首页 > 最新文献

Music Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Phenomenological Differences in Music- and Television-Evoked Autobiographical Memories 音乐与电视的现象学差异唤起自传记忆
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.435
Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, T. Eerola
Music can be a potent cue for autobiographical memories in both everyday and clinical settings. Understanding the extent to which music may have privileged access to aspects of our personal histories requires critical comparisons to other types of memories and exploration of how music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) vary across individuals. We compared the retrieval characteristics, content, and emotions of MEAMs to television-evoked autobiographical memories (TEAMs) in an online sample of 657 participants who were representative of the British adult population on age, gender, income, and education. Each participant reported details of a recent MEAM and a recent TEAM experience. MEAMs exhibited significantly greater episodic reliving, personal significance, and social content than TEAMs, and elicited more positive and intense emotions. The majority of these differences between MEAMs and TEAMs persisted in an analysis of a subset of responses in which the music and television cues were matched on familiarity. Age and gender effects were smaller, and consistent across both MEAMs and TEAMs. These results indicate phenomenological differences in naturally occurring memories cued by music as compared to television that are maintained across adulthood. Findings are discussed in the context of theoretical accounts of autobiographical memory, functions of music, and healthy aging.
在日常和临床环境中,音乐都可以成为自传记忆的有力线索。要了解音乐在多大程度上有权接触我们个人历史的各个方面,需要与其他类型的记忆进行批判性比较,并探索音乐引发的自传体记忆(MEAM)在个体之间的差异。我们在657名参与者的在线样本中比较了MEAM与电视诱发自传体记忆(TEAM)的检索特征、内容和情绪,这些参与者在年龄、性别、收入和教育方面代表了英国成年人口。每位参与者都报告了最近MEAM的详细信息和最近TEAM的经历。MEAM比TEAM表现出更大的情节重温、个人意义和社交内容,并引发更积极和强烈的情绪。MEAM和TEAM之间的大多数差异持续存在于对一组反应的分析中,其中音乐和电视提示在熟悉度上匹配。年龄和性别的影响较小,并且在MEAM和TEAM中是一致的。这些结果表明,与成年期保持的电视相比,音乐提示的自然发生的记忆存在现象学差异。研究结果是在自传体记忆、音乐功能和健康衰老的理论描述的背景下进行讨论的。
{"title":"Phenomenological Differences in Music- and Television-Evoked Autobiographical Memories","authors":"Kelly Jakubowski, Amy M. Belfi, T. Eerola","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.435","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.435","url":null,"abstract":"Music can be a potent cue for autobiographical memories in both everyday and clinical settings. Understanding the extent to which music may have privileged access to aspects of our personal histories requires critical comparisons to other types of memories and exploration of how music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) vary across individuals. We compared the retrieval characteristics, content, and emotions of MEAMs to television-evoked autobiographical memories (TEAMs) in an online sample of 657 participants who were representative of the British adult population on age, gender, income, and education. Each participant reported details of a recent MEAM and a recent TEAM experience. MEAMs exhibited significantly greater episodic reliving, personal significance, and social content than TEAMs, and elicited more positive and intense emotions. The majority of these differences between MEAMs and TEAMs persisted in an analysis of a subset of responses in which the music and television cues were matched on familiarity. Age and gender effects were smaller, and consistent across both MEAMs and TEAMs. These results indicate phenomenological differences in naturally occurring memories cued by music as compared to television that are maintained across adulthood. Findings are discussed in the context of theoretical accounts of autobiographical memory, functions of music, and healthy aging.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47415023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Probe Tone Paradigm Reveals Less Differentiated Tonal Hierarchy in Rock Music 探索音调范式揭示摇滚乐中较少分化的音调层次
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.425
Dominique Vuvan, Bryn Hughes
Krumhansl and Kessler’s (1982) pioneering experiments on tonal hierarchies in Western music have long been considered the gold standard for researchers interested in the mental representation of musical pitch structure. The current experiment used the probe tone technique to investigate the tonal hierarchy in classical and rock music. As predicted, the observed profiles for these two styles were structurally similar, reflecting a shared underlying Western tonal structure. Most interestingly, however, the rock profile was significantly less differentiated than the classical profile, reflecting theoretical work that describes pitch organization in rock music as more permissive and less hierarchical than in classical music. This line of research contradicts the idea that music from the common-practice era is representative of all Western musics, and challenges music cognition researchers to explore style-appropriate stimuli and models of pitch structure for their experiments.
