首页 > 最新文献

Music Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Children’s Sensitivity to Performance Expression and its Relationship to Children’s Empathy 儿童对行为表达的敏感性及其与共情的关系
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.12
Cecilia Taher
Emotional communication is central to music performance expression and empathy. Research has shown that music activities can enhance empathy in children and that more empathic adults can more accurately recognize and feel performers’ expressive intentions. Nevertheless, little is known about performance expression during childhood and the specific music-related factors affecting empathy development. This paper explores children’s sensitivity to a performer’s expressive or mechanical intentions and its relationship to children’s everyday empathy. Twenty-seven children listened to expressive and mechanical versions of Romantic flute excerpts with and without accompanying video, rating their perceived level of the performer’s expression and their enjoyment of the performance. The results indicate that children recognize performers’ intended expression or lack thereof and enjoy expressive performances more than mechanical ones. Children aged 10–12 recognized performance expression better than those aged 8–9, especially in audiovisual conditions. Children with higher cognitive empathy rated performance expression more in line with their enjoyment of the performance, which was also more concordant with the performer’s expressive intention. The findings support a relationship between music and socio-emotional skills and emphasize the importance of the visual component of music performance for children, an aspect that has received little attention among researchers and educators.
情感交流是音乐表演、表达和移情的核心。研究表明,音乐活动可以增强儿童的同理心,而更有同理心的成年人可以更准确地识别和感受表演者的表达意图。然而,对儿童时期的表演表达和影响共情发展的具体音乐相关因素知之甚少。本文探讨儿童对表演者的表达性或机械性意图的敏感性及其与儿童日常共情的关系。27个孩子听了浪漫长笛的表达和机械版本,有和没有附带视频,评价他们对表演者表达的感知水平和他们对表演的享受。结果表明,儿童能够识别表演者有意表达或缺乏有意表达的意图,并且更喜欢表现性的表演而不是机械性的表演。10-12岁儿童对表演表达的识别能力优于8-9岁儿童,尤其是在视听条件下。认知共情水平高的儿童对表演表达的评价更符合他们对表演的享受,也更符合表演者的表达意图。研究结果支持了音乐与社会情感技能之间的关系,并强调了儿童音乐表演中视觉成分的重要性,这方面很少受到研究人员和教育工作者的关注。
{"title":"Children’s Sensitivity to Performance Expression and its Relationship to Children’s Empathy","authors":"Cecilia Taher","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.12","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.12","url":null,"abstract":"Emotional communication is central to music performance expression and empathy. Research has shown that music activities can enhance empathy in children and that more empathic adults can more accurately recognize and feel performers’ expressive intentions. Nevertheless, little is known about performance expression during childhood and the specific music-related factors affecting empathy development. This paper explores children’s sensitivity to a performer’s expressive or mechanical intentions and its relationship to children’s everyday empathy. Twenty-seven children listened to expressive and mechanical versions of Romantic flute excerpts with and without accompanying video, rating their perceived level of the performer’s expression and their enjoyment of the performance. The results indicate that children recognize performers’ intended expression or lack thereof and enjoy expressive performances more than mechanical ones. Children aged 10–12 recognized performance expression better than those aged 8–9, especially in audiovisual conditions. Children with higher cognitive empathy rated performance expression more in line with their enjoyment of the performance, which was also more concordant with the performer’s expressive intention. The findings support a relationship between music and socio-emotional skills and emphasize the importance of the visual component of music performance for children, an aspect that has received little attention among researchers and educators.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47736552","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotions, Mechanisms, and Individual Differences in Music Listening 音乐聆听中的情绪、机制和个体差异
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.55
Patrik N. Juslin, Laura S. Sakka, G. Barradas, O. Lartillot
Emotions have been found to play a paramount role in both everyday music experiences and health applications of music, but the applicability of musical emotions depends on: 1) which emotions music can induce, 2) how it induces them, and 3) how individual differences may be explained. These questions were addressed in a listening test, where 44 participants (aged 19–66 years) reported both felt emotions and subjective impressions of emotion mechanisms (Mec Scale), while listening to 72 pieces of music from 12 genres, selected using a stratified random sampling procedure. The results showed that: 1) positive emotions (e.g., happiness) were more prevalent than negative emotions (e.g., anger); 2) Rhythmic entrainment was the most and Brain stem reflex the least frequent of the mechanisms featured in the BRECVEMA theory; 3) felt emotions could be accurately predicted based on self-reported mechanisms in multiple regression analyses; 4) self-reported mechanisms predicted felt emotions better than did acoustic features; and 5) individual listeners showed partly different emotion-mechanism links across stimuli, which may help to explain individual differences in emotional responses. Implications for future research and applications of musical emotions are discussed.
