Pub Date : 2023-10-31DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.5.291
Vanda Kuběnová, Aleš Kuběna, Martin Dolejš, Zuzana Bílková, Hedwig van Bakel, Zdeňka Bajgarová
{"title":"Rodičovské vyhoření – základní psychometrické charakteristiky české verze dotazníku Parental Burnout Assessment (PBA) a souvislost rodičovského vyhoření s demografickými ukazateli","authors":"Vanda Kuběnová, Aleš Kuběna, Martin Dolejš, Zuzana Bílková, Hedwig van Bakel, Zdeňka Bajgarová","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.5.291","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.5.291","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"68 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135871673","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-31DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.5.308
Michal Hrdlička, Tomáš Urbánek, Adam Mrkvička, Vladimír Komárek, Lenka Pospíšilová, Markéta Mohaplová, Marek Blatný, Iva Dudová
Objectives. The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and the Empathizing/Systemizing Quotient (EQ/SQ) scale are both used for the assessment of individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study aimed to investigate the use of both scales to assess another neurodevelopmental disorder, namely developmental dysphasia (DD). Additionally, we examined differences in social, empathetic, and systemizing characteristics between ASD and DD groups. Sample and settings. The authors examined 103 children with (1) ASD (n = 30, mean age 8.4 ± 2.6 years), (2) DD (n = 35, mean age 8.9 ± 2.3 years), and (3) healthy control children (HC; n = 38, mean age 9.2 ± 1.6 years) using the SRS and EQ/SQ assessments. Subjects with additional psychiatric diagnoses, e.g., intellectual disabilities and/or genetic syndromes, were excluded from the study. Statistical analysis. Descriptive statistics, one-way ANOVA, chi-square test, and Kruskal-Wallis test with the Bonferroni correction were used. Hypotheses. The null hypothesis was that there are no significant differences between the ASD, DD and HC subgroups on the Social Responsiveness Scale and on the Empathizing/Systemizing Quotients. Results. There were statistically significant differences on the SRS between the ASD, DD, and HC groups (97.0 vs. 52.0 vs. 29.0, p<0.001). Differences in EQ scores were also significant between the groups in total (17.0 vs. 30.0 vs. 35.0, p<0.001); however, post-hoc tests did not confirm a significant difference between the DD and HC groups. There were no statistically significant differences on the SQ score between the groups. Limitations. The gender imbalance of our subgroups and the relatively small sample size of our study were the main limitations of our study.
目标。社会反应量表(SRS)和共情/系统化商数量表(EQ/SQ)都被用于自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)个体的评估。本研究旨在探讨使用这两种量表来评估另一种神经发育障碍,即发育性言语障碍(DD)。此外,我们还研究了ASD和DD群体在社交、同理心和系统化特征方面的差异。示例和设置。作者对103例儿童进行了检查:(1)ASD (n = 30,平均年龄8.4±2.6岁),(2)DD (n = 35,平均年龄8.9±2.3岁),(3)健康对照儿童(HC;n = 38,平均年龄9.2±1.6岁),采用SRS和EQ/SQ评估。有其他精神诊断的受试者,如智力残疾和/或遗传综合征,被排除在研究之外。统计分析。采用描述性统计、单因素方差分析、卡方检验和经Bonferroni校正的Kruskal-Wallis检验。假设。原假设是ASD、DD和HC亚组在社会反应量表和共情/系统化商数上没有显著差异。结果。ASD、DD和HC组的SRS差异有统计学意义(97.0 vs. 52.0 vs. 29.0, p<0.001)。总体而言,各组之间的情商得分差异也很显著(17.0 vs. 30.0 vs. 35.0, p<0.001);然而,事后测试没有证实DD组和HC组之间有显著差异。两组之间的心理素质得分没有统计学上的显著差异。的局限性。我们的亚组的性别不平衡和我们研究的样本量相对较小是我们研究的主要局限性。
{"title":"Autistic and dysphasic children differ in social responsiveness and empathy but not in systemizing behavior","authors":"Michal Hrdlička, Tomáš Urbánek, Adam Mrkvička, Vladimír Komárek, Lenka Pospíšilová, Markéta Mohaplová, Marek Blatný, Iva Dudová","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.5.308","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.5.308","url":null,"abstract":"Objectives. The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and the Empathizing/Systemizing Quotient (EQ/SQ) scale are both used for the assessment of individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study aimed to investigate the use of both scales to assess another