Krumhansl和Kessler(1982)对西方音乐中音调等级的开创性实验长期以来一直被认为是对音乐音高结构的心理表征感兴趣的研究人员的黄金标准。本实验采用探测音调技术对古典音乐和摇滚乐的音调层次进行了研究。正如预测的那样,观察到的这两种风格的轮廓在结构上是相似的,反映了共同的潜在的西方色调结构。然而,最有趣的是,与古典音乐相比,摇滚音乐的音高差异明显更小,这反映了理论工作,即摇滚音乐的音高组织比古典音乐更宽松,层次更少。这一研究路线与“普通实践时代的音乐代表所有西方音乐”的观点相矛盾,并挑战了音乐认知研究者为他们的实验探索适合风格的刺激和音高结构模型。
{"title":"Probe Tone Paradigm Reveals Less Differentiated Tonal Hierarchy in Rock Music","authors":"Dominique Vuvan, Bryn Hughes","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.425","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.425","url":null,"abstract":"Krumhansl and Kessler’s (1982) pioneering experiments on tonal hierarchies in Western music have long been considered the gold standard for researchers interested in the mental representation of musical pitch structure. The current experiment used the probe tone technique to investigate the tonal hierarchy in classical and rock music. As predicted, the observed profiles for these two styles were structurally similar, reflecting a shared underlying Western tonal structure. Most interestingly, however, the rock profile was significantly less differentiated than the classical profile, reflecting theoretical work that describes pitch organization in rock music as more permissive and less hierarchical than in classical music. This line of research contradicts the idea that music from the common-practice era is representative of all Western musics, and challenges music cognition researchers to explore style-appropriate stimuli and models of pitch structure for their experiments.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47928086","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
What Drives Narrative Engagement With Music? 是什么推动了音乐的叙事参与?
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.509
J. Devin McAuley, P. Wong, Lucas Bellaiche, E. Margulis
Although people across multiple cultures have been shown to experience music narratively, it has proven difficult to disentangle whether narrative dimensions of music derive from learned extramusical associations within a culture or from less experience-dependent elements of the music, such as musical contrast. Toward this end, two experiments investigated factors contributing to listeners’ narrative engagement with music, comparing the narrative experiences of Western and Chinese instrumental music for listeners in two suburban locations in the United States with those of listeners living in a remote rural village in China with different patterns of musical exposure. Supporting an enculturation perspective where learned extramusical associations (i.e., Topicality) play an important role in narrative perceptions of music, results from the first experiment show that for Western listeners, greater Topicality, rather than greater Contrast, increases narrative engagement, as long as listeners have sufficient exposure to its patterns of use within a culture. Strengthening this interpretation, results for the second experiment, which directly manipulated Topicality and Contrast, show that reducing an excerpt’s Topicality, but not its Contrast reduces listeners’ narrative engagement.
