首页 > 最新文献

Infant Behavior & Development最新文献

英文 中文
The breadth and specificity of 18-month-old’s infant-initiated interactions in naturalistic home settings 自然家庭环境中 18 个月大婴儿主动互动的广度和特异性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101927
Didar Karadağ , Marina Bazhydai , Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar , Hilal H. Şen

Infants actively initiate social interactions aiming to elicit different types of responses from other people. This study aimed to document a variety of communicative interactions initiated by 18-month-old Turkish infants from diverse SES (N = 43) with their caregivers in their natural home settings. The infant-initiated interactions such as use of deictic gestures (e.g., pointing, holdouts), action demonstrations, vocalizations, and non-specific play actions were coded from video recordings and classified into two categories as need-based and non-need-based. Need-based interactions were further classified as a) biological (e.g., feeding); b) socio-emotional (e.g., cuddling), and non-need-based interactions (i.e., communicative intentions) were coded as a) expressive, b) requestive; c) information/help-seeking; d) information-giving. Infant-initiated non-need-based (88%) interactions were more prevalent compared to need-based interactions (12%). Among the non-need-based interactions, 50% aimed at expressing or sharing attention or emotion, 26% aimed at requesting an object or an action, and 12% aimed at seeking information or help. Infant-initiated information-giving events were rare. We further investigated the effects of familial SES and infant sex, finding no effect of either on the number of infant-initiated interactions. These findings suggest that at 18 months, infants actively communicate with their social partners to fulfil their need-based and non-need-based motivations using a wide range of verbal and nonverbal behaviors, regardless of their sex and socio-economic background. This study thoroughly characterizes a wide and detailed range of infant-initiated spontaneous communicative bids in hard-to-access contexts (infants’ daily lives at home) and with a traditionally underrepresented non-WEIRD population.

婴儿会主动发起社交互动,旨在从其他人那里获得不同类型的回应。本研究旨在记录来自不同社会经济地位的 18 个月大土耳其婴儿(43 人)在自然家庭环境中与照料者进行的各种交流互动。根据录像对婴儿主动进行的互动进行了编码,并将其分为两类,即基于需要的互动和非基于需要的互动。基于需要的互动又分为 a) 生物性互动(如喂食);b) 社会情感性互动(如拥抱);而非基于需要的互动(即交流意图)则分为 a) 表达性互动;b) 请求性互动;c) 寻求信息/帮助性互动;d) 给予信息性互动。与基于需求的互动(12%)相比,由婴儿发起的非基于需求的互动(88%)更为普遍。在非基于需要的互动中,50%旨在表达或分享关注或情感,26%旨在要求物品或行动,12%旨在寻求信息或帮助。婴儿主动提供信息的情况很少见。我们进一步调查了家庭经济状况和婴儿性别的影响,结果发现两者对婴儿主动互动的数量都没有影响。这些研究结果表明,18 个月大的婴儿会主动与他们的社会伙伴交流,通过各种语言和非语言行为来满足他们的需求和非需求动机,而与他们的性别和社会经济背景无关。这项研究全面描述了在难以接近的环境(婴儿在家中的日常生活)中,以及在传统上代表性不足的非世界幼儿保育和教育协会(WEIRD)人群中,由婴儿发起的广泛而详细的自发交流行为。
{"title":"The breadth and specificity of 18-month-old’s infant-initiated interactions in naturalistic home settings","authors":"Didar Karadağ ,&nbsp;Marina Bazhydai ,&nbsp;Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar ,&nbsp;Hilal H. Şen","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101927","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101927","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Infants actively initiate social interactions aiming to elicit different types of responses from other people. This study aimed to document a variety of communicative interactions initiated by 18-month-old Turkish infants from diverse SES (<em>N</em> = 43) with their caregivers in their natural home settings. The infant-initiated interactions such as use of deictic gestures (e.g., pointing, holdouts), action demonstrations, vocalizations, and non-specific play actions were coded from video recordings and classified into two categories as need-based and non-need-based. Need-based interactions were further classified as a) biological (e.g., feeding); b) socio-emotional (e.g., cuddling), and non-need-based interactions (i.e., communicative intentions) were coded as a) expressive, b) requestive; c) information/help-seeking; d) information-giving. Infant-initiated non-need-based (88%) interactions were more prevalent compared to need-based interactions (12%). Among the non-need-based interactions, 50% aimed at expressing or sharing attention or emotion, 26% aimed at requesting an object or an action, and 12% aimed at seeking information or help. Infant-initiated information-giving events were rare. We further investigated the effects of familial SES and infant sex, finding no effect of either on the number of infant-initiated interactions. These findings suggest that at 18 months, infants actively communicate with their social partners to fulfil their need-based and non-need-based motivations using a wide range of verbal and nonverbal behaviors, regardless of their sex and socio-economic background. This study thoroughly characterizes a wide and detailed range of infant-initiated spontaneous communicative bids in hard-to-access contexts (infants’ daily lives at home) and with a traditionally underrepresented non-WEIRD population.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101927"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000067/pdfft?md5=14b93fc8dc1e6177d5a2b285a465d932&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000067-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140000256","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Let’s make music as we normally do: A systematic review of how early natural musical interactions between infant and caregiver have been studied in research 让我们像平常一样做音乐:系统回顾婴儿与照顾者之间早期自然音乐互动的研究情况
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101928
Beatriz Cavero , Pastora Martínez-Castilla , Ruth Campos

