首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Child Language最新文献

英文 中文
Different paths to multilingualism in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): Naturalistic and non-interactive 自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)中多种语言的不同途径:自然的和非互动的
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-20 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000540
Iris Hindi, Natalia Meir

This study is one of the few research efforts investigating unexpected non-interactive foreign language acquisition in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Participants included 46 English-Hebrew-speaking children (ages 4;10 to 12;0): 14 autistic children who acquired English via non-interactive input (ASD-NI); 12 autistic children (ASD-Nat), and 20 non-autistic children with typical language development (TLD-Nat) who acquired English and Hebrew naturalistically. Morpho-syntactic abilities were assessed using Sentence Repetition tasks in both languages. The results showed no group differences for morpho-syntax in English; in Hebrew, the ASD-NI group scored similarly to the ASD-Nat group but lower than the TLD-Nat group. Individual performance differences between Hebrew and English were observed across all groups. Additionally, correlations between exposure and SRep scores were found in both groups for Hebrew but not English. These findings highlight diverse paths to language acquisition in ASD, with children acquiring foreign languages via both naturalistic and non-interactive input.

本研究是少数研究自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)儿童意外非互动外语习得的研究之一。参与者包括46名以英语-希伯来语为母语的儿童(4岁;10岁至12岁;0岁);14名通过非互动输入(ASD-NI)获得英语的自闭症儿童;12名自闭症儿童(ASD-Nat)和20名非自闭症典型语言发展儿童(TLD-Nat)自然习得英语和希伯来语。在两种语言中使用句子重复任务评估词法句法能力。结果显示,英语语素-句法组间无差异;在希伯来语中,ASD-NI组的得分与ASD-Nat组相似,但低于TLD-Nat组。所有小组都观察到希伯来语和英语之间的个人表现差异。此外,在希伯来语和英语两组中,暴露程度和SRep评分之间存在相关性,但没有发现相关性。这些发现强调了自闭症患者语言习得的不同途径,儿童通过自然输入和非互动输入习得外语。
{"title":"Different paths to multilingualism in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): Naturalistic and non-interactive","authors":"Iris Hindi, Natalia Meir","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000540","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000540","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study is one of the few research efforts investigating unexpected non-interactive foreign language acquisition in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Participants included 46 English-Hebrew-speaking children (ages 4;10 to 12;0): 14 autistic children who acquired English via non-interactive input (ASD-NI); 12 autistic children (ASD-Nat), and 20 non-autistic children with typical language development (TLD-Nat) who acquired English and Hebrew naturalistically. Morpho-syntactic abilities were assessed using Sentence Repetition tasks in both languages. The results showed no group differences for morpho-syntax in English; in Hebrew, the ASD-NI group scored similarly to the ASD-Nat group but lower than the TLD-Nat group. Individual performance differences between Hebrew and English were observed across all groups. Additionally, correlations between exposure and SRep scores were found in both groups for Hebrew but not English. These findings highlight diverse paths to language acquisition in ASD, with children acquiring foreign languages via both naturalistic and non-interactive input.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"74 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142989858","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bilingual children reach early language milestones at the same age as monolingual peers 双语儿童与单语儿童在相同的年龄达到早期语言里程碑
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-15 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000655
Karolina Muszyńska, Grzegorz Krajewski, Agnieszka Dynak, Nina Gram Garmann, Anna Sara H. Romøren, Magdalena Łuniewska, Katie Alcock, Napoleon Katsos, Joanna Kołak, Hanne Gram Simonsen, Pernille Hansen, Magdalena Krysztofiak, Krzysztof Sobota, Ewa Haman

In this longitudinal study, we compare the age of reaching early developmental milestones in bilingual and monolingual children and between the bilinguals’ two languages. We present data from 302 Polish bilinguals (living outside of Poland with various majority languages) and 302 Polish monolinguals, aged M = 12.78 months on study entry (range: 024 months), matched on sex, age at study entry, duration of parental reporting, and parental education. The milestones under investigation include crawling, walking, babbling, first, 10th, 50th word, and first multi-word utterances. The data was collected with a specially designed mobile app, in which parents reported their children’s development repeatedly. Using this relatively big sample and looking at a wide range of investigated milestones, we present evidence that typical bilingual development follows a trajectory similar to monolingual development. We also evaluate the feasibility and usefulness of online data collection using mobile apps to study early language development.