neurodevelopmental disorder, namely developmental dysphasia (DD). Additionally, we examined differences in social, empathetic, and systemizing characteristics between ASD and DD groups. Sample and settings. The authors examined 103 children with (1) ASD (n = 30, mean age 8.4 ± 2.6 years), (2) DD (n = 35, mean age 8.9 ± 2.3 years), and (3) healthy control children (HC; n = 38, mean age 9.2 ± 1.6 years) using the SRS and EQ/SQ assessments. Subjects with additional psychiatric diagnoses, e.g., intellectual disabilities and/or genetic syndromes, were excluded from the study. Statistical analysis. Descriptive statistics, one-way ANOVA, chi-square test, and Kruskal-Wallis test with the Bonferroni correction were used. Hypotheses. The null hypothesis was that there are no significant differences between the ASD, DD and HC subgroups on the Social Responsiveness Scale and on the Empathizing/Systemizing Quotients. Results. There were statistically significant differences on the SRS between the ASD, DD, and HC groups (97.0 vs. 52.0 vs. 29.0, p<0.001). Differences in EQ scores were also significant between the groups in total (17.0 vs. 30.0 vs. 35.0, p<0.001); however, post-hoc tests did not confirm a significant difference between the DD and HC groups. There were no statistically significant differences on the SQ score between the groups. Limitations. The gender imbalance of our subgroups and the relatively small sample size of our study were the main limitations of our study.","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"69 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135809303","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-31DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.5.315
Ivan Lukšík, Barbora Petrů Puhrová, Jakub Šrol
Individual representations of young people´s love in Slovakia and the Czech Republic Objectives. The investigation of love has produced several psychological concepts and theories. In addition to general signs of love, several cross-cultural differences were also identified. The aim of the study was to verify the previously created a 5-factor model of love, which was created on the basis of individual representations of love of young people in Slovakia, and to find out whether this model is also valid in a cross-cultural Czech-Slovak comparison.Sample and procedure. The research was conducted on a sample of 397 respondents from the Slovak Republic (M=23.1, SD=2.54, men=115; women=282) and 441 respondents from the Czech Republic (M=21, 5, SD=2.15; men=107; women=334) who were aged 18–29. The research was focused on this age cohort due to the fact that for this period of the so-called emerging adulthood is important to explore in the area of relationships and love.Methods. Quantitative methodology was used in the research, the research was carried out with the help of an electronically administered questionnaire. In the questionnaire, a previously verified scale of Individual representations of love was used. In the questionnaire were included questions concerning selected characteristics of partner relationships and demographic data.Analyses. The data were processed by confirmatory factor analysis separately for the Czech and Slovak samples, further by correlation analyses and linear regression analysis, which showed relationships with other variables.Results. The results showed in both countries 5 factors of individual representations of love: 1. physical love, commitment, building and searching, 2. spiritual love, 3. power and positive benefits of love, 4. the opposite side of love and 5. biological and self-centred love. Age and strength of religious faith were shown to be predictors of individual factors.Limits. Since the research was not conducted on a representative sample and it mostly consisted of female university students, the possibilities of generalization are limited. This quantitative research also worked with a selected limited number of meanings of love. Ciele. Doterajšie skúmanie lásky prinieslo viacero psychologických koncepcií a teórií. Okrem