Musical interactions between babies and their primary caregivers are very frequent during the early years of life and their impact on dyadic interaction and infants’ development has garnered significant attention in recent literature. However, the difficulties that natural observations entail have meant that research often carries out methodological manipulations that have a significant impact on the phenomenon studied. In order to clarify how to investigate best natural musical interactions and the information that these can provide, we have carried out a systematic review to analyze the proposed scenarios and the variables analyzed in the studies published on such interactions between main caregivers and babies under three years old. We have screened 971 articles and yielded 27. We have found a higher prevalence in the literature of studies on singing interactions, between mothers and babies under 12 months of age. We have also been able to identify two extremes in terms of methodological structuring of natural interactions. Regarding the analysis variables, a few behaviors are repeated throughout the studies, being emotions, rhythmic behaviors and characterizations of the vocal emissions common between parents and babies. Synchrony is the dyadic variable with the most weight and also one of the preferred focuses of interest in the most recent literature that has undergone a shift of focus from characterization of musical interactions to the search for the mechanisms that underlie and make them specific.

婴儿与主要照顾者之间的音乐互动在其生命的早期阶段非常频繁,其对双亲互动和婴儿发展的影响在最近的文献中引起了极大的关注。然而,自然观察所带来的困难意味着,研究往往会在方法上进行操作,从而对所研究的现象产生重大影响。为了明确如何更好地调查自然音乐互动以及这些互动所能提供的信息,我们进行了一次系统性的回顾,分析了已发表的关于主要照顾者与三岁以下婴儿之间互动的研究中提出的情景和分析的变量。我们筛选了 971 篇文章,得出了 27 项结论。我们发现,在有关母亲与 12 个月以下婴儿之间歌唱互动的研究文献中,这种情况较为普遍。在自然互动的方法结构方面,我们还发现了两个极端。在分析变量方面,有几种行为在研究中反复出现,即父母和婴儿之间常见的情绪、节奏行为和发声特点。同步性是最重要的二元变量,也是最新文献中最关注的焦点之一,这些文献的重点已经从音乐互动的特征描述转移到寻找音乐互动的基础机制和特殊机制上。
{"title":"Let’s make music as we normally do: A systematic review of how early natural musical interactions between infant and caregiver have been studied in research","authors":"Beatriz Cavero ,&nbsp;Pastora Martínez-Castilla ,&nbsp;Ruth Campos","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101928","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101928","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Musical interactions between babies and their primary caregivers are very frequent during the early years of life and their impact on dyadic interaction and infants’ development has garnered significant attention in recent literature. However, the difficulties that natural observations entail have meant that research often carries out methodological manipulations that have a significant impact on the phenomenon studied. In order to clarify how to investigate best natural musical interactions and the information that these can provide, we have carried out a systematic review to analyze the proposed scenarios and the variables analyzed in the studies published on such interactions between main caregivers and babies under three years old. We have screened 971 articles and yielded 27. We have found a higher prevalence in the literature of studies on singing interactions, between mothers and babies under 12 months of age. We have also been able to identify two extremes in terms of methodological structuring of natural interactions. Regarding the analysis variables, a few behaviors are repeated throughout the studies, being emotions, rhythmic behaviors and characterizations of the vocal emissions common between parents and babies. Synchrony is the dyadic variable with the most weight and also one of the preferred focuses of interest in the most recent literature that has undergone a shift of focus from characterization of musical interactions to the search for the mechanisms that underlie and make them specific.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"75 ","pages":"Article 101928"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139993226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Breakdowns and repairs: Communication initiation and effectiveness in infants with and without an older sibling with autism 破损与修复:有或没有患有自闭症的哥哥姐姐的婴儿的沟通启动和有效性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101924
Samantha Plate , Jana M. Iverson