在这项纵向研究中,我们比较了双语儿童和单语儿童以及双语儿童两种语言之间达到早期发展里程碑的年龄。我们提供了302名波兰双语者(居住在波兰境外,使用各种多数语言)和302名波兰单语者的数据,他们在研究开始时年龄M = 12.78个月(范围:0-24个月),在性别、研究开始时的年龄、父母报告的持续时间和父母的教育程度上相匹配。研究中的里程碑包括爬行、行走、咿呀学语、第一个、第10个、第50个单词和第一个多单词的话语。这些数据是通过一个专门设计的移动应用程序收集的,在这个应用程序中,父母反复报告他们孩子的发展情况。使用这个相对较大的样本,并观察广泛的调查里程碑,我们提出的证据表明,典型的双语发展遵循类似于单语发展的轨迹。我们还评估了使用移动应用程序在线数据收集来研究早期语言发展的可行性和实用性。
{"title":"Bilingual children reach early language milestones at the same age as monolingual peers","authors":"Karolina Muszyńska, Grzegorz Krajewski, Agnieszka Dynak, Nina Gram Garmann, Anna Sara H. Romøren, Magdalena Łuniewska, Katie Alcock, Napoleon Katsos, Joanna Kołak, Hanne Gram Simonsen, Pernille Hansen, Magdalena Krysztofiak, Krzysztof Sobota, Ewa Haman","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000655","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000655","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this longitudinal study, we compare the age of reaching early developmental milestones in bilingual and monolingual children and between the bilinguals’ two languages. We present data from 302 Polish bilinguals (living outside of Poland with various majority languages) and 302 Polish monolinguals, aged <span>M</span> = 12.78 months on study entry (range: 0<span>–</span>24 months), matched on sex, age at study entry, duration of parental reporting, and parental education. The milestones under investigation include crawling, walking, babbling, first, 10th, 50th word, and first multi-word utterances. The data was collected with a specially designed mobile app, in which parents reported their children’s development repeatedly. Using this relatively big sample and looking at a wide range of investigated milestones, we present evidence that typical bilingual development follows a trajectory similar to monolingual development. We also evaluate the feasibility and usefulness of online data collection using mobile apps to study early language development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"92 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142981949","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prosodic variation between contexts in infant-directed speech 婴儿指向语中不同语境的韵律变化
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000709
Jenna DiStefano, Michelle Cohn, Georgia Zellou, Katharine Graf Estes

Speakers consider their listeners and adjust the way they communicate. One well-studied example is the register of infant-directed speech (IDS), which differs acoustically from speech directed to adults. However, little work has explored how parents adjust speech to infants across different contexts. This is important because infants and parents engage in many activities throughout each day. The current study tests whether the properties of IDS in English vary across three in-lab tasks (sorting objects, free play, and storytelling). We analysed acoustic features associated with prosody, including mean fundamental frequency (F0, perceived as pitch), F0 range, and word rate. We found that both parents’ pitch ranges and word rates varied depending on the task in IDS. The storytelling task stood out among the tasks for having a wider pitch range and faster word rate. The results depict how context can drive parents’ speech adjustments to infants.

说话者会考虑他们的听众,并调整他们的沟通方式。一个被充分研究的例子是婴儿指向语(IDS)的语域,它在声学上不同于指向成人的言语。然而,很少有研究探讨父母如何在不同的环境中调整婴儿的语言。这一点很重要,因为婴儿和父母每天都要进行许多活动。目前的研究测试了英语中IDS的特性是否在三个实验室任务(分类对象、自由游戏和讲故事)中有所不同。我们分析了与韵律相关的声学特征,包括平均基频(F0,被认为是音高)、F0范围和词频。我们发现父母双方的音高范围和词频根据IDS的任务而变化。讲故事的任务在所有任务中脱颖而出,因为它的音调范围更广,语速更快。研究结果描述了环境如何推动父母对婴儿的语言调整。
{"title":"Prosodic variation between contexts in infant-directed speech","authors":"Jenna DiStefano, Michelle Cohn, Georgia Zellou, Katharine Graf Estes","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000709","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000709","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Speakers consider their listeners and adjust the way they communicate. One well-studied example is the register of infant-directed speech (IDS), which differs acoustically from speech directed to adults. However, little work has explored how parents adjust speech to infants across different contexts. This is important because infants and parents engage in many activities throughout each day. The current study tests whether the properties of IDS in English vary across three in-lab tasks (sorting objects, free play, and storytelling). We analysed acoustic features associated with prosody, including mean fundamental frequency (F0, perceived as pitch), F0 range, and word rate. We found that both parents’ pitch ranges and word rates varied depending on the task in IDS. The storytelling task stood out among the tasks for having a wider pitch range and faster word rate. The results depict how context can drive parents’ speech adjustments to infants.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"29 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142968117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between working memory, production, and comprehension: evidence from children’s errors in complex wh questions
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000564
C. Jane Lutken, Geraldine Legendre