尽管不同文化背景的人都以叙事的方式体验音乐,但事实证明,很难理清音乐的叙事维度是来自一种文化中习得的音乐外联想,还是来自音乐中较少依赖经验的元素,比如音乐对比。为此,两个实验调查了影响听众对音乐的叙述参与的因素,比较了美国两个郊区的听众对西方和中国器乐的叙述体验,以及生活在中国偏远农村的听众对不同音乐接触模式的叙述体验。第一个实验的结果表明,对于西方听众来说,只要听众充分了解其在文化中的使用模式,更强的话题性(Topicality),而不是更强的对比,就会增加叙事的参与度,这支持了一种文化适应的观点。为了加强这种解释,第二个实验的结果直接操纵了话题性和对比,结果表明,减少节选的话题性,而不是对比,会降低听者的叙事参与度。
{"title":"What Drives Narrative Engagement With Music?","authors":"J. Devin McAuley, P. Wong, Lucas Bellaiche, E. Margulis","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.509","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.509","url":null,"abstract":"Although people across multiple cultures have been shown to experience music narratively, it has proven difficult to disentangle whether narrative dimensions of music derive from learned extramusical associations within a culture or from less experience-dependent elements of the music, such as musical contrast. Toward this end, two experiments investigated factors contributing to listeners’ narrative engagement with music, comparing the narrative experiences of Western and Chinese instrumental music for listeners in two suburban locations in the United States with those of listeners living in a remote rural village in China with different patterns of musical exposure. Supporting an enculturation perspective where learned extramusical associations (i.e., Topicality) play an important role in narrative perceptions of music, results from the first experiment show that for Western listeners, greater Topicality, rather than greater Contrast, increases narrative engagement, as long as listeners have sufficient exposure to its patterns of use within a culture. Strengthening this interpretation, results for the second experiment, which directly manipulated Topicality and Contrast, show that reducing an excerpt’s Topicality, but not its Contrast reduces listeners’ narrative engagement.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47035118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Singing in the Brain 大脑中的歌唱
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.456
Callula Killingly, Philippe Lacherez, R. Meuter
Music that gets “stuck” in the head is commonly conceptualized as an intrusive “thought”; however, we argue that this experience is better characterized as automatic mental singing without an accompanying sense of agency. In two experiments, a dual-task paradigm was employed, in which participants undertook a phonological task once while hearing music, and then again in silence following its presentation. We predicted that the music would be maintained in working memory, interfering with the task. Experiment 1 (N = 30) used songs predicted to be more or less catchy; half of the sample heard truncated versions. Performance was indeed poorer following catchier songs, particularly if the songs were unfinished. Moreover, the effect was stronger for songs rated higher in terms of the desire to sing along. Experiment 2 (N = 50) replicated the effect using songs with which the participants felt compelled to sing along. Additionally, results from a lexical decision task indicated that many participants’ keystrokes synchronized with the tempo of the song just heard. Together, these findings suggest that an earworm results from an unconscious desire to sing along to a familiar song.
“卡在脑子里”的音乐通常被概念化为一种侵入性的“思想”;然而,我们认为,这种体验最好被描述为没有伴随的代理感的自动心理歌唱。在两个实验中,采用了双重任务范式,参与者在听音乐时进行一次语音任务,然后在音乐呈现后再次沉默。我们预测音乐会保留在工作记忆中,干扰任务。实验1(N=30)使用了被预测为或多或少朗朗上口的歌曲;一半的样本听到了截短的版本。在更吸引人的歌曲之后,表现确实较差,尤其是在歌曲未完成的情况下。此外,在随唱欲望方面评分较高的歌曲的效果更强。实验2(N=50)使用参与者觉得必须跟着唱的歌曲来复制这种效果。此外,词汇决策任务的结果表明,许多参与者的击键与刚听到的歌曲的节奏同步。总之,这些发现表明,耳虫是由于无意识地想跟着熟悉的歌曲唱歌而产生的。
{"title":"Singing in the Brain","authors":"Callula Killingly, Philippe Lacherez, R. Meuter","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.456","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.456","url":null,"abstract":"Music that gets “stuck” in the head is commonly conceptualized as an intrusive “thought”; however, we argue that this experience is better characterized as automatic mental singing without an accompanying sense of agency. In two experiments, a dual-task paradigm was employed, in which participants undertook a phonological task once while hearing music, and then again in silence following its presentation. We predicted that the music would be maintained in working memory, interfering with the task. Experiment 1 (N = 30) used songs predicted to be more or less catchy; half of the sample heard truncated versions. Performance was indeed poorer following catchier songs, particularly if the songs were unfinished. Moreover, the effect was stronger for songs rated higher in terms of the desire to sing along. Experiment 2 (N = 50) replicated the effect using songs with which the participants felt compelled to sing along. Additionally, results from a lexical decision task indicated that many participants’ keystrokes synchronized with the tempo of the song just heard. Together, these findings suggest that an earworm results from an unconscious desire to sing along to a familiar song.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49220788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Music to Your Ears 音乐入耳