人们发现情绪在日常音乐体验和音乐的健康应用中都起着至关重要的作用,但音乐情绪的适用性取决于:1)音乐可以诱导哪些情绪,2)如何诱导情绪,以及3)如何解释个体差异。在一项听力测试中,44名参与者(年龄在19-66岁之间)报告了他们的感受情绪和情绪机制的主观印象(Mec量表),同时听了12种类型的72首音乐,这些音乐是通过分层随机抽样程序选择的。结果表明:1)积极情绪(如快乐)比消极情绪(如愤怒)更为普遍;2)在BRECVEMA理论中,节律性夹带的频率最高,脑干反射的频率最低;3)多元回归分析中,基于自我报告机制的感觉情绪能够准确预测;4)自我报告机制对感觉情绪的预测优于声学特征;个体听者在不同刺激下表现出部分不同的情绪机制联系,这可能有助于解释情绪反应的个体差异。讨论了音乐情感对未来研究和应用的启示。
{"title":"Emotions, Mechanisms, and Individual Differences in Music Listening","authors":"Patrik N. Juslin, Laura S. Sakka, G. Barradas, O. Lartillot","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.55","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.55","url":null,"abstract":"Emotions have been found to play a paramount role in both everyday music experiences and health applications of music, but the applicability of musical emotions depends on: 1) which emotions music can induce, 2) how it induces them, and 3) how individual differences may be explained. These questions were addressed in a listening test, where 44 participants (aged 19–66 years) reported both felt emotions and subjective impressions of emotion mechanisms (Mec Scale), while listening to 72 pieces of music from 12 genres, selected using a stratified random sampling procedure. The results showed that: 1) positive emotions (e.g., happiness) were more prevalent than negative emotions (e.g., anger); 2) Rhythmic entrainment was the most and Brain stem reflex the least frequent of the mechanisms featured in the BRECVEMA theory; 3) felt emotions could be accurately predicted based on self-reported mechanisms in multiple regression analyses; 4) self-reported mechanisms predicted felt emotions better than did acoustic features; and 5) individual listeners showed partly different emotion-mechanism links across stimuli, which may help to explain individual differences in emotional responses. Implications for future research and applications of musical emotions are discussed.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43131392","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Musicians Can Reliably Discriminate Between String Register Locations on the Violoncello 音乐家可以可靠地区分小提琴弦寄存器的位置
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.27
C. Trevor, J. Devaney, David Huron
Vocal range location is an important vocal affective signal. Humans use different areas of their vocal range to communicate emotional intensity. Consequently, humans are good at identifying where someone is speaking within their vocal range. Research on music and emotion has demonstrated that musical expressive behaviors often reflect or take inspiration from vocal expressive behaviors. Is it possible for musicians to utilize range-related signals on their instrument similarly to how humans use vocal range-related signals? Might musicians therefore be similarly sensitive to instrumental range location? We present two experiments that investigate musicians’ ability to hear instrumental range location, specifically string register location on the violoncello. Experiment 1 is a behavioral study that tests whether musicians can reliably distinguish between higher and lower string register locations. In Experiment 2, we analyze acoustic features that could be impacted by string register location. Our results support the conjecture that musicians can reliably discriminate between string register locations, although perhaps only when vibrato is utilized. Our results also suggest that higher string register locations have a darker timbre and possibly a wider and faster vibrato. Further research on whether musicians can effectively imitate vocal range location signals with their instruments is warranted.