Infants initiate interactions to get their wants and needs met; but sometimes they are not effective in their communication and are misunderstood by caregivers. When this happens, they must recognize this breakdown in communication and attempt repairs. Experimental literature suggests that in neurotypically developing infants these skills develop during the first two years. However, little work has investigated communication breakdowns and repairs in populations of infants with known social communication difficulties (e.g., infants with an elevated likelihood for autism). Here we explored early social communication initiations, breakdowns, and repair strategies in naturalistic videos of 18-month-old infants (N = 64) with elevated likelihood (EL) for autism and other developmental delays (N = 49) and infants with population-level likelihood for autism (e.g., typical likelihood, TL, N = 15). EL infants, including those who later met criteria for autism (EL-AUT), initiated with caregivers, experienced breakdowns, and made repairs at similar rates to TL infants. However, the types of behaviors used differed, such that EL infants appeared to have a relative strength in making behavior regulation bids. EL-AUT infants used a large proportion of developmentally appropriate repair behaviors (i.e., addition and substitution), even though their repertoires of repair strategies were smaller. Additionally, EL-AUT infants produced a larger proportion of simplification repairs, which are less developmentally advanced and less helpful to interlocutors. Identifying patterns in how EL infants communicate with caregivers and capitalizing on their strengths could improve interventions focused on social communication.

婴儿会主动与人交流,以满足自己的愿望和需要;但有时他们的交流并不有效,会被照护者误解。当这种情况发生时,他们必须认识到这种沟通障碍并尝试修复。实验文献表明,神经发育正常的婴儿在头两年就会发展出这些技能。然而,很少有研究对已知有社交沟通障碍的婴儿(如自闭症可能性较高的婴儿)的沟通中断和修复进行调查。在此,我们通过自然视频探讨了 18 个月大的自闭症和其他发育迟缓可能性增高(EL)婴儿(64 个)(49 个)和人群自闭症可能性(如典型可能性,TL,15 个)婴儿的早期社交沟通启动、中断和修复策略。EL 婴儿(包括后来符合自闭症标准的婴儿(EL-AUT))与 TL 婴儿一样,会主动与照顾者沟通、经历崩溃并进行修复。然而,所使用的行为类型却有所不同,因此 EL 婴儿在进行行为调节方面似乎相对更有优势。尽管 EL-AUT 婴儿的修复策略范围较小,但他们使用了很大比例的适合其发展的修复行为(即添加和替换)。此外,EL-AUT 婴儿的简化修复行为所占比例较大,而简化修复行为在发育上并不先进,对对话者的帮助也较小。找出 EL 婴儿与照顾者沟通的模式并利用他们的优势,可以改善以社会沟通为重点的干预措施。
{"title":"Breakdowns and repairs: Communication initiation and effectiveness in infants with and without an older sibling with autism","authors":"Samantha Plate ,&nbsp;Jana M. Iverson","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101924","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101924","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Infants initiate interactions to get their wants and needs met; but sometimes they are not effective in their communication and are misunderstood by caregivers. When this happens, they must recognize this breakdown in communication and attempt repairs. Experimental literature suggests that in neurotypically developing infants these skills develop during the first two years. However, little work has investigated communication breakdowns and repairs in populations of infants with known social communication difficulties (e.g., infants with an elevated likelihood for autism). Here we explored early social communication initiations, breakdowns, and repair strategies in naturalistic videos of 18-month-old infants (<em>N</em> = 64) with elevated likelihood (EL) for autism and other developmental delays (<em>N</em> = 49) and infants with population-level likelihood for autism (e.g., typical likelihood, TL, <em>N</em> = 15). EL infants, including those who later met criteria for autism (EL-AUT), initiated with caregivers, experienced breakdowns, and made repairs at similar rates to TL infants. However, the types of behaviors used differed, such that EL infants appeared to have a relative strength in making behavior regulation bids. EL-AUT infants used a large proportion of developmentally appropriate repair behaviors (i.e., addition and substitution), even though their repertoires of repair strategies were smaller. Additionally, EL-AUT infants produced a larger proportion of simplification repairs, which are less developmentally advanced and less helpful to interlocutors. Identifying patterns in how EL infants communicate with caregivers and capitalizing on their strengths could improve interventions focused on social communication.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101924"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139699711","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Focused attention as a new sitter: How do infants balance it all? 作为一名新保姆,集中注意力:婴儿如何平衡这一切?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101926
Michele Gonçalves Maia , Sapir Soker-Elimaliah , Karl Jancart , Regina T. Harbourne , Sarah E. Berger