všeobecných znakov lásky boli identifikované aj viaceré kroskulturálne rozdiely. Cieľom štúdie bolo overiť 5-faktorový model lásky, ktorý bol vytvorený na základe individuálnych reprezentácií lásky mladých ľudí na Slovensku a zistiť, či tento model platí aj v kroskulturálnom porovnaní Českej a Slovenskej republiky.Výskumný výber. Výskum bol realizovaný na vzorke 397 respondentov zo Slovenskej republiky (muži = 115; ženy = 282, M = 23,1; SD = 2,54) a 441 respondentov z Českej republiky (muži = 107; ženy = 334; M = 21,5, SD = 2,15). Výskum bol zameraný na vekovú kohortu 18–29 rokov, pretože pre toto obdobie tzv. vynárajúcej sa dospelosti je dôležité explorovanie v oblasti vzťahov a lásky.Metó
斯洛伐克和捷克共和国年轻人爱情的个体表现对爱情的研究产生了几个心理学概念和理论。除了一般的爱的迹象外,还发现了一些跨文化差异。本研究的目的是验证先前建立的基于斯洛伐克年轻人爱情的个体表征的五因素爱情模型,并找出该模型在跨文化的捷克斯洛伐克比较中是否也有效。样品和程序。该研究对来自斯洛伐克共和国的397名受访者进行了抽样调查(M=23.1, SD=2.54,男性=115;女性=282)和来自捷克共和国的441名受访者(M= 21,5, SD=2.15;人= 107;女性=334),年龄在18-29岁之间。这项研究主要集中在这个年龄段,因为在这个所谓的新兴成年期,在人际关系和爱情领域的探索是很重要的。研究中采用了定量方法,研究是在电子管理问卷的帮助下进行的。在问卷中,使用了先前验证过的爱的个体表征量表。在调查问卷中包括关于伴侣关系的选择特征和人口统计数据的问题。对捷克和斯洛伐克样本的数据分别进行验证性因子分析,并进一步进行相关分析和线性回归分析,显示了与其他变量的关系。结果显示,在两国,爱情的个体表征有5个因素:1。身体的爱,承诺,建立和寻找,2。精神上的爱;爱的力量和积极的好处,4。5.爱情的反面和反面。生理和自我中心的爱。年龄和宗教信仰的强度被证明是个体因素的预测因子。由于本研究的样本不具有代表性,且主要以女大学生为研究对象,因此泛化的可能性有限。这项定量研究也适用于选定的有限数量的爱的含义。Ciele。Doterajšie skúmanie lásky prinieslo viacero psychologických koncepcií a teórií。Okrem všeobecných znakov lásky boli identifikovan aj viacer kroskulturálne rozdiely。Cieľom štúdie bolo overikv 5-faktorový模型lásky, ktorý bol vytvorený na základe individuálnych reprezentácií lásky mladých ľudí na Slovensku a zistikv, i tento模型platí aj v kroskulturálnom porovnaní Českej a Slovenskej共和国。Vyskumny vyber。Výskum bol realizovaný na vzorke 397名受访者(muži = 115;ženy = 282, M = 23,1;SD = 2,54) a 441受访者z Českej共和国(muži = 107;ženy = 334;M = 21,5, sd = 2,15)。Výskum bol zameraný na vekovú kohortu 18-29 rokov, pretože pre toto obdobie tzv。vynárajúcej sa dospelosti je dôležité explorovanie v oblasti vzťahov a lásky.Metóda。Vo výskume bola použitá kvantitatívna metodológia, výskum sa realize prostredníctvom electronic administrative vanachiho dotazníka。V dotazníku bola použitá predtým overen škála Individuálnych reprezentácií lásky。Dotazník sa tieje pýtal na vybraniccharakterisky partnerských vzťahov a na demografick údaje.Analýzy。数据勃利spracovane konfirmač努美阿faktorovou analyzou osobitne preč艾斯库一个slovensku vzorku,ďalej korelačnymi analyzami一linearnou regresnou analyzou, ktore ukazali vzťahy年代ďalšimi premennymi.Vysledky。Výsledky ukázali v oboch krajinách 5 faktorov individuálnych reprezentácií lásky: 1;fyzick láska, záväzok, budovanie a hľadanie, 2。Spirituálna láska, 3;Sila pozitívne prínosy lásky, 4岁。Odvrátená strana lásky a 5。biologicka sebastredn láska。Prediktormi jednotlivých faktorov sa ukázali by.com vek a sila náboženskej vierry . limit。Keďže výskum nebol realizovaný na reprezentatívnej vzorke a súbor zväčša tvorili vysokoškolské študentky, možnosti zovšeobecnenia sú obmedzen。Tento kvantitatívny výskum tiejepracoval len vybraným obmedzeným po tom významov lásky。
{"title":"Individuálne reprezentácie lásky mladých ľudí na Slovensku a v Českej republike","authors":"Ivan Lukšík, Barbora Petrů Puhrová, Jakub Šrol","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.5.315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.5.315","url":null,"abstract":"Individual representations of young people´s love in Slovakia and the Czech Republic Objectives. The investigation of love has produced several psychological concepts and theories. In addition to general signs of love, several cross-cultural differences were also identified. The aim of the study was to verify the previously created a 5-factor model of love, which was created on the basis of individual representations of love of young people in Slovakia, and to find out whether this model is also valid in a cross-cultural Czech-Slovak comparison.Sample and procedure. The research was conducted on a sample of 397 respondents from the Slovak Republic (M=23.1, SD=2.54, men=115; women=282) and 