English-speaking children sometimes make errors in production and comprehension of biclausal questions, known as “Scope-Marking Errors”. In production, these errors surface as medial wh questions (e.g., What do you think who the cat chased? (Thornton, 1990)). In comprehension, children respond to questions like How did the boy say what he caught? by answering what was caught (de Villiers & Roeper, 1995). These errors resemble wh-scope marking questions, attested in languages like German. Together, these errors suggest temporary adoption of multiple UG-licensed grammars (e.g., Yang, 2002). However, Lutken et al. (2020) found that children who make these errors in production do not necessarily make errors in comprehension and vice versa. They suggest these errors stem from children’s immature processing mechanisms. This article examines children’s production, comprehension, and processing capabilities, specifically working memory (WM). We find a correlation between WM and error rate and suggest separate causes for production and comprehension errors.

以英语为母语的儿童有时会在双关语问题的产生和理解上犯错误,这被称为“范围标记错误”。在生产中,这些错误会以中间问题的形式出现(例如,你认为猫追的是谁?(桑顿,1990))。在理解能力方面,孩子们会回答这样的问题:这个男孩是怎么说他抓到的东西的?通过回答所捕捉到的东西(德维利尔&;鲁普,1995)。这些错误类似于在德语等语言中出现的wh范围标记问题。总之,这些错误表明暂时采用了多个ug许可的语法(例如,Yang, 2002)。然而,Lutken et al.(2020)发现,在生产中犯这些错误的儿童并不一定在理解上犯错误,反之亦然。他们认为这些错误源于儿童不成熟的处理机制。本文考察了儿童的生产、理解和加工能力,特别是工作记忆(WM)。我们发现了WM和错误率之间的相关性,并提出了产生和理解错误的不同原因。
{"title":"The relationship between working memory, production, and comprehension: evidence from children’s errors in complex wh questions","authors":"C. Jane Lutken, Geraldine Legendre","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000564","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000564","url":null,"abstract":"<p>English-speaking children sometimes make errors in production and comprehension of biclausal questions, known as “Scope-Marking Errors”. In production, these errors surface as medial wh questions (e.g., What do you think who the cat chased? (Thornton, 1990)). In comprehension, children respond to questions like How did the boy say what he caught? by answering what was caught (de Villiers &amp; Roeper, 1995). These errors resemble wh-scope marking questions, attested in languages like German. Together, these errors suggest temporary adoption of multiple UG-licensed grammars (e.g., Yang, 2002). However, Lutken et al. (2020) found that children who make these errors in production do not necessarily make errors in comprehension and vice versa. They suggest these errors stem from children’s immature processing mechanisms. This article examines children’s production, comprehension, and processing capabilities, specifically working memory (WM). We find a correlation between WM and error rate and suggest separate causes for production and comprehension errors.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"128 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142968118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Null subject comprehension and production revisited: a look at English and Italian 重新审视空主语理解和生产:看一下英语和意大利语
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000588
Juliana Gerard, Muskaan Singh, Giulia Bencini, Virginia Valian

This study will investigate how children acquire the option to drop the subject of a sentence, or null subjects (e.g., “Tickles me” instead of “He tickles me”). In languages that do not permit null subjects, children produce sentences with null subjects from 1 to 3 years of age. This non-adultlike production has been explained by two main accounts: first, the null subject sentences may accurately reflect the children’s linguistic knowledge, that is, a competence account. Alternatively, they may result from immature processing resources, therefore underestimating children’s competence, that is, a performance account. We will test the predictions of these accounts by using a central fixation preference procedure and elicited imitation to measure children’s comprehension and production, respectively, in monolingual 19- to 28-month-olds acquiring English (a non-null subject language) and Italian (a null subject language). The results will shed light on acquisition across languages, and the features that provide evidence to a learner.