音域定位是一个重要的声乐情感信号。人类使用不同的音域来传达情感强度。因此,人类善于识别某人在其音域内的讲话位置。对音乐和情感的研究表明,音乐表达行为往往反映或从声音表达行为中获得灵感。音乐家有可能像人类使用音域相关信号一样,在乐器上使用音域信号吗?因此,音乐家可能对乐器音域的位置也同样敏感吗?我们提出了两个实验,研究音乐家听到乐器音域位置的能力,特别是中提琴上的弦乐音域位置。实验1是一项行为研究,测试音乐家是否能够可靠地区分较高和较低的字符串寄存器位置。在实验2中,我们分析了字符串寄存器位置可能影响的声学特征。我们的结果支持了这样一种推测,即音乐家可以可靠地区分字符串寄存器的位置,尽管可能只有当使用颤音时。我们的结果还表明,较高的弦寄存器位置具有较暗的音色,并且可能具有更宽更快的颤音。进一步研究音乐家是否可以用他们的乐器有效地模仿音域位置信号是有必要的。
{"title":"Musicians Can Reliably Discriminate Between String Register Locations on the Violoncello","authors":"C. Trevor, J. Devaney, David Huron","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.27","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.27","url":null,"abstract":"Vocal range location is an important vocal affective signal. Humans use different areas of their vocal range to communicate emotional intensity. Consequently, humans are good at identifying where someone is speaking within their vocal range. Research on music and emotion has demonstrated that musical expressive behaviors often reflect or take inspiration from vocal expressive behaviors. Is it possible for musicians to utilize range-related signals on their instrument similarly to how humans use vocal range-related signals? Might musicians therefore be similarly sensitive to instrumental range location? We present two experiments that investigate musicians’ ability to hear instrumental range location, specifically string register location on the violoncello. Experiment 1 is a behavioral study that tests whether musicians can reliably distinguish between higher and lower string register locations. In Experiment 2, we analyze acoustic features that could be impacted by string register location. Our results support the conjecture that musicians can reliably discriminate between string register locations, although perhaps only when vibrato is utilized. Our results also suggest that higher string register locations have a darker timbre and possibly a wider and faster vibrato. Further research on whether musicians can effectively imitate vocal range location signals with their instruments is warranted.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43958090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
You Can Tell a Prodigy From a Professional Musician 你可以分辨出一个天才和一个专业音乐家
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.39
Viola Pausch, Nina Düvel, R. Kopiez
According to Feldman (1993), musical prodigies are expected to perform at the same high level as professional adult musicians and, therefore, are indistinguishable from adults. This widespread definition was the basis for the study by Comeau et al. (2017), which investigated if participants could determine whether an audio sample was played by a professional pianist or a child prodigy. Our paper is a replication of this previous study under more controlled conditions. Our main findings partly confirmed the previous findings: Comparable to Comeau et al.’s (2017) study (N = 51), the participants in our study (N = 278) were able to discriminate between prodigies and adult professionals by listening to music recordings of the same pieces. The overall discrimination performance was slightly above chance (correct responses: 53.7%; sensitivity d’ = 0.20), which was similar to Comeau et al.’s (2017) results of the identification task with prodigies aged between 11 and 14 years (approximately 54.6% correct responses; sensitivity approximately d’ = 0.13). Contrary to the original study, musicians and pianists in our study did not perform significantly better than other participants. Nevertheless, it is generally possible for listeners to differentiate prodigies from adult performers—although this is a demanding task.