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.499
Tamara Rathcke, Simone Falk, S. D. Bella
Listeners usually have no difficulties telling the difference between speech and song. Yet when a spoken phrase is repeated several times, they often report a perceptual transformation that turns speech into song. There is a great deal of variability in the perception of the speech-to-song illusion (STS). It may result partly from linguistic properties of spoken phrases and be partly due to the individual processing difference of listeners exposed to STS. To date, existing evidence is insufficient to predict who is most likely to experience the transformation, and which sentences may be more conducive to the transformation once spoken repeatedly. The present study investigates these questions with French and English listeners, testing the hypothesis that the transformation is achieved by means of functional re-evaluation of phrasal prosody during repetition. Such prosodic re-analysis places demands on the phonological structure of sentences and language proficiency of listeners. Two experiments show that STS is facilitated in high-sonority sentences and in listeners’ non-native languages and support the hypothesis that STS involves a switch between musical and linguistic perception modes.
听众通常很容易分辨出语音和歌曲之间的区别。然而,当一个口语短语重复几次时,他们通常会报告一种感知转变,将语音转化为歌曲。语音对歌曲错觉(STS)的感知存在很大的可变性。这在一定程度上可能是由于口语短语的语言特性造成的,也可能是由于接触STS的听众的个体处理差异造成的。到目前为止,现有的证据不足以预测谁最有可能经历这种转变,以及重复说出哪些句子可能更有利于这种转变。本研究对法语和英语听众的这些问题进行了调查,检验了通过在重复过程中对短语韵律进行功能性重新评估来实现转换的假设。这种韵律重新分析对句子的语音结构和听众的语言能力提出了要求。两个实验表明,在高发音句子和听众的非母语中,STS是容易的,并支持STS涉及音乐和语言感知模式之间的转换的假设。
{"title":"Music to Your Ears","authors":"Tamara Rathcke, Simone Falk, S. D. Bella","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.499","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.5.499","url":null,"abstract":"Listeners usually have no difficulties telling the difference between speech and song. Yet when a spoken phrase is repeated several times, they often report a perceptual transformation that turns speech into song. There is a great deal of variability in the perception of the speech-to-song illusion (STS). It may result partly from linguistic properties of spoken phrases and be partly due to the individual processing difference of listeners exposed to STS. To date, existing evidence is insufficient to predict who is most likely to experience the transformation, and which sentences may be more conducive to the transformation once spoken repeatedly. The present study investigates these questions with French and English listeners, testing the hypothesis that the transformation is achieved by means of functional re-evaluation of phrasal prosody during repetition. Such prosodic re-analysis places demands on the phonological structure of sentences and language proficiency of listeners. Two experiments show that STS is facilitated in high-sonority sentences and in listeners’ non-native languages and support the hypothesis that STS involves a switch between musical and linguistic perception modes.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47914250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Establishing the Reliability and Validity of Web-based Singing Research 建立网络歌唱研究的信度和有效性
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.386
Y. Tan, I. Peretz, G. McPherson, Sarah J Wilson
In this study, the robustness of an online tool for objectively assessing singing ability was examined by: (1) determining the internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the tool; (2) comparing the task performance of web-based participants (n = 285) with a group (n = 52) completing the tool in a controlled laboratory setting, and then determining the convergent validity between settings, and (3) comparing participants’ task performance with previous research using similar singing tasks and populations. Results indicated that the online singing tool exhibited high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .92), and moderate-to-high test-retest reliabilities (.65–.80) across an average 4.5-year-span. Task performance for web- and laboratory-based participants (n = 82) matched on age, sex, and music training were not significantly different. Moderate-to-large correlations (|r| =.31–.59) were found between self-rated singing ability and the various singing tasks, supporting convergent validity. Finally, task performance of the web-based sample was not significantly different to previously reported findings. Overall the findings support the robustness of the online tool for objectively measuring singing pitch accuracy beyond a controlled laboratory environment and its potential application in large-scale investigations of singing and music ability.