这项研究将调查儿童是如何获得放弃句子主语或不使用主语的选择的(例如,“逗我痒”而不是“他逗我痒”)。在不允许空主语的语言中,儿童从1岁到3岁就会造出空主语的句子。这种非成人的产生主要有两个解释:第一,空主语句可以准确地反映儿童的语言知识,即能力说。另一种可能是由于加工资源不成熟,因此低估了儿童的能力,即绩效账户。我们将通过使用中心固定偏好程序和诱导模仿来测试这些说法的预测,分别在19至28个月的单语儿童中学习英语(非无效主体语言)和意大利语(无效主体语言),以衡量儿童的理解和生产。研究结果将揭示跨语言习得,以及为学习者提供证据的特征。
{"title":"Null subject comprehension and production revisited: a look at English and Italian","authors":"Juliana Gerard, Muskaan Singh, Giulia Bencini, Virginia Valian","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000588","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000588","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study will investigate how children acquire the option to drop the subject of a sentence, or null subjects (e.g., “Tickles me” instead of “He tickles me”). In languages that do not permit null subjects, children produce sentences with null subjects from 1 to 3 years of age. This non-adultlike production has been explained by two main accounts: first, the null subject sentences may accurately reflect the children’s linguistic knowledge, that is, a competence account. Alternatively, they may result from immature processing resources, therefore underestimating children’s competence, that is, a performance account. We will test the predictions of these accounts by using a central fixation preference procedure and elicited imitation to measure children’s comprehension and production, respectively, in monolingual 19- to 28-month-olds acquiring English (a non-null subject language) and Italian (a null subject language). The results will shed light on acquisition across languages, and the features that provide evidence to a learner.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"36 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142968116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Evaluation of LENA Start™ Using Measures Derived from Parent–Child Interactions 利用亲子互动衍生的测量方法评估LENA Start™
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000606
Kirstin Kuchler, Marianne Elmquist, Scott R. McConnell, Lizbeth H. Finestack

It is important to assess learning in both familiar and unfamiliar conditions to determine the extent of learning generalisation. In this study, we evaluated parent language outcomes of LENA Start™, a parent-implemented intervention, using distal measures derived from a parent–child free play interaction.

Forty-four parents and their child (mean: 20.8 months) participated in LENA Start™ or early childhood education curriculum intervention, in either English (n = 34) or Spanish (n = 10). We completed between- and within-group analyses using language and communication behaviour measures derived from parent–child interactions elicited outside the home with researcher-supplied materials (i.e., distal measures).

Group comparisons revealed significant differences on a subset of behavioural measures. Spanish-speaking parents in the LENA Start™ group demonstrated significant gains on three measures. While LENA Start™ has been associated with parent gains on proximal language measures, the results of this study reveal limited generalisation of skills based on distal measures.

评估在熟悉和不熟悉条件下的学习,以确定学习泛化的程度是很重要的。在这项研究中,我们评估了父母实施的干预措施LENA Start™的父母语言结果,采用了来自亲子自由游戏互动的远端测量。44位家长和他们的孩子(平均:20.8个月)参加了LENA Start™或早期儿童教育课程干预,英语(n = 34)或西班牙语(n = 10)。我们使用研究人员提供的材料(即远端测量),使用语言和交流行为测量方法,完成了组内和组间分析。这些测量方法来自于父母与孩子在家庭外的互动。组间比较显示,在行为测量的一个子集上存在显著差异。在LENA Start™组中,讲西班牙语的父母在三个指标上表现出显著的进步。虽然LENA Start™与父母在近端语言测量方面的收益有关,但本研究的结果显示,基于远端测量的技能泛化有限。
{"title":"An Evaluation of LENA Start™ Using Measures Derived from Parent–Child Interactions","authors":"Kirstin Kuchler, Marianne Elmquist, Scott R. McConnell, Lizbeth H. Finestack","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000606","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000606","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It is important to assess learning in both familiar and unfamiliar conditions to determine the extent of learning generalisation. In this study, we evaluated parent language outcomes of LENA Start™, a parent-implemented intervention, using distal measures derived from a parent–child free play interaction.</p><p>Forty-four parents and their child (mean: 20.8 months) participated in LENA Start™ or early childhood education curriculum intervention, in either English (n = 34) or Spanish (n = 10). We completed between- and within-group analyses using language and communication behaviour measures derived from parent–child interactions elicited outside the home with researcher-supplied materials (i.e., distal measures).</p><p>Group comparisons revealed significant differences on a subset of behavioural measures. Spanish-speaking parents in the LENA Start™ group demonstrated significant gains on three measures. While LENA Start™ has been associated with parent gains on proximal language measures, the results of this study reveal limited generalisation of skills based on distal measures.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"88 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142968114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The dynamics of initiation in caregiver–child conversational interactions 照料者与儿童对话互动中起始的动态
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/s030500092400062x
Jun Ho Chai, Jongmin Jung, Eon-Suk Ko

