest alphabet knowledge for the whole group was significantly related to progress. In addition, scatterplots showed that some children started school with high levels of alphabet knowledge but did not make progress, indicating that alphabet knowledge is necessary for literacy progress but not sufficient.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"108 - 95"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1399913","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48848504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Links between early oral narrative and decoding skills and later reading in a New Zealand sample* 新西兰样本中早期口头叙述和解码技能与后期阅读之间的联系*
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1399914
E. Schaughency, S. Suggate, E. Reese
Abstract We examined earlier oral narrative and decoding and later reading in two samples spanning the first four years of reading instruction. The Year 1 sample (n = 44) was initially assessed after one year of instruction (M = 6; 1 years) and followed through their third year (M = 8; 1 years); the Year 2 sample (n = 34) assessed after two years of instruction (M = 7; 0 years) and followed to their fourth year (M = 9; 0 years). Oral narrative and decoding were assessed initially, oral reading, retell, and maze fluency, plus reading age, obtained at outcome. For the Year 2 sample, oral narrative and decoding contributed to oral reading and comprehension two years later. For the Year 1 sample, decoding contributed to most reading outcomes, with narrative quality uniquely predicting Year 3 retell fluency. Post-hoc exploratory analyses suggest story memory indirectly contributed to Year 3 reading via Year 2 retell.
摘要我们在阅读教学的前四年中,对两个样本中的早期口头叙述和解码以及后期阅读进行了研究。第一年的样本(n=44)在接受一年的教学(M=6;1年)后进行初步评估,并随访至第三年(M=8;1岁);2年级样本(n=34)在接受两年的教学(M=7;0年)后进行评估,并随访至第四年(M=9;0岁)。最初评估口头叙述和解码,在结果中获得口头阅读、复述和迷宫流畅度,以及阅读年龄。对于二年级的样本,口头叙述和解码有助于两年后的口头阅读和理解。对于第一年的样本,解码对大多数阅读结果有贡献,叙事质量是唯一预测第三年复述流利性的因素。事后探索性分析表明,故事记忆通过二年级复述间接促进了三年级的阅读。
{"title":"Links between early oral narrative and decoding skills and later reading in a New Zealand sample*","authors":"E. Schaughency, S. Suggate, E. Reese","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1399914","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1399914","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract We examined earlier oral narrative and decoding and later reading in two samples spanning the first four years of reading instruction. The Year 1 sample (n = 44) was initially assessed after one year of instruction (M = 6; 1 years) and followed through their third year (M = 8; 1 years); the Year 2 sample (n = 34) assessed after two years of instruction (M = 7; 0 years) and followed to their fourth year (M = 9; 0 years). Oral narrative and decoding were assessed initially, oral reading, retell, and maze fluency, plus reading age, obtained at outcome. For the Year 2 sample, oral narrative and decoding contributed to oral reading and comprehension two years later. For the Year 1 sample, decoding contributed to most reading outcomes, with narrative quality uniquely predicting Year 3 retell fluency. Post-hoc exploratory analyses suggest story memory indirectly contributed to Year 3 reading via Year 2 retell.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"109 - 132"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1399914","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47147988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 15
Does linguistic comprehension support the decoding skills of struggling readers? 语言理解是否支持困难读者的解码技能?
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1389760
Michele Anne Blick, T. Nicholson, J. Chapman, J. Berman
Abstract This study investigated the contribution of linguistic comprehension to the decoding skills of struggling readers. Participants were 36 children aged between eight and 12 years, all below average in decoding but differing in linguistic comprehension. The children read passages from the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability and their first 25 miscues were categorised into syntactic, semantic, phonemic, and graphophonic similarity. Children were first grouped in terms of higher and lower linguistic comprehension levels. Analysis of miscues showed no differences between the groups in miscue similarity. They were then grouped according to pseudoword reading skill. There were significant differences between the groups for all miscue types except semantic similarity. An analysis of miscues using multiple regression showed that, after taking account of age, pseudoword reading was the best predictor of quality of miscues. In addition, linguistic comprehension contributed to syntactic similarity of miscues over and above decoding.