在本研究中,通过以下方法检验了客观评估歌唱能力的在线工具的稳健性:(1)确定该工具的内部一致性和重测信度;(2)将基于网络的参与者(n = 285)的任务表现与在受控实验室环境中完成工具的一组(n = 52)进行比较,然后确定设置之间的收敛效度;(3)将参与者的任务表现与先前使用类似唱歌任务和人群的研究进行比较。结果表明,在线歌唱工具在平均4.5年的时间跨度内表现出较高的内部一致性(Cronbach 's alpha = 0.92)和中高的重测信度(0.65 - 0.80)。基于网络和实验室的参与者(n = 82)的任务表现在年龄、性别和音乐训练方面没有显著差异。自评歌唱能力与各种歌唱任务之间存在中高相关性(| = 0.31 - 0.59),支持收敛效度。最后,基于网络的样本的任务表现与先前报道的结果没有显著差异。总的来说,这些发现支持了在线工具在受控实验室环境之外客观测量歌唱音调准确性的稳健性,以及它在歌唱和音乐能力的大规模调查中的潜在应用。
{"title":"Establishing the Reliability and Validity of Web-based Singing Research","authors":"Y. Tan, I. Peretz, G. McPherson, Sarah J Wilson","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.386","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.386","url":null,"abstract":"In this study, the robustness of an online tool for objectively assessing singing ability was examined by: (1) determining the internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the tool; (2) comparing the task performance of web-based participants (n = 285) with a group (n = 52) completing the tool in a controlled laboratory setting, and then determining the convergent validity between settings, and (3) comparing participants’ task performance with previous research using similar singing tasks and populations. Results indicated that the online singing tool exhibited high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .92), and moderate-to-high test-retest reliabilities (.65–.80) across an average 4.5-year-span. Task performance for web- and laboratory-based participants (n = 82) matched on age, sex, and music training were not significantly different. Moderate-to-large correlations (|r| =.31–.59) were found between self-rated singing ability and the various singing tasks, supporting convergent validity. Finally, task performance of the web-based sample was not significantly different to previously reported findings. Overall the findings support the robustness of the online tool for objectively measuring singing pitch accuracy beyond a controlled laboratory environment and its potential application in large-scale investigations of singing and music ability.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44355524","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Wait For It 注意了
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.345
A. Turrell, A. Halpern, A. Javadi
Previous brain-related studies on music-evoked emotions have relied on listening to long music segments, which may reduce the precision of correlating emotional cues to specific brain areas. Break routines in electronic dance music (EDM) are emotive but short music moments containing three passages: breakdown, build-up, and drop. Within build-ups music features increase to peak moments prior to highly expected drop passages and peak-pleasurable emotions when these expectations are fulfilled. The neural correlates of peak-pleasurable emotions (such as excitement) in the short seconds of build-up and drop passages in EDM break routines are therefore good candidates to study brain correlates of emotion. Thirty-six participants listened to break routines while undergoing continuous EEG. Source reconstruction of EEG epochs for one second of build-up and of drop passages showed that pre- and post-central gyri and precuneus were more active during build-ups, and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) were more active within drop passages. Importantly, IFG and MFG activity showed a correlation with ratings of subjective excitement during drop passages. The results suggest expectation is important in inducing peak-pleasurable experiences and brain activity changes within seconds of reported feelings of excitement during EDM break routines.