We investigated the dynamics of communicative initiation in infant−caregiver interactions across ages and language abilities. Analyses of 228 Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) recordings from 141 Korean adult−child dyads (60 girls; aged 7−30 months) replicated the initiator effect reported in North American populations. This effect, demonstrated by longer utterances, more frequent speech, and shorter response times in self-initiated interactions for both children and adults, suggests potential cross-cultural consistency in this conversational dynamic and remained consistent across ages in most conversational measures. A focused analysis of 13–14 month-olds (N = 40) and their K-CDI scores revealed that the initiator effect in segment duration and number persisted across most vocabulary percentiles. Additionally, nuanced findings indicated that caregivers increased their input frequency and adjusted segment duration in adult-initiated conversations in tandem with children’s higher receptive abilities. The robustness of the initiator role across cultures, ages, and vocabulary abilities points to a fundamental aspect of human communication.

我们研究了不同年龄和语言能力的婴儿-照顾者互动中交际开始的动态。141对韩国成人-儿童(60名女孩)228份语言环境分析(LENA)记录分析年龄为7 - 30个月),在北美人群中重复了报道的启动器效应。这种影响表现在儿童和成人在自我发起的互动中说话时间更长、说话频率更高、反应时间更短,这表明这种会话动态具有潜在的跨文化一致性,并且在大多数会话测量中在不同年龄保持一致。一项针对13-14个月大婴儿(N = 40)及其K-CDI分数的重点分析显示,在大多数词汇百分位数中,片段持续时间和数量的启动效应持续存在。此外,细微的发现表明,在成人发起的对话中,照顾者增加了他们的输入频率和调整的片段持续时间,与儿童更高的接受能力相一致。跨文化、年龄和词汇能力的发起者角色的稳健性指出了人类交流的一个基本方面。
{"title":"The dynamics of initiation in caregiver–child conversational interactions","authors":"Jun Ho Chai, Jongmin Jung, Eon-Suk Ko","doi":"10.1017/s030500092400062x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s030500092400062x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We investigated the dynamics of communicative initiation in infant−caregiver interactions across ages and language abilities. Analyses of 228 Language ENvironment Analysis (LENA) recordings from 141 Korean adult−child dyads (60 girls; aged 7−30 months) replicated the initiator effect reported in North American populations. This effect, demonstrated by longer utterances, more frequent speech, and shorter response times in self-initiated interactions for both children and adults, suggests potential cross-cultural consistency in this conversational dynamic and remained consistent across ages in most conversational measures. A focused analysis of 13–14 month-olds (N = 40) and their K-CDI scores revealed that the initiator effect in segment duration and number persisted across most vocabulary percentiles. Additionally, nuanced findings indicated that caregivers increased their input frequency and adjusted segment duration in adult-initiated conversations in tandem with children’s higher receptive abilities. The robustness of the initiator role across cultures, ages, and vocabulary abilities points to a fundamental aspect of human communication.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142968115","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How strong is the relationship between caregiver speech and language development? A meta-analysis 照顾者言语与语言发展之间的关系有多紧密?一个荟萃分析
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-10 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000692
Joseph R. Coffey, Jesse Snedeker

A growing body of research has found that talking to young children is positively associated with language outcomes. However, there is tremendous heterogeneity in the design of these studies, which could potentially affect the strength and reliability of this association. The present meta-analysis, comprising 4760 participants across 71 studies, goes beyond prior research by including: 1) more recent studies, 2) non-English-speaking populations, 3) more fine-grained categorization of measures of input, 4) additional moderators, and 5) a multilevel model design allowing us to consider multiple effect sizes per study. We find a moderate association between input and outcomes (R2=0.04-0.07) across four input measures, with some evidence of publication bias. We find no differences in effect size across any of the input measures. Child age and study duration moderated some effects of input. Our findings suggest that language input-outcome associations remain robust but modest across a multitude of contexts and measures.