This study investigated the impact of postural control on infants’ Focused Attention (FA). Study 1 examined whether and how sitting independently versus with support impacted 6- to 8-month-old infants’ ability to focus attention during object exploration. FA measures did not depend on support condition. However, sitting experience was significantly negatively correlated with FA measures in the supported condition, suggesting that infants with more sitting experience performed fewer exploratory movements, possibly due to faster information processing ability compared to infants with less sitting experience. These unexpected findings prompted an exploration of more subtle looking behaviors during FA in Study 2—a case study of three infants who wore a head-mounted eye-tracker during an FA task. The ability to rapidly shift visual attention was key to gathering environmental information useful for problem solving—an interpretation that is supported by prior findings of the relationship between fast looks and faster information processing.

本研究探讨了姿势控制对婴儿集中注意力(FA)的影响。研究 1 探讨了独立坐姿与有支撑坐姿是否以及如何影响 6 至 8 个月大婴儿在探索物体时的注意力集中能力。FA测量并不取决于支撑条件。然而,在有支持的条件下,坐姿经验与注意力集中测量呈明显的负相关,这表明与坐姿经验较少的婴儿相比,坐姿经验较多的婴儿所做的探索动作较少,这可能是由于他们的信息处理能力较快。这些意料之外的发现促使我们在研究 2 中探讨了婴儿在注意力转移过程中更微妙的注视行为--该研究是对三名在注意力转移任务中佩戴头戴式眼动仪的婴儿进行的个案研究。快速转移视觉注意力的能力是收集对解决问题有用的环境信息的关键--这一解释得到了先前关于快速注视与快速信息处理之间关系的研究结果的支持。
{"title":"Focused attention as a new sitter: How do infants balance it all?","authors":"Michele Gonçalves Maia ,&nbsp;Sapir Soker-Elimaliah ,&nbsp;Karl Jancart ,&nbsp;Regina T. Harbourne ,&nbsp;Sarah E. Berger","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101926","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101926","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>This study investigated the impact of postural control on infants’ Focused Attention (FA). Study 1 examined whether and how sitting independently versus with support impacted 6- to 8-month-old infants’ ability to focus attention during object exploration. FA measures did not depend on support condition. However, sitting experience was significantly negatively correlated with FA measures in the supported condition, suggesting that infants with more sitting experience performed fewer exploratory movements, possibly due to faster </span>information processing<span> ability compared to infants with less sitting experience. These unexpected findings prompted an exploration of more subtle looking behaviors during FA in Study 2—a case study of three infants who wore a head-mounted eye-tracker during an FA task. The ability to rapidly shift visual attention was key to gathering environmental information useful for problem solving—an interpretation that is supported by prior findings of the relationship between fast looks and faster information processing.</span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101926"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139673863","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early object skill supports growth in role-differentiated bimanual manipulation in infants 早期物体技能支持婴儿角色差异化双臂操作的成长。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101925
Megan A. Taylor , Stefany Coxe , Eliza L. Nelson