441 respondents from the Czech Republic (M=21, 5, SD=2.15; men=107; women=334) who were aged 18–29. The research was focused on this age cohort due to the fact that for this period of the so-called emerging adulthood is important to explore in the area of relationships and love.Methods. Quantitative methodology was used in the research, the research was carried out with the help of an electronically administered questionnaire. In the questionnaire, a previously verified scale of Individual representations of love was used. In the questionnaire were included questions concerning selected characteristics of partner relationships and demographic data.Analyses. The data were processed by confirmatory factor analysis separately for the Czech and Slovak samples, further by correlation analyses and linear regression analysis, which showed relationships with other variables.Results. The results showed in both countries 5 factors of individual representations of love: 1. physical love, commitment, building and searching, 2. spiritual love, 3. power and positive benefits of love, 4. the opposite side of love and 5. biological and self-centred love. Age and strength of religious faith were shown to be predictors of individual factors.Limits. Since the research was not conducted on a representative sample and it mostly consisted of female university students, the possibilities of generalization are limited. This quantitative research also worked with a selected limited number of meanings of love. Ciele. Doterajšie skúmanie lásky prinieslo viacero psychologických koncepcií a teórií. Okrem všeobecných znakov lásky boli identifikované aj viaceré kroskulturálne rozdiely. Cieľom štúdie bolo overiť 5-faktorový model lásky, ktorý bol vytvorený na základe individuálnych reprezentácií lásky mladých ľudí na Slovensku a zistiť, či tento model platí aj v kroskulturálnom porovnaní Českej a Slovenskej republiky.Výskumný výber. Výskum bol realizovaný na vzorke 397 respondentov zo Slovenskej republiky (muži = 115; ženy = 282, M = 23,1; SD = 2,54) a 441 respondentov z Českej republiky (muži = 107; ženy = 334; M = 21,5, SD = 2,15). Výskum bol zameraný na vekovú kohortu 18–29 rokov, pretože pre toto obdobie tzv. vynárajúcej sa dospelosti je dôležité explorovanie v oblasti vzťahov a lásky.Metó","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"21 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135809293","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-31DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.5.244
Mersiha Jusic
Feelings of being deprived in comparison to similar others have occupied authors from multiple disciplines, primarily as a potential predictor of various individual and societal outcomes, especially among youth. The present study,however, aims to explore whether this subjective relative deprivation, both individual and collective, can itself be predicted from particular personality variables, to shed more light on individual dispositions that lead to (un)favorable perceptions of contextual circumstances. Therefore, within the frame of Relative Deprivation Theory, this correlational study, including 735 participants recruited through the public educational system in Bosnia and Herzegovina (mean age 18.72; SD=1.54), indicated that lower levels of self-esteem and openness to experience, and higher levels of neuroticism and sensation-seeking are associated with higher relative deprivation. Canonical regression analysis revealed a statistically significant relationship between personality and relative deprivation, deriving a significant canonical root (λ of .799, F(8,1384)=20.583, p=.000), accounting for 21% (rc=.444) of the variance shared between the variable sets. Personality traits’ contribution to the collective relative deprivation was lower than to the individual. The article analyzes the role that selected personality traits might play in the perception of one’s deprivation. Recommendations are given regarding future research to include a more diverse set of personality traits as predictors.