先前关于音乐诱发情绪的大脑相关研究依赖于听长音乐片段,这可能会降低将情绪线索与特定大脑区域关联的准确性。电子舞曲(EDM)中的中断程序是情绪化但短暂的音乐时刻,包含三个段落:崩溃、积累和下降。在构建过程中,音乐特征在高度期望的下降段落之前达到峰值,当这些期望得到满足时,达到愉悦情绪的峰值。因此,在EDM休息程序中,在短暂的几秒钟内,愉悦情绪(如兴奋)峰值的神经相关性是研究情绪大脑相关性的好候选者。36名受试者在接受连续脑电图检查的同时,聆听了中断的日常活动。对建立和下降通道1秒的EEG历元进行源重建,结果显示,在建立过程中,前、后中央回和楔前叶更活跃,额下回(IFG)和额中回(MFG)在下降通道内更活跃。重要的是,IFG和MFG活动与下降通道中的主观兴奋评级相关。研究结果表明,在EDM休息过程中,期望在报告的兴奋感的几秒钟内诱导最高的愉悦体验和大脑活动变化是很重要的。
{"title":"Wait For It","authors":"A. Turrell, A. Halpern, A. Javadi","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.345","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.345","url":null,"abstract":"Previous brain-related studies on music-evoked emotions have relied on listening to long music segments, which may reduce the precision of correlating emotional cues to specific brain areas. Break routines in electronic dance music (EDM) are emotive but short music moments containing three passages: breakdown, build-up, and drop. Within build-ups music features increase to peak moments prior to highly expected drop passages and peak-pleasurable emotions when these expectations are fulfilled. The neural correlates of peak-pleasurable emotions (such as excitement) in the short seconds of build-up and drop passages in EDM break routines are therefore good candidates to study brain correlates of emotion. Thirty-six participants listened to break routines while undergoing continuous EEG. Source reconstruction of EEG epochs for one second of build-up and of drop passages showed that pre- and post-central gyri and precuneus were more active during build-ups, and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) were more active within drop passages. Importantly, IFG and MFG activity showed a correlation with ratings of subjective excitement during drop passages. The results suggest expectation is important in inducing peak-pleasurable experiences and brain activity changes within seconds of reported feelings of excitement during EDM break routines.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41459818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Playing It Straight 直来直去
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.372
C. Corcoran, K. Frieler
The most recognizable features of the jazz phrasing style known as “swing” is the articulation of tactus beat subdivisions into long-short patterns (known as “swing eighths”). The subdivisions are traditionally assumed to form a 2:1 beat-upbeat ratio (BUR); however, several smaller case studies have suggested that the 2:1 BUR is a gross oversimplification. Here we offer a more conclusive approach to the issue, offering a corpus analysis of 456 jazz solos using the Weimar Jazz Database. Results indicate that most jazz soloists tend to play with only slightly uneven swing eighths (BUR = 1.3:1), while BURs approaching 2:1 and higher are only used occasionally. High BURs are more likely to be used systematically at slow and moderate tempi and in Postbop and Hardbop styles. Overall, the data suggests that a stable 2:1 swing BUR for solos is a conceptual myth, which may be based on various perceptual effects. We suggest that higher BURs are likely saved for specific effect, since higher BURs may maximize entrainment and the sense of groove at the tactus beat level among listeners and performers. Consequently our results contribute with insights relevant to jazz, groove, and microrhythm studies, practical and historical jazz research, and music perception.