摘要本研究调查了语言理解对困难读者解码技能的贡献。参与者是36名年龄在8-12岁之间的儿童,他们的解码能力都低于平均水平,但语言理解能力不同。孩子们阅读了《阅读能力精细分析》中的段落,他们的前25个错误被分为句法、语义、音位和笔迹相似性。儿童首先根据较高和较低的语言理解水平进行分组。对失误的分析表明,各组之间在失误相似性方面没有差异。然后根据假名阅读技巧对他们进行分组。除语义相似外,各组在所有混杂类型上均存在显著差异。一项使用多元回归的误传分析表明,在考虑年龄后,假名阅读是误传质量的最佳预测因素。除此之外,语言理解对歧义的句法相似性也有一定的促进作用。
{"title":"Does linguistic comprehension support the decoding skills of struggling readers?","authors":"Michele Anne Blick, T. Nicholson, J. Chapman, J. Berman","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1389760","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1389760","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This study investigated the contribution of linguistic comprehension to the decoding skills of struggling readers. Participants were 36 children aged between eight and 12 years, all below average in decoding but differing in linguistic comprehension. The children read passages from the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability and their first 25 miscues were categorised into syntactic, semantic, phonemic, and graphophonic similarity. Children were first grouped in terms of higher and lower linguistic comprehension levels. Analysis of miscues showed no differences between the groups in miscue similarity. They were then grouped according to pseudoword reading skill. There were significant differences between the groups for all miscue types except semantic similarity. An analysis of miscues using multiple regression showed that, after taking account of age, pseudoword reading was the best predictor of quality of miscues. In addition, linguistic comprehension contributed to syntactic similarity of miscues over and above decoding.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"75 - 94"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1389760","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43500369","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
To what extent does children’s spelling improve as a result of learning words with the look, say, cover, write, check, fix strategy compared with phonological spelling strategies? 与语音拼写策略相比,采用“看、说、盖、写、检查、固定”策略学习单词能在多大程度上提高儿童的拼写水平?
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1398766
S. Dymock, T. Nicholson
Abstract The ubiquitous weekly spelling test assumes that words are best learned by memorisation and testing but is this the best way? This study compared two well-known approaches to spelling instruction, the rule based and visual memory approaches. A group of 55 seven-year-olds in two Year 3 classrooms was taught spelling in small groups for three lessons a week, 20-min per lesson, over ten weeks. In the first intervention, students learned statistically likely spelling strategies for vowel sounds, syllable breaking strategies, and the doubling rule. In the second intervention, students used a look, say, cover, write, check, fix strategy, listed words in alphabetical order, and wrote them in sentences. The control group completed non-spelling activities. Results showed that although both intervention groups learned to spell taught words better than the control group, the rule-based approach had greater transfer to spelling of new words for both proficient and less proficient spellers.