{"title":"You’re better off than me: Personality predictors of relative deprivation in youth","authors":"Mersiha Jusic","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.5.244","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.5.244","url":null,"abstract":"Feelings of being deprived in comparison to similar others have occupied authors from multiple disciplines, primarily as a potential predictor of various individual and societal outcomes, especially among youth. The present study,however, aims to explore whether this subjective relative deprivation, both individual and collective, can itself be predicted from particular personality variables, to shed more light on individual dispositions that lead to (un)favorable perceptions of contextual circumstances. Therefore, within the frame of Relative Deprivation Theory, this correlational study, including 735 participants recruited through the public educational system in Bosnia and Herzegovina (mean age 18.72; SD=1.54), indicated that lower levels of self-esteem and openness to experience, and higher levels of neuroticism and sensation-seeking are associated with higher relative deprivation. Canonical regression analysis revealed a statistically significant relationship between personality and relative deprivation, deriving a significant canonical root (λ of .799, F(8,1384)=20.583, p=.000), accounting for 21% (rc=.444) of the variance shared between the variable sets. Personality traits’ contribution to the collective relative deprivation was lower than to the individual. The article analyzes the role that selected personality traits might play in the perception of one’s deprivation. Recommendations are given regarding future research to include a more diverse set of personality traits as predictors.","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"66 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135809296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-31DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.5.260
Brigitta Szabó, Judit Futo, Mónika Miklósi
Objectives. Maternal attachment style plays a major role in the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology. Previous studies indicated that a secure attachment style is associated with higher levels of mindfulness and higher quality of parent-child relationship. However, most of these studies assessed general mindfulness instead of mindful parenting. This study aimed to explore the relationship between the mothersʼ attachment style, mindful parenting, and perception of their child.Sample and settings. One hundred forty-four mothers who had a child below the age of three years completed self-report questionnaires, including a demographic questionnaire, the Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ), the Interpersonal Mindfulness in Parenting Scale (IMP), and the Mothersʼ Object Relations Scale (MORS-SF).Hypotheses. The authors hypothesized that mothers with insecure attachment styles and a lower level of mindful parenting would have more invasive and less warm representations of their children than mothers with a secure attachment style and a higher level of mindful parenting.Statistical analyses. The authors conducted two mediation analyses with Mothersʼ Object Relations Scale warmth and invasion subscales as dependent variables, the motherʼs attachment style as an independent variable and mindfulparenting as a mediator.Results. In the mediation analysis, the direct effects of the mothersʼ attachment style on the perception of the child were not significant. However, the indirect effects through mindful parenting were significant; a higher level of mindful parenting was associated with higher levels of MORS-SF warmth and lower levels of MORS-SF invasion. These findings suggest that attachment styles are related to the perception of the child through mindful parenting. Mindfulness-based parental training programs might be helpful in improving the parent-child relationship in case of attachment-related problems.Limitations. Cross-sectional design and the use of self-report measures are the main limitations of the current study.