根据Feldman(1993)的观点,音乐神童被期望在与专业成年音乐家相同的高水平上表演,因此,与成年人无法区分。这一广泛的定义是Comeau等人(2017)研究的基础,该研究调查了参与者是否能够确定音频样本是由专业钢琴家还是神童演奏的。我们的论文是在更严格的控制条件下复制先前的研究。我们的主要发现部分证实了之前的发现:与Comeau等人(2017)的研究(N = 51)相比,我们研究的参与者(N = 278)能够通过听相同作品的音乐录音来区分天才和成年专业人士。总体判别表现略高于随机(正确率:53.7%;灵敏度d ' = 0.20),这与Comeau et al.(2017)对11 - 14岁神童识别任务的结果相似(约54.6%的正确率;灵敏度约为d ' = 0.13)。与最初的研究相反,在我们的研究中,音乐家和钢琴家的表现并没有明显好于其他参与者。尽管如此,一般来说,听众还是有可能将天才和成年表演者区分开来——尽管这是一项艰巨的任务。
{"title":"You Can Tell a Prodigy From a Professional Musician","authors":"Viola Pausch, Nina Düvel, R. Kopiez","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.39","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.39","url":null,"abstract":"According to Feldman (1993), musical prodigies are expected to perform at the same high level as professional adult musicians and, therefore, are indistinguishable from adults. This widespread definition was the basis for the study by Comeau et al. (2017), which investigated if participants could determine whether an audio sample was played by a professional pianist or a child prodigy. Our paper is a replication of this previous study under more controlled conditions. Our main findings partly confirmed the previous findings: Comparable to Comeau et al.’s (2017) study (N = 51), the participants in our study (N = 278) were able to discriminate between prodigies and adult professionals by listening to music recordings of the same pieces. The overall discrimination performance was slightly above chance (correct responses: 53.7%; sensitivity d’ = 0.20), which was similar to Comeau et al.’s (2017) results of the identification task with prodigies aged between 11 and 14 years (approximately 54.6% correct responses; sensitivity approximately d’ = 0.13). Contrary to the original study, musicians and pianists in our study did not perform significantly better than other participants. Nevertheless, it is generally possible for listeners to differentiate prodigies from adult performers—although this is a demanding task.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41591905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Music Empathizing and Music Systemizing are Associated with Music Listening Reward 音乐移情和音乐系统化与音乐听力奖励
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-09-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.3
G. Kreutz, Anja-Xiaoxing Cui
Music empathizing (ME) and music systemizing (MS) are constructs representing cognitive styles that address different facets of interest in music listening. Here we investigate whether ME and MS are positively associated with feelings of reward in response to music listening (MR). We conducted an online-survey in which n = 202 (127 identifying as female) participants, Mage = 26.06 years, SDage = 8.66 years, filled out the Music-Empathizing-Music-Systemizing (MEMS) Inventory, the Barcelona Questionnaire of Music Reward (BMRQ), further music-related inventories, and ad hoc items representing general interest and investment in music listening. Results from a conditional inference tree analysis confirm our hypothesis by showing ME followed by MS were the most important predictors of MR. In addition, subscription to music streaming services and investing free time into music listening were also associated with higher MR. These results suggest that perceiving reward through music listening is a function of both music empathizing and music systemizing. The nonsignificant contributions of music sophistication and music style preferences deny a larger role of these factors in MR. Further research is needed to investigate the interrelationships of musical cognitive styles and MR to refine our understanding of the affective value of music listening.
音乐移情(ME)和音乐系统化(MS)是代表认知风格的结构,它们涉及音乐听力兴趣的不同方面。在这里,我们调查了脑脊髓炎和多发性硬化症是否与听音乐(MR)的奖励感呈正相关。我们进行了一项在线调查,其中n=202(127名女性)参与者,Mage=26.06岁,SDage=8.66岁,填写了音乐移情音乐系统化(MEMS)清单、巴塞罗那音乐奖励问卷(BMRQ)、进一步的音乐相关清单,以及代表对音乐聆听的普遍兴趣和投资的特设项目。条件推理树分析的结果证实了我们的假设,表明ME和MS是MR的最重要预测因素。此外,订阅音乐流媒体服务和投入空闲时间听音乐也与较高的MR相关。这些结果表明,通过听音乐来感知奖励既是音乐移情的功能,也是音乐系统化的功能。音乐成熟度和音乐风格偏好的非显著贡献否认了这些因素在MR中的更大作用。需要进一步研究音乐认知风格和MR之间的相互关系,以完善我们对音乐听力情感价值的理解。
{"title":"Music Empathizing and Music Systemizing are Associated with Music Listening Reward","authors":"G. Kreutz, Anja-Xiaoxing Cui","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.40.1.3","url":null,"abstract":"Music empathizing (ME) and music systemizing (MS) are constructs representing cognitive styles that address different facets of interest in music listening. Here we investigate whether ME and MS are positively associated with feelings of reward in response to music listening (MR). We conducted an online-survey in which n = 202 (127 identifying as female) participants, Mage = 26.06 years, SDage = 8.66 years, filled out the Music-Empathizing-Music-Systemizing (MEMS) Inventory, the Barcelona Questionnaire of Music Reward (BMRQ), further music-related inventories, and ad hoc items representing general interest and investment in music listening. Results from a conditional inference tree analysis confirm our hypothesis by showing ME followed by MS were the most important predictors of MR. In addition, subscription to music streaming services and investing free time into music listening were also associated with higher MR. These results suggest that perceiving reward through music listening is a function of both music empathizing and music systemizing. The nonsignificant contributions of music sophistication and music style preferences deny a larger role of these factors in MR. Further research is needed to investigate the interrelationships of musical cognitive styles and MR to refine our understanding of the affective value of music listening.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43539198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived Motor Synchrony With the Beat is More Strongly Related to Groove Than Measured Synchrony 与测量的同步性相比,感知到的电机和节拍的同步性与凹槽的相关性更强