The ability to coordinate the hands together to act on objects where each hand does something different is known as role-differentiated bimanual manipulation (RDBM). This study investigated two motor skills that may support the development of RDBM: infants’ early object skill and their early sitting skill. To evaluate these potential predictors of RDBM growth, 90 infants were examined in a lab-based longitudinal design over a 9-month period. Latent growth modeling was used to estimate RDBM growth trajectories over 9 to 14 months from infants’ object and sitting skills at 6 months, controlling for infant’s sex, mother’s education, and family income. Higher object skill, controlling for sitting skill, was related to a higher increase in RDBM over time. Sitting did not predict infants’ change in RDBM over time, controlling for object skill. The ability to manage multiple objects may support collaborative hand use by providing infants with opportunities to practice actions that will be needed later for RDBM. By comparison, sitting may free the hands in an unspecified manner for manipulation.

协调双手共同作用于物体的能力被称为角色分化双手操作(RDBM)。本研究调查了可能支持 RDBM 发展的两种运动技能:婴儿的早期物体技能和早期坐姿技能。为了评估这些潜在的 RDBM 成长预测因素,我们在实验室对 90 名婴儿进行了为期 9 个月的纵向研究。在控制婴儿性别、母亲教育程度和家庭收入的情况下,我们使用潜增长模型,根据婴儿 6 个月时的物体和坐姿技能,估计了 9 到 14 个月的 RDBM 增长轨迹。在不考虑坐姿技能的情况下,较高的物体技能与RDBM随时间推移的较高增长相关。在不考虑坐姿技能的情况下,坐姿并不能预测婴儿随着时间推移RDBM的变化。管理多个物体的能力可为婴儿提供练习动作的机会,从而支持他们协作使用手,而这些动作将是他们日后进行 RDBM 时所需的。相比之下,坐着可能会以一种不确定的方式解放婴儿的双手进行操作。
{"title":"Early object skill supports growth in role-differentiated bimanual manipulation in infants","authors":"Megan A. Taylor ,&nbsp;Stefany Coxe ,&nbsp;Eliza L. Nelson","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101925","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101925","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The ability to coordinate the hands together to act on objects where each hand does something different is known as role-differentiated bimanual manipulation (RDBM). This study investigated two motor skills that may support the development of RDBM: infants’ early object skill and their early sitting skill. To evaluate these potential predictors of RDBM growth, 90 infants were examined in a lab-based longitudinal design over a 9-month period. Latent growth modeling was used to estimate RDBM growth trajectories over 9 to 14 months from infants’ object and sitting skills at 6 months, controlling for infant’s sex, mother’s education, and family income. Higher object skill, controlling for sitting skill, was related to a higher increase in RDBM over time. Sitting did not predict infants’ change in RDBM over time, controlling for object skill. The ability to manage multiple objects may support collaborative hand use by providing infants with opportunities to practice actions that will be needed later for RDBM. By comparison, sitting may free the hands in an unspecified manner for manipulation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101925"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139577330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants’ pointing at nine months is associated with maternal sensitivity but not vocabulary 九个月大婴儿的指点能力与母亲的敏感性有关,但与词汇量无关
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101923
Elena Nicoladis, Poliana G. Barbosa

Infants often start pointing toward the end of their first year of life. Pointing shows a strong link to language, perhaps because parents label what infants point to. In the present study, we tested whether 9-month-olds’ pointing was related to parental sensitivity and concurrent and subsequent vocabulary scores. Observations were made of 88 9-month-old infants in free-play situations with their mothers. Less than half the infants produced at least one index-finger point. The mothers’ reactions to their infants’ behaviour were coded for sensitivity. The mothers of the infants who pointed were less directing and responded more contingently than the mothers of the infants who did not point. However, there was no difference in vocabulary scores of pointers and non-pointers, either concurrently or at 12 and 18 months of age. These results could mean that parents’ reactions play an important role in shaping pointing to be communicative.