{"title":"The relationship between mothers’ attachment style, mindful parenting, and the perception of their child","authors":"Brigitta Szabó, Judit Futo, Mónika Miklósi","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.5.260","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.5.260","url":null,"abstract":"Objectives. Maternal attachment style plays a major role in the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology. Previous studies indicated that a secure attachment style is associated with higher levels of mindfulness and higher quality of parent-child relationship. However, most of these studies assessed general mindfulness instead of mindful parenting. This study aimed to explore the relationship between the mothersʼ attachment style, mindful parenting, and perception of their child.Sample and settings. One hundred forty-four mothers who had a child below the age of three years completed self-report questionnaires, including a demographic questionnaire, the Attachment Style Questionnaire (ASQ), the Interpersonal Mindfulness in Parenting Scale (IMP), and the Mothersʼ Object Relations Scale (MORS-SF).Hypotheses. The authors hypothesized that mothers with insecure attachment styles and a lower level of mindful parenting would have more invasive and less warm representations of their children than mothers with a secure attachment style and a higher level of mindful parenting.Statistical analyses. The authors conducted two mediation analyses with Mothersʼ Object Relations Scale warmth and invasion subscales as dependent variables, the motherʼs attachment style as an independent variable and mindfulparenting as a mediator.Results. In the mediation analysis, the direct effects of the mothersʼ attachment style on the perception of the child were not significant. However, the indirect effects through mindful parenting were significant; a higher level of mindful parenting was associated with higher levels of MORS-SF warmth and lower levels of MORS-SF invasion. These findings suggest that attachment styles are related to the perception of the child through mindful parenting. Mindfulness-based parental training programs might be helpful in improving the parent-child relationship in case of attachment-related problems.Limitations. Cross-sectional design and the use of self-report measures are the main limitations of the current study.","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"16 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135809305","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-08-29DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.242
Karel Paulík
{"title":"M. H. Erickson, E. L. Rossi: Hypnotické světy. Klinická hypnóza a nepřímé sugesce.","authors":"Karel Paulík","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.242","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.242","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"11 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136282844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-08-29DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.166
Natálie Kikoťová, Natalia Nudga, Jana Šimsová, Zita Maršíková, Kateřina Chládková
Language-based social preferences: The effect of dialect on friendship choices Objectives: This study investigated whether preschool and early school age children's friendship choices are modulated by a talker's regional dialect, and whether the potential preference for local/native dialect would be attenuated by a child's more diverse language experience. Methods: An online forced-choice experiment was administered. Each of 12 trials contained a pair of sentences recorded by two different same-sex speakers: one was a speaking the Central Bohemian variety of Czech and the other speaking a phonetically distinct Nothern Moravian-Silesian dialect. Data were collected from a total 81 participants, aged 5 to 8 years from both regions, who, upon listening to both sentences in each trial, indicated which of the two children they would like to play with. Results: A generalized linear mixed-effects model revealed a statistically significant interaction of age and participant location (average estimated effect size = 16.4 %, SE = 8.1 %, z = 2.031, p = .042). Pairwise comparisons showed that younger Moravian-Silesian children had a stronger preference for their own local dialect than younger Central Bohemian children, and marginally also than the older Moravian-Silesian children. Conclusions: The results can be explained by considering that older Moravian-Silesian children have a more diverse language experience (associated with starting school), similarly to Central Bohemian children of any age who have a more diverse language experience due to a greater linguistic variation in their region. Such richer, more varied, language experience might then attenuate children's preferences for their own dialect. Limitations: The recruitment procedures for both regions were kept identical in order to recruit comparably motivated participants' parents, which is crucial to collect comparably reliable data using an online experiment administered by the children's parents. This however resulted in a relatively small sample of the younger group of Moravian-Silesian children. Follow-up research, ideally lab-based, is needed to test the replicability of the present findings and to collect more detailed information on children's language, dialectal, and education background. Cíl. Experiment zkoumal, zda jsou sociální preference dětí předškolního a mladšího školního věku ovlivněny regionálním dialektem mluvčího a zda mohou být potenciální preference pro lokální/vlastní dialekt tlumeny bohatší jazykovou zkušeností dítěte.Metody. V percepčním online experimentu byla zkoumána dialektově podmíněná volba kamaráda. Každá z 12 experimentálních položek obsahovala dvojici vět, které byly nahrány dvěma různými mluvčími stejného pohlaví: jeden mluvil středočeskou varietou češtiny a druhý mluvil foneticky odlišným moravskoslezským dialektem. Formou nuceného výběru měli participanti v každém pokusu rozhodnout, se kterým ze dvou dětí by si chtěli raději hrát. Data byla sebrána od 81 partic