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.423
T. Matthews, Maria A. G. Witek, J. Thibodeau, P. Vuust, V. Penhune
The sensation of groove can be defined as the pleasurable urge to move to rhythmic music. When moving to the beat of a rhythm, both how well movements are synchronized to the beat, and the perceived difficulty in doing so, are associated with groove. Interestingly, when tapping to a rhythm, participants tend to overestimate their synchrony, suggesting a potential discrepancy between perceived and measured synchrony, which may impact their relative relation with groove. However, these relations, and the influence of syncopation and musicianship on these relations, have yet to be tested. Therefore, we asked participants to listen to 50 drum patterns with varying rhythmic complexity and rate their sensation of groove. They then tapped to the beat of the same drum patterns and rated how well they thought their taps synchronized with the beat. Perceived synchrony showed a stronger relation with groove ratings than measured synchrony and syncopation, and this effect was strongest for medium complexity rhythms. We interpret these results in the context of meter-based temporal predictions. We propose that the certainty of these predictions determine the weight and number of movements that are perceived as synchronous and thus reflect rewarding prediction confirmations.
凹槽的感觉可以定义为进入有节奏的音乐的愉悦冲动。当移动到节奏的节拍时,动作与节拍同步的程度以及感知到的难度都与凹槽有关。有趣的是,当敲击节奏时,参与者往往高估了他们的同步性,这表明感知到的和测量到的同步性之间存在潜在的差异,这可能会影响他们与凹槽的相对关系。然而,这些关系,以及切分音和音乐才能对这些关系的影响,还有待检验。因此,我们要求参与者听50种节奏复杂度不同的鼓型,并对他们的凹槽感进行评分。然后,他们敲击相同鼓型的节拍,并评估他们认为自己的敲击与节拍同步的程度。感知的同步性与凹槽评级的关系比测量的同步性和切分音更强,这种影响在中等复杂度的节奏中最强。我们在基于计量表的时间预测的背景下解释这些结果。我们提出,这些预测的确定性决定了被视为同步的运动的权重和数量,从而反映了有回报的预测确认。
{"title":"Perceived Motor Synchrony With the Beat is More Strongly Related to Groove Than Measured Synchrony","authors":"T. Matthews, Maria A. G. Witek, J. Thibodeau, P. Vuust, V. Penhune","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.423","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.423","url":null,"abstract":"The sensation of groove can be defined as the pleasurable urge to move to rhythmic music. When moving to the beat of a rhythm, both how well movements are synchronized to the beat, and the perceived difficulty in doing so, are associated with groove. Interestingly, when tapping to a rhythm, participants tend to overestimate their synchrony, suggesting a potential discrepancy between perceived and measured synchrony, which may impact their relative relation with groove. However, these relations, and the influence of syncopation and musicianship on these relations, have yet to be tested. Therefore, we asked participants to listen to 50 drum patterns with varying rhythmic complexity and rate their sensation of groove. They then tapped to the beat of the same drum patterns and rated how well they thought their taps synchronized with the beat. Perceived synchrony showed a stronger relation with groove ratings than measured synchrony and syncopation, and this effect was strongest for medium complexity rhythms. We interpret these results in the context of meter-based temporal predictions. We propose that the certainty of these predictions determine the weight and number of movements that are perceived as synchronous and thus reflect rewarding prediction confirmations.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45414521","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Investigating the Shared Meaning of Metaphorical Sound Attributes 隐喻性声音属性的共有意义研究
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.468
Victor Rosi, O. Houix, N. Misdariis, P. Susini
Music or sound professionals use specific terminology to communicate about timbre. Some key terms do not come from the sound domain and do not have a clear definition due to their metaphorical nature. This work aims to reveal shared meanings of four well-used timbre attributes: bright, warm, round, and rough. We conducted two complementary studies with French sound and music experts (e.g., composers, sound engineers, sound designers, musicians, etc.). First, we led interviews to gather definitions and instrumental sound examples for the four attributes (N = 32). Second, using an online survey, we tested the relevance and consensus on multiple descriptions most frequently evoked during the interviews (N = 51). The analysis of the rich corpus of verbalizations from the interviews yielded the main description strategies used by the experts, namely acoustic, metaphorical, and source-related. We also derived definitions for the attributes based on significantly relevant and consensual descriptions according to the survey results. Importantly, the definitions rely heavily on metaphorical descriptions. In sum, this study presents an overview of the shared meaning and perception of four metaphorical timbre attributes in the French language.