越来越多的研究发现,与幼儿交谈与语言成绩呈正相关。然而,这些研究的设计存在巨大的异质性,这可能会影响这种关联的强度和可靠性。目前的荟萃分析,包括71项研究的4760名参与者,超越了之前的研究,包括:1)最近的研究,2)非英语人群,3)更细粒度的输入测量分类,4)额外的调节因子,5)多层次模型设计允许我们考虑每个研究的多个效应大小。我们发现在四个输入测量中,输入与结果之间存在适度的关联(R2=0.04-0.07),并有一些证据表明存在发表偏倚。我们发现在任何输入度量中效应大小没有差异。儿童年龄和学习时间对输入有一定的调节作用。我们的研究结果表明,语言输入-结果的关联在许多情境和测量中仍然是稳健但适度的。
{"title":"How strong is the relationship between caregiver speech and language development? A meta-analysis","authors":"Joseph R. Coffey, Jesse Snedeker","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000692","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000692","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A growing body of research has found that talking to young children is positively associated with language outcomes. However, there is tremendous heterogeneity in the design of these studies, which could potentially affect the strength and reliability of this association. The present meta-analysis, comprising 4760 participants across 71 studies, goes beyond prior research by including: 1) more recent studies, 2) non-English-speaking populations, 3) more fine-grained categorization of measures of input, 4) additional moderators, and 5) a multilevel model design allowing us to consider multiple effect sizes per study. We find a moderate association between input and outcomes (R<span>2</span>=0.04-0.07) across four input measures, with some evidence of publication bias. We find no differences in effect size across any of the input measures. Child age and study duration moderated some effects of input. Our findings suggest that language input-outcome associations remain robust but modest across a multitude of contexts and measures.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"24 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142939791","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Motherese Directed at Prelinguistic Infants at Risk for Neurological Disorders: An Exploratory Study 针对具有神经障碍风险的语言前婴儿的母亲:一项探索性研究
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-10 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000217
Okko Räsänen, Manu Airaksinen, Viviana Marchi, Olena Chorna, Andrea Guzzetta, Fabrizia Festante

To investigate how a high risk for infant neurological impairment affects the quality of infant verbal interactions, and in particular properties of infant-directed speech, spontaneous interactions between 14 mothers and their 4.5-month-old infants at high risk for neurological disorders (7 female) were recorded and acoustically compared with those of 14 dyads with typically developing infants (8 female). Mothers of at-risk infants had proportionally less voicing, and the proportion of voicing decreased with increasing severity of the infants’ long-term outcome. Follow-up analysis based on manual annotation of phonation style revealed breathy phonation as more common toward infants with more severe long-term outcomes (N=7; 44.7% of speech) than controls (N=14; 22.0%; p=0.005) or at-risk infants with typical or mildly abnormal long-term outcomes (N=7; 16.5%; p=0.002). The results indicate that maternal phonation style during early dyadic interactions is affected by the infant’s neurological condition.