摘要无处不在的每周拼写测试假设单词最好通过记忆和测试来学习,但这是最好的方法吗?本研究比较了两种众所周知的拼写教学方法,即基于规则的方法和视觉记忆方法。在两个三年级的教室里,55名七岁的孩子被分成小组,每周上三节课,每节课20分钟,为期十周。在第一次干预中,学生们学习了元音的统计可能拼写策略、断音节策略和加倍规则。在第二次干预中,学生们使用了看、说、盖、写、检查、修复策略,按字母顺序列出单词,并将其写成句子。对照组完成了非拼写活动。结果表明,尽管两个干预组都比对照组更好地学会了拼写所教单词,但对于熟练和不熟练的拼写者来说,基于规则的方法在拼写新词方面有更大的转移。
{"title":"To what extent does children’s spelling improve as a result of learning words with the look, say, cover, write, check, fix strategy compared with phonological spelling strategies?","authors":"S. Dymock, T. Nicholson","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1398766","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1398766","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The ubiquitous weekly spelling test assumes that words are best learned by memorisation and testing but is this the best way? This study compared two well-known approaches to spelling instruction, the rule based and visual memory approaches. A group of 55 seven-year-olds in two Year 3 classrooms was taught spelling in small groups for three lessons a week, 20-min per lesson, over ten weeks. In the first intervention, students learned statistically likely spelling strategies for vowel sounds, syllable breaking strategies, and the doubling rule. In the second intervention, students used a look, say, cover, write, check, fix strategy, listed words in alphabetical order, and wrote them in sentences. The control group completed non-spelling activities. Results showed that although both intervention groups learned to spell taught words better than the control group, the rule-based approach had greater transfer to spelling of new words for both proficient and less proficient spellers.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"171 - 187"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1398766","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43194561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Who benefits from which reading intervention in the primary years? Match the intervention with the reading profile 在小学阶段,哪种阅读干预对谁有利?将干预与阅读档案相匹配
IF 0.9 Q3 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2017-07-03 DOI: 10.1080/19404158.2017.1379027
J. Munro
Abstract Reading difficulties may have multiple causes. Effective approaches to reading intervention need to target the specific causes for individual readers. The Early Reading Intervention Knowledge program comprises three intervention pathways: a phonological-phonemic pathway, a phonic-orthographic pathway, and an oral language pathway. This study examines the effectiveness of each pathway for 902 underachieving students in their second to fifth years of primary education. The students differed in their reading profiles. The interventions were implemented in a regular school context. Intervention in the three pathways improved reading accuracy and comprehension for both the younger (Years 2 and 3) and older (Years 4 and 5) age groups. The pattern in accuracy gains across the three pathways was similar for each group, with the older students making smaller gains. Reading profiles influenced the rate or amount of gain. For comprehension, the phonological and oral language pathways yielded higher gains for both age groups than the orthographic pathway. The most at-risk students showed the highest gains. The implications for intervention are discussed.
摘要阅读困难可能有多种原因。有效的阅读干预方法需要针对个别读者的具体原因。早期阅读干预知识计划包括三条干预途径:语音音位途径、语音拼写途径和口语途径。这项研究考察了902名小学二至五年级成绩不佳的学生的每种途径的有效性。学生们的阅读情况各不相同。干预措施是在普通学校实施的。对这三种途径的干预提高了年轻(2岁和3岁)和老年(4岁和5岁)年龄组的阅读准确性和理解力。各组在这三种途径中的准确性提高模式相似,年龄较大的学生的提高幅度较小。阅读档案会影响增益的速率或数量。在理解方面,语音和口语途径对两个年龄组的收益都高于拼写途径。风险最大的学生表现出最高的收益。讨论了干预的影响。
{"title":"Who benefits from which reading intervention in the primary years? Match the intervention with the reading profile","authors":"J. Munro","doi":"10.1080/19404158.2017.1379027","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/19404158.2017.1379027","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Reading difficulties may have multiple causes. Effective approaches to reading intervention need to target the specific causes for individual readers. The Early Reading Intervention Knowledge program comprises three intervention pathways: a phonological-phonemic pathway, a phonic-orthographic pathway, and an oral language pathway. This study examines the effectiveness of each pathway for 902 underachieving students in their second to fifth years of primary education. The students differed in their reading profiles. The interventions were implemented in a regular school context. Intervention in the three pathways improved reading accuracy and comprehension for both the younger (Years 2 and 3) and older (Years 4 and 5) age groups. The pattern in accuracy gains across the three pathways was similar for each group, with the older students making smaller gains. Reading profiles influenced the rate or amount of gain. For comprehension, the phonological and oral language pathways yielded higher gains for both age groups than the orthographic pathway. The most at-risk students showed the highest gains. The implications for intervention are discussed.","PeriodicalId":44419,"journal":{"name":"Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties","volume":"22 1","pages":"133 - 151"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2017-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/19404158.2017.1379027","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45938919","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
期刊
Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1