目的:本研究考察了学龄前和学龄前儿童的友谊选择是否受到说话人的地域方言的调节,以及儿童对当地/本族方言的潜在偏好是否会因儿童更多样化的语言经验而减弱。方法:采用在线强迫选择实验。12个试验中的每一个都包含由两个不同的同性说话者记录的一对句子:一个说的是捷克中部波西米亚方言,另一个说的是语音上截然不同的北摩拉维亚-西里西亚方言。来自两个地区的81名年龄在5到8岁之间的参与者收集了数据,他们在每次试验中听到两个句子后,表明他们喜欢和两个孩子中的哪一个一起玩。结果:广义线性混合效应模型显示,年龄和参与者所在地区的交互作用具有统计学意义(平均估计效应大小= 16.4%,SE = 8.1%, z = 2.031, p = 0.042)。两两比较表明,年轻的摩拉维亚-西里西亚儿童比年轻的中部波西米亚儿童对自己的方言有更强的偏好,比年长的摩拉维亚-西里西亚儿童也有轻微的偏好。结论:考虑到年龄较大的摩拉维亚-西里西亚儿童有更多样化的语言体验(与开始上学有关),可以解释这一结果,类似于任何年龄的中波希米亚儿童,由于他们所在地区的语言差异更大,他们有更多样化的语言体验。这种更丰富、更多样的语言体验可能会减弱孩子们对自己方言的偏好。限制:两个地区的招募程序保持相同,以招募具有同等动机的参与者的父母,这对于使用由儿童父母管理的在线实验收集相对可靠的数据至关重要。然而,这导致了相对较小的摩拉维亚-西里西亚儿童群体样本。后续研究(理想情况下以实验室为基础)需要测试当前研究结果的可重复性,并收集有关儿童语言、方言和教育背景的更详细信息。Cil。实验zkoumal zda jsou socialni偏好děti předškolniho一mladš日后školniho věku ovlivně纽约regionalnim dialektem mluvč日后一个zda mohou时代potencialni偏好pro lokalni / vlastni dialekt tlumeny bohatš我jazykovou zkušenosti说ěte.Metody。V percepčním在线实验byla zkoumána dialektovyi podmíněná volba kamaráda。Každa z 12 experimentalnich马球žek obsahovala dvojici vět, ktere由nahrany dvě马různymi mluvčimi stejneho pohlavi: jeden mluvil圣ř江户českou varietoučeš小druhy mluvil foneticky odlišnym moravskoslezskym dialektem。Formou nucensamicao výběru milu li participanti v každém pokusu rozhidout, se kterým ze dvou dětí by si chtreli raduji hrát。数据由la sebrána od 81参与者,kteří si poslechli obje nahrávky a následně se rozhdli, se kterým ze dvou mluvčích由si radirji hráli.Výsledky。Smíšený模型logistic回归模型odhalil významnou interakci v a局域性参与者(průměrná modelovan velikost efektu = 16.4%, SE = 8.1%, z = 2031, p = 0.042)。Párová srovnání ukázala, že mladší (vvk 4 a 5 let) moravskoslezsk dendriti milly silnější preferenci pro svůj lokální dialekt nekv mladší středočeské devi a okrajovje tak nekv starší (vvk 6 a 7 let) moravskoslezsk děti.Závěry。Výsledky naznačují, že jazykovov podmíněné sociální preference mohou být ovlivnnvny variovaností jazykov prostředí, ve kter se dítě nachází。Slabší preference pro rodný dialekt u mladších dětí ve Středočeském kraji nerov moravskoslezsksksamzm kraji mohou být výsledkem variovanějšího jazykovsamzho prostředí středočeského regionu (v etnje Prahy)。Stejně达克může时代(okrajověvyznamne) slabnuti偏好pro rodny dialekt u明星š我děti v ramci Moravskoslezskeho kraje spjato年代nastupem做ško,我ž说ěti taktežposkytuje variovanějš我jazykove普罗斯特ředi.Limity。Relativně马利vzorek mladš我skupiny moravskoslezskych děti, ktery ovšem reflektuje demografickou situaci: způ呜咽naboru参与者ů做experimentu byl pro oba regiony stejny,赎participanti v obou regionech pochazeli泽srovnatelněmotivovanychč阿斯蒂民众,což我柯某人ě俄文spolehlivych dat普罗斯特řednictvim在线experimentu klič林里。Autorky navrhují provcastnavazující výzkum, ideálně v laboratoři, který ověří replikovatelnost výsledků a bude sbírat více podrobných údajů ohlednje jazykovsamho a dialektovsamho pozadí dítěte。
{"title":"Jazykově podmíněné sociální preference dětí: vliv regionálního dialektu na výběr kamaráda","authors":"Natálie Kikoťová, Natalia Nudga, Jana Šimsová, Zita Maršíková, Kateřina Chládková","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.166","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.166","url":null,"abstract":"Language-based social preferences: The effect of dialect on friendship choices Objectives: This study investigated whether preschool and early school age children's friendship choices are modulated by a talker's regional dialect, and whether the potential preference for local/native dialect would be attenuated by a child's more diverse language experience. Methods: An online forced-choice experiment was administered. Each of 12 trials contained a pair of sentences recorded by two different same-sex speakers: one was a speaking the Central Bohemian variety of Czech and the other speaking a phonetically distinct Nothern Moravian-Silesian dialect. Data were collected from a total 81 participants, aged 5 to 8 years from both regions, who, upon listening to both sentences in each trial, indicated which of the two children they would like to play with. Results: A generalized linear mixed-effects model revealed a statistically significant interaction of age and participant location (average estimated