被称为“摇摆”的爵士乐句风格最明显的特征是将节拍细分为长短模式(被称为“摇摆八分”)。传统上,细分被认为形成2:1的拍升比(BUR);然而,一些较小的案例研究表明,2:1的BUR是一种过度简化。在这里,我们提供了一个更结论性的方法来解决这个问题,使用魏玛爵士数据库提供了456首爵士独奏的语料库分析。结果表明,大多数爵士独奏家倾向于只使用稍微不均匀的摇摆八分(BUR = 1.3:1),而接近2:1及更高的BURs只是偶尔使用。高BURs更有可能在慢节奏和中等节奏以及Postbop和Hardbop风格中系统地使用。总的来说,数据表明,一个稳定的2:1的摇摆BUR为独奏是一个概念上的神话,这可能是基于各种知觉效应。我们认为较高的BURs可能是为了特定的效果而保留的,因为较高的BURs可能会最大化听众和表演者在节拍水平上的愉悦感和律动感。因此,我们的研究结果有助于对爵士乐、groove和微节奏研究、实践和历史爵士乐研究以及音乐感知的相关见解。
{"title":"Playing It Straight","authors":"C. Corcoran, K. Frieler","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.372","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.372","url":null,"abstract":"The most recognizable features of the jazz phrasing style known as “swing” is the articulation of tactus beat subdivisions into long-short patterns (known as “swing eighths”). The subdivisions are traditionally assumed to form a 2:1 beat-upbeat ratio (BUR); however, several smaller case studies have suggested that the 2:1 BUR is a gross oversimplification. Here we offer a more conclusive approach to the issue, offering a corpus analysis of 456 jazz solos using the Weimar Jazz Database. Results indicate that most jazz soloists tend to play with only slightly uneven swing eighths (BUR = 1.3:1), while BURs approaching 2:1 and higher are only used occasionally. High BURs are more likely to be used systematically at slow and moderate tempi and in Postbop and Hardbop styles. Overall, the data suggests that a stable 2:1 swing BUR for solos is a conceptual myth, which may be based on various perceptual effects. We suggest that higher BURs are likely saved for specific effect, since higher BURs may maximize entrainment and the sense of groove at the tactus beat level among listeners and performers. Consequently our results contribute with insights relevant to jazz, groove, and microrhythm studies, practical and historical jazz research, and music perception.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41500770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Reproduction Accuracy for Short Rhythms Following Melodic or Monotonic Presentation 旋律或单调呈现后短节奏的再现准确性
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.360
Andrew V. Frane, M. Monti
Some researchers and study participants have expressed an intuition that novel rhythmic sequences are easier to recall and reproduce if they have a melody, implying that melodicity (the presence of musical pitch variation) fundamentally enhances perception and/or representation of rhythm. But the psychoacoustics literature suggests that pitch variation often impairs perception of temporal information. To examine the effect of melodicity on rhythm reproduction accuracy, we presented simple nine-note auditory rhythms to 100 college students, who attempted to reproduce those rhythms by tapping. Reproductions tended to be more accurate when the presented notes all had the same pitch than when the presented notes had a melody. Nonetheless, a plurality of participants judged that the melodically presented rhythms were easier to remember. We also found that sequences containing a Scotch snap (a sixteenth note at a quarter note beat position followed by a dotted eighth note) were reproduced less accurately than other sequences in general, and less accurately than other sequences containing a dotted eighth note.
一些研究人员和研究参与者表达了一种直觉,即如果有旋律,新颖的节奏序列更容易回忆和再现,这意味着旋律性(音高变化的存在)从根本上增强了对节奏的感知和/或表现。但心理声学文献表明,音高的变化往往会削弱对时间信息的感知。为了检验旋律性对节奏再现准确性的影响,我们向100名大学生展示了简单的九音符听觉节奏,他们试图通过敲击来再现这些节奏。当呈现的音符都有相同的音高时,再现往往比呈现的音符有旋律时更准确。尽管如此,许多参与者认为旋律优美的节奏更容易记住。我们还发现,通常情况下,包含苏格兰音符的序列(四分之一音符节拍位置的十六分音符,后面是带点的八分音符)的再现不如其他序列准确,也不如其他包含带点八分音符的序列准确。
{"title":"Reproduction Accuracy for Short Rhythms Following Melodic or Monotonic Presentation","authors":"Andrew V. Frane, M. Monti","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.360","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.360","url":null,"abstract":"Some researchers and study participants have expressed an intuition that novel rhythmic sequences are easier to recall and reproduce if they have a melody, implying that melodicity (the presence of musical pitch variation) fundamentally enhances perception and/or representation of rhythm. But the psychoacoustics literature suggests that pitch variation often impairs perception of temporal information. To examine the effect of melodicity on rhythm reproduction accuracy, we presented simple nine-note auditory rhythms to 100 college students, who attempted to reproduce those rhythms by tapping. Reproductions tended to be more accurate when the presented notes all had the same pitch than when the presented notes had a melody. Nonetheless, a plurality of participants judged that the melodically presented rhythms were easier to remember. We also found that sequences containing a Scotch snap (a sixteenth note at a quarter note beat position followed by a dotted eighth note) were reproduced less accurately than other sequences in general, and less accurately than other sequences containing a dotted eighth note.