婴儿通常在出生后第一年的末期就开始指点。指点与语言有密切联系,这可能是因为父母会给婴儿指的东西贴上标签。在本研究中,我们测试了 9 个月大婴儿的指点是否与父母的敏感性、同时和随后的词汇量得分有关。我们观察了 88 名 9 个月大的婴儿在与母亲自由玩耍时的情况。只有不到一半的婴儿至少用食指点了一下。对母亲对婴儿行为的反应进行了敏感度编码。与没有用手指指婴儿的母亲相比,用手指指婴儿的母亲的指导性更弱,反应更偶然。然而,无论是同时还是在 12 个月和 18 个月大时,指点婴儿和不指点婴儿的词汇量得分均无差异。这些结果可能意味着,父母的反应在塑造指点交流方面起着重要作用。
{"title":"Infants’ pointing at nine months is associated with maternal sensitivity but not vocabulary","authors":"Elena Nicoladis,&nbsp;Poliana G. Barbosa","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101923","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101923","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Infants often start pointing toward the end of their first year of life. Pointing shows a strong link to language, perhaps because parents label what infants point to. In the present study, we tested whether 9-month-olds’ pointing was related to parental sensitivity and concurrent and subsequent vocabulary scores. Observations were made of 88 9-month-old infants in free-play situations with their mothers. Less than half the infants produced at least one index-finger point. The mothers’ reactions to their infants’ behaviour were coded for sensitivity. The mothers of the infants who pointed were less directing and responded more contingently than the mothers of the infants who did not point. However, there was no difference in vocabulary scores of pointers and non-pointers, either concurrently or at 12 and 18 months of age. These results could mean that parents’ reactions play an important role in shaping pointing to be communicative.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101923"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016363832400002X/pdfft?md5=f451ad272d6bd4fef472c1d9198e8bba&pid=1-s2.0-S016363832400002X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139493540","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mother-infant self- and interactive contingency at four months and infant cognition at one year: A view from microanalysis 四个月时的母婴自我和互动偶然性与一岁时的婴儿认知:微观分析视角
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920
Beatrice Beebe , Gavkhar Abdurokhmonova , Sang Han Lee , Georgios Dougalis , Frances Champagne , Virginia Rauh , Molly Algermissen , Julie Herbstman , Amy E. Margolis

Although a considerable literature documents associations between early mother-infant interaction and cognitive outcomes in the first years of life, few studies examine the contributions of contingently coordinated mother-infant interaction to infant cognitive development. This study examined associations between the temporal dynamics of the contingent coordination of mother-infant face-to-face interaction at 4 months and cognitive performance on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at age one year in a sample of (N = 100) Latina mother-infant pairs. Split-screen videotaped interactions were coded on a one second time base for the communication modalities of infant and mother gaze and facial affect, infant vocal affect, and mother touch. Multi-level time-series models evaluated self- and interactive contingent processes in these modalities and revealed 4-month patterns of interaction associated with higher one-year cognitive performance, not identified in prior studies. Infant and mother self-contingency, the moment-to-moment probability that the individual’s prior behavior predicts the individual’s future behavior, was the most robust measure associated with infant cognitive performance. Self-contingency findings showed that more varying infant behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater modulation of negative affect; more stable maternal behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater likelihood of sustaining positive facial affect. Although interactive contingency findings were sparse, they showed that, when mothers looked away, or dampened their faces to interest or mild negative facial affect, infants with higher 12-month cognitive performance were less likely to show negative vocal affect. We suggest that infant ability to modulate negative affect, and maternal ability to sustain positive affect, may be mutually reinforcing, together creating a dyadic climate that is associated with more optimal infant cognitive development.