为了研究婴儿神经损伤的高风险如何影响婴儿语言互动的质量,特别是婴儿定向语言的特性,我们记录了14位患有神经疾病高风险的母亲与其4.5个月大的婴儿(7位女性)之间的自发互动,并与14位发育正常的婴儿(8位女性)进行了声学比较。高危婴儿的母亲发声比例较低,且发声比例随婴儿长期预后严重程度的增加而降低。基于手工标注发声方式的随访分析显示,呼吸式发声在长期预后更严重的婴儿中更为常见(N=7;44.7%的语音)比对照组(N=14;22.0%;p=0.005)或有典型或轻度异常长期结局的高危婴儿(N=7;16.5%;p = 0.002)。结果表明,母亲在早期二元互动中的发声方式受到婴儿神经系统状况的影响。
{"title":"Motherese Directed at Prelinguistic Infants at Risk for Neurological Disorders: An Exploratory Study","authors":"Okko Räsänen, Manu Airaksinen, Viviana Marchi, Olena Chorna, Andrea Guzzetta, Fabrizia Festante","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000217","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000217","url":null,"abstract":"<p>To investigate how a high risk for infant neurological impairment affects the quality of infant verbal interactions, and in particular properties of infant-directed speech, spontaneous interactions between 14 mothers and their 4.5-month-old infants at high risk for neurological disorders (7 female) were recorded and acoustically compared with those of 14 dyads with typically developing infants (8 female). Mothers of at-risk infants had proportionally less voicing, and the proportion of voicing decreased with increasing severity of the infants’ long-term outcome. Follow-up analysis based on manual annotation of phonation style revealed breathy phonation as more common toward infants with more severe long-term outcomes (N=7; 44.7% of speech) than controls (N=14; 22.0%; <span>p</span>=0.005) or at-risk infants with typical or mildly abnormal long-term outcomes (N=7; 16.5%; <span>p</span>=0.002). The results indicate that maternal phonation style during early dyadic interactions is affected by the infant’s neurological condition.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"35 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142939792","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of age of arrival and language environment factors in Arabic heritage language development: A longitudinal study 到达年龄和语言环境因素在阿拉伯语遗产语言发展中的作用:一项纵向研究
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2025-01-10 DOI: 10.1017/s0305000924000679
Johanne Paradis, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Evangelia Daskalaki, Redab Al Janaideh, Xi Chen, Alexandra Gottardo

The Arabic development of Syrian refugee children (N = 133; mean age = 9;4 at Time 1) was examined over 3 time periods during their first five years in Canada. Children were administered sentence repetition and receptive vocabulary tasks in English and Arabic, and information about age-of-arrival (AOA), schooling in Arabic and language environment factors was obtained via parent report. Older AOA was associated with superior Arabic abilities across time, but regardless of AOA, children showed plateau/attrition patterns in Arabic and shifts to English dominance by Time 3. Increases in English over Arabic were observed for language use at home and language-rich activities overtime. Stronger Arabic Time 3 outcomes were predicted by more Arabic and less English use with siblings, more schooling in Arabic, more frequent listening-speaking and extra-curricular activities in Arabic, and more Arabic use with friends. We conclude that the heritage language can be vulnerable even for first-generation bilinguals.

叙利亚难民儿童的阿拉伯语发展(N = 133;平均年龄为9岁,时间1为4岁),在加拿大的前5年接受了3个时间段的检查。对儿童进行英语和阿拉伯语句子重复和接受性词汇任务,并通过家长报告获得儿童的入学年龄、阿拉伯语学校教育和语言环境因素的信息。随着时间的推移,年龄较大的AOA与较强的阿拉伯语能力有关,但无论AOA如何,儿童在阿拉伯语方面表现出平稳/磨损模式,并在时间3时转向英语优势。在家庭语言使用和语言丰富的活动中,英语比阿拉伯语的使用人数有所增加。与兄弟姐妹更多地使用阿拉伯语而较少使用英语,更多地使用阿拉伯语上学,更频繁地使用阿拉伯语听说和课外活动,以及与朋友更多地使用阿拉伯语,这些都预示着更强的阿拉伯时间3的结果。我们的结论是,即使对第一代双语者来说,传统语言也可能是脆弱的。
{"title":"The role of age of arrival and language environment factors in Arabic heritage language development: A longitudinal study","authors":"Johanne Paradis, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Evangelia Daskalaki, Redab Al Janaideh, Xi Chen, Alexandra Gottardo","doi":"10.1017/s0305000924000679","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000924000679","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Arabic development of Syrian refugee children (<span>N</span> = 133; mean age = 9;4 at Time 1) was examined over 3 time periods during their first five years in Canada. Children were administered sentence repetition and receptive vocabulary tasks in English and Arabic, and information about age-of-arrival (AOA), schooling in Arabic and language environment factors was obtained via parent report. Older AOA was associated with superior Arabic abilities across time, but regardless of AOA, children showed plateau/attrition patterns in Arabic and shifts to English dominance by Time 3. Increases in English over Arabic were observed for language use at home and language-rich activities overtime. Stronger Arabic Time 3 outcomes were predicted by more Arabic and less English use with siblings, more schooling in Arabic, more frequent listening-speaking and extra-curricular activities in Arabic, and more Arabic use with friends. We conclude that the heritage language can be vulnerable even for first-generation bilinguals.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":"2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142939854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Child Language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1