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41448926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing Music Expertise 评估音乐专业知识
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.406
Ziyong Lin, A. Werner, U. Lindenberger, A. Brandmaier, Elisabeth Wenger
We introduce the Berlin Gehoerbildung Scale (BGS), a multidimensional assessment of music expertise in amateur musicians and music professionals. The BGS is informed by music theory and uses a variety of testing methods in the ear-training tradition, with items covering four different dimensions of music expertise: (1) intervals and scales, (2) dictation, (3) chords and cadences, and (4) complex listening. We validated the test in a sample of amateur musicians, aspiring professional musicians, and students attending a highly competitive music conservatory (n = 59). Using structural equation modeling, we compared two factor models: a unidimensional model postulating a single factor of music expertise; and a hierarchical model, according to which four first-order subscale factors load on a second-order factor of general music expertise. The hierarchical model showed better fit to the data than the unidimensional model, indicating that the four subscales capture reliable variance above and beyond the general factor of music expertise. There were reliable group differences on both the second-order general factor and the four subscales, with music students outperforming aspiring professionals and amateur musicians. We conclude that the BGS is an adequate measurement instrument for assessing individual differences in music expertise, especially at high levels of expertise.
我们介绍柏林Gehoerbildung量表(BGS),这是对业余音乐家和音乐专业人士的音乐专业知识的多维评估。BGS以音乐理论为基础,采用多种耳训练传统的测试方法,包括四个不同方面的音乐专业知识:(1)音程和音阶,(2)听写,(3)和弦和节奏,(4)复杂听力。我们在业余音乐家、有抱负的专业音乐家和竞争激烈的音乐学院的学生(n = 59)的样本中验证了该测试。利用结构方程模型,我们比较了两个因素模型:一维模型假设一个单一因素的音乐专长;以及一个层次模型,根据该模型,四个一阶子尺度因子负载在一般音乐专业知识的二阶因子上。层次模型比一维模型更适合于数据,表明四个子量表捕获了超出音乐专业一般因素的可靠方差。在二阶一般因素和四个分量表上都存在可靠的群体差异,音乐学生的表现优于有抱负的专业人士和业余音乐家。我们得出结论,BGS是评估音乐专业知识个体差异的适当测量工具,特别是在高水平的专业知识中。
{"title":"Assessing Music Expertise","authors":"Ziyong Lin, A. Werner, U. Lindenberger, A. Brandmaier, Elisabeth Wenger","doi":"10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.406","url":null,"abstract":"We introduce the Berlin Gehoerbildung Scale (BGS), a multidimensional assessment of music expertise in amateur musicians and music professionals. The BGS is informed by music theory and uses a variety of testing methods in the ear-training tradition, with items covering four different dimensions of music expertise: (1) intervals and scales, (2) dictation, (3) chords and cadences, and (4) complex listening. We validated the test in a sample of amateur musicians, aspiring professional musicians, and students attending a highly competitive music conservatory (n = 59). Using structural equation modeling, we compared two factor models: a unidimensional model postulating a single factor of music expertise; and a hierarchical model, according to which four first-order subscale factors load on a second-order factor of general music expertise. The hierarchical model showed better fit to the data than the unidimensional model, indicating that the four subscales capture reliable variance above and beyond the general factor of music expertise. There were reliable group differences on both the second-order general factor and the four subscales, with music students outperforming aspiring professionals and amateur musicians. We conclude that the BGS is an adequate measurement instrument for assessing individual differences in music expertise, especially at high levels of expertise.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44225425","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Music Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1