音乐或声音专业人士使用特定的术语来交流音色。一些关键术语并非来自声音领域,由于其隐喻性而没有明确的定义。这项工作旨在揭示四种常用的音色属性的共同含义:明亮,温暖,圆形和粗糙。我们与法国音响和音乐专家(如作曲家、音响工程师、音响设计师、音乐家等)进行了两项互补研究。首先,我们通过访谈收集了四个属性的定义和器乐声音示例(N = 32)。其次,使用在线调查,我们测试了在访谈中最常引起的多个描述的相关性和共识(N = 51)。通过对访谈中丰富的语言描述语料库的分析,得出了专家使用的主要描述策略,即声学、隐喻和源相关策略。根据调查结果,我们还根据显著相关和共识的描述推导了属性的定义。重要的是,这些定义严重依赖于隐喻性描述。总之,本研究概述了法语中四种隐喻音色属性的共同意义和感知。
{"title":"Investigating the Shared Meaning of Metaphorical Sound Attributes","authors":"Victor Rosi, O. Houix, N. Misdariis, P. Susini","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.468","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.468","url":null,"abstract":"Music or sound professionals use specific terminology to communicate about timbre. Some key terms do not come from the sound domain and do not have a clear definition due to their metaphorical nature. This work aims to reveal shared meanings of four well-used timbre attributes: bright, warm, round, and rough. We conducted two complementary studies with French sound and music experts (e.g., composers, sound engineers, sound designers, musicians, etc.). First, we led interviews to gather definitions and instrumental sound examples for the four attributes (N = 32). Second, using an online survey, we tested the relevance and consensus on multiple descriptions most frequently evoked during the interviews (N = 51). The analysis of the rich corpus of verbalizations from the interviews yielded the main description strategies used by the experts, namely acoustic, metaphorical, and source-related. We also derived definitions for the attributes based on significantly relevant and consensual descriptions according to the survey results. Importantly, the definitions rely heavily on metaphorical descriptions. In sum, this study presents an overview of the shared meaning and perception of four metaphorical timbre attributes in the French language.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47699724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Real-Time Modulation Perception in Western Classical Music 西方古典音乐中的实时调制感知
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.484
Brendon Mizener, W. Dowling
The task of music listening involves an auditory scene analysis in which the listener makes judgments related to melody, harmony, and consonance or dissonance, all of which are made within the context of key or tonic region. Here we examine whether the process of tracking key region is independent of the process of tracking surface cues, and what surface cues may influence that process. To this end, highly trained, moderately trained, and untrained listeners listened to excerpts from string quartets, quintets, and sextets from the classical and romantic eras and responded when they heard a modulation. Each excerpt featured either a pivot chord modulation, a direct modulation, a common tone modulation, or no modulation. Listeners performed above chance across modulation conditions, and an interaction effect was observed for modulation type and participant training level. We also present an exploratory PCA that suggests that harmonic language and phrasing are both significant factors in guiding modulation perception, both of which merit further investigation.