effect size = 16.4 %, SE = 8.1 %, z = 2.031, p = .042). Pairwise comparisons showed that younger Moravian-Silesian children had a stronger preference for their own local dialect than younger Central Bohemian children, and marginally also than the older Moravian-Silesian children. Conclusions: The results can be explained by considering that older Moravian-Silesian children have a more diverse language experience (associated with starting school), similarly to Central Bohemian children of any age who have a more diverse language experience due to a greater linguistic variation in their region. Such richer, more varied, language experience might then attenuate children's preferences for their own dialect. Limitations: The recruitment procedures for both regions were kept identical in order to recruit comparably motivated participants' parents, which is crucial to collect comparably reliable data using an online experiment administered by the children's parents. This however resulted in a relatively small sample of the younger group of Moravian-Silesian children. Follow-up research, ideally lab-based, is needed to test the replicability of the present findings and to collect more detailed information on children's language, dialectal, and education background. Cíl. Experiment zkoumal, zda jsou sociální preference dětí předškolního a mladšího školního věku ovlivněny regionálním dialektem mluvčího a zda mohou být potenciální preference pro lokální/vlastní dialekt tlumeny bohatší jazykovou zkušeností dítěte.Metody. V percepčním online experimentu byla zkoumána dialektově podmíněná volba kamaráda. Každá z 12 experimentálních položek obsahovala dvojici vět, které byly nahrány dvěma různými mluvčími stejného pohlaví: jeden mluvil středočeskou varietou češtiny a druhý mluvil foneticky odlišným moravskoslezským dialektem. Formou nuceného výběru měli participanti v každém pokusu rozhodnout, se kterým ze dvou dětí by si chtěli raději hrát. Data byla sebrána od 81 partic","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"11 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136242773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-08-29DOI: 10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.213
Ľuboš Chvála, Zbyněk Vybíral
Objective: The aim of this study was to describe the many experiences of Mr. L with several psychotherapies over a 15-year period. Method: To present the experiences of Mr. L with psychotherapies, we used inductive thematic analysis. Results: We found many themes that were categorized under three major clusters as 1. negative experiences, 2. feelings of unfitness and inappropriate methods and styles of treatment, and 3. negative consequences of the therapies. We proposed an explanatory model to summarize how the experiences of Mr. L might be connected. Conclusions: The experiences of Mr. L with psychotherapies over the course of 15 years were associated with strong negative feelings, thoughts, and ongoing consequences. Some of his experiences were very unique. Practitioners of psychotherapy need to consider their work, because it sometimes can lead to feelings, thoughts, and consequences such as those Mr. L experienced. We proposed several possible methods for avoiding or addressing these phenomena.
{"title":"Case study: Inductive thematic analysis of negative experiences from several psychotherapies over 15 years","authors":"Ľuboš Chvála, Zbyněk Vybíral","doi":"10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.213","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.51561/cspsych.67.3-4.213","url":null,"abstract":"Objective: The aim of this study was to describe the many experiences of Mr. L with several psychotherapies over a 15-year period. Method: To present the experiences of Mr. L with psychotherapies, we used inductive thematic analysis. Results: We found many themes that were categorized under three major clusters as 1. negative experiences, 2. feelings of unfitness and inappropriate methods and styles of treatment, and 3. negative consequences of the therapies. We proposed an explanatory model to summarize how the experiences of Mr. L might be connected. Conclusions: The experiences of Mr. L with psychotherapies over the course of 15 years were associated with strong negative feelings, thoughts, and ongoing consequences. Some of his experiences were very unique. Practitioners of psychotherapy need to consider their work, because it sometimes can lead to feelings, thoughts, and consequences such as those Mr. L experienced. We proposed several possible methods for avoiding or addressing these phenomena.","PeriodicalId":44544,"journal":{"name":"Ceskoslovenska Psychologie","volume":"61 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136282838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}