音乐聆听的任务涉及听觉场景分析,在听觉场景分析中,听众对旋律、和声、和声或不和谐做出判断,所有这些都是在关键或主音区域的背景下做出的。在这里,我们研究了跟踪关键区域的过程是否独立于跟踪表面线索的过程,以及哪些表面线索可能影响该过程。为此,受过高度训练、中等训练和未受过训练的听众收听古典和浪漫时代的弦乐四重奏、五重奏和六重奏的节选,并在听到调制时做出反应。每段选段都有一个枢轴和弦调制、一个直接调制、一种普通音调调制或没有调制。听众在不同的调制条件下表现出了以上的机会,并且观察到调制类型和参与者训练水平的交互效应。我们还提出了一个探索性的主成分分析,表明调和语言和短语都是引导调制感知的重要因素,这两个因素都值得进一步研究。
{"title":"Real-Time Modulation Perception in Western Classical Music","authors":"Brendon Mizener, W. Dowling","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.484","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.484","url":null,"abstract":"The task of music listening involves an auditory scene analysis in which the listener makes judgments related to melody, harmony, and consonance or dissonance, all of which are made within the context of key or tonic region. Here we examine whether the process of tracking key region is independent of the process of tracking surface cues, and what surface cues may influence that process. To this end, highly trained, moderately trained, and untrained listeners listened to excerpts from string quartets, quintets, and sextets from the classical and romantic eras and responded when they heard a modulation. Each excerpt featured either a pivot chord modulation, a direct modulation, a common tone modulation, or no modulation. Listeners performed above chance across modulation conditions, and an interaction effect was observed for modulation type and participant training level. We also present an exploratory PCA that suggests that harmonic language and phrasing are both significant factors in guiding modulation perception, both of which merit further investigation.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44445169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Syncopation and Groove in Polyphonic Music 复调音乐中的切分与槽
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-06-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.503
G. Sioros, G. Madison, Diogo Cocharro, A. Danielsen, F. Gouyon
Music often evokes a regular beat and a pleasurable sensation of wanting to move to that beat called groove. Recent studies show that a rhythmic pattern’s ability to evoke groove increases at moderate levels of syncopation, essentially, when some notes occur earlier than expected. We present two studies that investigate that effect of syncopation in more realistic polyphonic music examples. First, listeners rated their urge to move to music excerpts transcribed from funk and rock songs, and to algorithmically transformed versions of these excerpts: 1) with the original syncopation removed, and 2) with various levels of pseudorandom syncopation introduced. While the original excerpts were rated higher than the de-syncopated, the algorithmic syncopation was not as successful in evoking groove. Consequently, a moderate level of syncopation increases groove, but only for certain syncopation patterns. The second study provides detailed comparisons of the original and transformed rhythmic structures that revealed key differences between them in: 1) the distribution of syncopation across instruments and metrical positions, 2) the counter-meter figures formed by the syncopating notes, and 3) the number of pickup notes. On this basis, we form four concrete hypotheses about the function of syncopation in groove, to be tested in future experiments.
音乐常常唤起一种规律的节奏和一种愉悦的感觉,想要移动到被称为groove的节奏上。最近的研究表明,当一些音符出现得比预期的要早时,节奏模式在适度的切分音水平上唤起凹槽的能力会增强。我们提出了两项研究,调查切分的影响,在更现实的复调音乐的例子。首先,听众对他们想听放克和摇滚歌曲的冲动进行了评分,并对这些歌曲的算法转换版本进行了评分:1)删除了原始切分音,2)引入了不同程度的伪随机切分音。虽然原始节选的评分高于去切分,但算法切分在唤起凹槽方面并不成功。因此,适度的切分增加了凹槽,但仅适用于某些切分模式。第二项研究详细比较了原始和转换后的节奏结构,揭示了它们之间的主要差异:1)切分在乐器和格律位置上的分布,2)切分音符形成的反拍子,以及3)拾取音符的数量。在此基础上,我们对切分在沟槽中的作用形成了四个具体的假设,并将在未来的实验中进行验证。
{"title":"Syncopation and Groove in Polyphonic Music","authors":"G. Sioros, G. Madison, Diogo Cocharro, A. Danielsen, F. Gouyon","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.39.5.503","url":null,"abstract":"Music often evokes a regular beat and a pleasurable sensation of wanting to move to that beat called groove. Recent studies show that a rhythmic pattern’s ability to evoke groove increases at moderate levels of syncopation, essentially, when some notes occur earlier than expected. We present two studies that investigate that effect of syncopation in more realistic polyphonic music examples. First, listeners rated their urge to move to music excerpts transcribed from funk and rock songs, and to algorithmically transformed versions of these excerpts: 1) with the original syncopation removed, and 2) with various levels of pseudorandom syncopation introduced. While the original excerpts were rated higher than the de-syncopated, the algorithmic syncopation was not as successful in evoking groove. Consequently, a moderate level of syncopation increases groove, but only for certain syncopation patterns. The second study provides detailed comparisons of the original and transformed rhythmic structures that revealed key differences between them in: 1) the distribution of syncopation across instruments and metrical positions, 2) the counter-meter figures formed by the syncopating notes, and 3) the number of pickup notes. On this basis, we form four concrete hypotheses about the function of syncopation in groove, to be tested in future experiments.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":"211 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67421613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Measuring Children’s Harmonic Knowledge with Implicit and Explicit Tests 内隐和外显测试测量儿童和谐知识
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2022.39.4.361
Kathleen A. Corrigall, B. Tillmann, E. Schellenberg
We used implicit and explicit tasks to measure knowledge of Western harmony in musically trained and untrained Canadian children. Younger children were 6–7 years of age; older children were 10–11. On each trial, participants heard a sequence of five piano chords. The first four chords established a major-key context. The final chord was the standard, expected tonic of the context or one of two deviant endings: the highly unexpected flat supertonic or the moderately unexpected subdominant. In the implicit task, children identified the timbre of the final chord (guitar or piano) as quickly as possible. Response times were faster for the tonic ending than for either deviant ending, but the magnitude of the priming effect was similar for the two deviants, and the effect did not vary as a function of age or music training. In the explicit task, children rated how good each chord sequence sounded. Ratings were highest for sequences with the tonic ending, intermediate for the subdominant, and lowest for the flat supertonic. Moreover, the difference between the tonic and deviant sequences was larger for older children with music training. Thus, the explicit task provided a more nuanced picture of musical knowledge than did the implicit task.
我们使用内隐和外显任务来衡量受过音乐训练和未受过音乐训练的加拿大儿童对西方和谐的了解。年龄较小的儿童为6-7岁;年龄较大的孩子是10-11岁。在每次试验中,参与者都会听到一系列五个钢琴和弦。前四个和弦建立了一个主要的关键上下文。最后的和弦是上下文中标准的、预期的主音,或者是两个异常的结尾之一:高度出乎意料的平主音或适度出乎意料的次主音。在隐含任务中,孩子们尽快识别出最后一个和弦(吉他或钢琴)的音色。主音结尾的反应时间比任何一个异常结尾都快,但两个异常结尾的启动效应的大小相似,并且这种效应没有随着年龄或音乐训练的变化而变化。在明确的任务中,孩子们评定每个和弦序列听起来有多好。主音结尾的序列评分最高,次主音为中等,平主音为最低。此外,对于接受音乐训练的年龄较大的儿童来说,主音序列和异常序列之间的差异更大。因此,显性任务比隐性任务提供了更细致的音乐知识。
{"title":"Measuring Children’s Harmonic Knowledge with Implicit and Explicit Tests","authors":"Kathleen A. Corrigall, B. Tillmann, E. Schellenberg","doi":"10.1525/mp.2022.39.4.361","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2022.39.4.361","url":null,"abstract":"We used implicit and explicit tasks to measure knowledge of Western harmony in musically trained and untrained Canadian children. Younger children were 6–7 years of age; older children were 10–11. On each trial, participants heard a sequence of five piano chords. The first four chords established a major-key context. The final chord was the standard, expected tonic of the context or one of two deviant endings: the highly unexpected flat supertonic or the moderately unexpected subdominant. In the implicit task, children identified the timbre of the final chord (guitar or piano) as quickly as possible. Response times were faster for the tonic ending than for either deviant ending, but the magnitude of the priming effect was similar for the two deviants, and the effect did not vary as a function of age or music training. In the explicit task, children rated how good each chord sequence sounded. Ratings were highest for sequences with the tonic ending, intermediate for the subdominant, and lowest for the flat supertonic. Moreover, the difference between the tonic and deviant sequences was larger for older children with music training. Thus, the explicit task provided a more nuanced picture of musical knowledge than did the implicit task.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41452186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Music Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1