尽管有大量文献记录了早期母婴互动与婴儿出生后最初几年的认知结果之间的关系,但很少有研究探讨偶然协调的母婴互动对婴儿认知发展的贡献。本研究以(N = 100)拉丁裔母婴对为样本,考察了 4 个月时母婴面对面互动的偶然协调的时间动态与 1 岁时贝利婴儿发展量表(Bayley Scales of Infant Development)认知表现之间的关系。分屏互动录像以一秒为基准,对婴儿和母亲的注视、面部情感、婴儿的声音情感和母亲的抚摸等交流方式进行编码。多层次时间序列模型评估了这些模式中的自我和互动或然过程,并揭示了 4 个月的互动模式与更高的一年期认知表现相关,这在之前的研究中是没有发现的。婴儿和母亲的自我权变,即个体先前行为预测个体未来行为的瞬间概率,是与婴儿认知表现相关的最可靠的测量指标。自我权变研究结果表明,婴儿行为变化越多,婴儿认知能力越强,即对负面情绪的调节能力越强;母亲行为越稳定,婴儿认知能力越强,即维持面部积极情绪的可能性越大。虽然交互式或然性研究结果很少,但它们表明,当母亲将目光移开,或将脸部表情减弱到感兴趣或轻微的负面面部情绪时,12 个月认知能力较高的婴儿较少表现出负面的声音情绪。我们认为,婴儿调节消极情绪的能力和母亲维持积极情绪的能力可能是相辅相成的,它们共同创造了一种与婴儿认知发展更理想相关的家庭氛围。
{"title":"Mother-infant self- and interactive contingency at four months and infant cognition at one year: A view from microanalysis","authors":"Beatrice Beebe ,&nbsp;Gavkhar Abdurokhmonova ,&nbsp;Sang Han Lee ,&nbsp;Georgios Dougalis ,&nbsp;Frances Champagne ,&nbsp;Virginia Rauh ,&nbsp;Molly Algermissen ,&nbsp;Julie Herbstman ,&nbsp;Amy E. Margolis","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101920","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span>Although a considerable literature documents associations between early mother-infant interaction and cognitive outcomes in the first years of life, few studies examine the contributions of contingently coordinated mother-infant interaction to infant cognitive development. This study examined associations between the temporal dynamics of the contingent coordination of mother-infant face-to-face interaction at 4 months and cognitive performance on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at age one year in a sample of (N = 100) Latina mother-infant pairs. Split-screen videotaped interactions were coded on a one second time base for the communication modalities of infant and mother gaze and facial affect, infant vocal affect, and mother touch. Multi-level time-series models evaluated self- and interactive contingent processes in these modalities and revealed 4-month patterns of interaction associated with higher one-year cognitive performance, not identified in prior studies. Infant and mother </span><em>self-contingency</em><span>, the moment-to-moment probability that the individual’s prior behavior<span> predicts the individual’s future behavior, was the most robust measure associated with infant cognitive performance. Self-contingency findings showed that more varying infant behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater modulation of negative affect; more stable maternal behavior was optimal for higher infant cognitive performance, namely, greater likelihood of sustaining positive facial affect. Although </span></span><em>interactive contingency</em> findings were sparse, they showed that, when mothers looked away, or dampened their faces to interest or mild negative facial affect, infants with higher 12-month cognitive performance were less likely to show negative vocal affect. We suggest that infant ability to modulate negative affect, and maternal ability to sustain positive affect, may be mutually reinforcing, together creating a dyadic climate that is associated with more optimal infant cognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101920"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139480238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The United States reference values of the Bayley III motor scale are suitable in Suriname 贝利 III 运动量表的美国参考值适用于苏里南
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922
Maria JAJ Fleurkens-Peeters , Wilco CWR Zijlmans , Reinier P. Akkermans , Maria WG Nijhuis-van der Sanden , Anjo JWM Janssen

To determine if the United States reference values of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, version III motor scale are suitable for Surinamese infants, we assessed 151 healthy infants at 3, 12, 24 and 36 months of age. The mean fine motor, gross motor, and composite scores of the total group did not significantly differ from the US norms, although some significant but not clinically relevant differences were found (lower fine motor scores at 12 months, lower gross motor and total composite scores at 24 months, and higher scores for gross motor and composite scores at 3 months).

为了确定美国贝利婴幼儿发展量表(Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development)第三版运动量表的参考值是否适合苏里南婴儿,我们对 151 名健康婴儿进行了 3、12、24 和 36 个月大时的评估。尽管发现了一些显著但与临床无关的差异(12 个月时精细运动得分较低、24 个月时粗大运动得分和综合得分较低、3 个月时粗大运动得分和综合得分较高),但总组的精细运动、粗大运动和综合得分的平均值与美国标准值没有显著差异。
{"title":"The United States reference values of the Bayley III motor scale are suitable in Suriname","authors":"Maria JAJ Fleurkens-Peeters ,&nbsp;Wilco CWR Zijlmans ,&nbsp;Reinier P. Akkermans ,&nbsp;Maria WG Nijhuis-van der Sanden ,&nbsp;Anjo JWM Janssen","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2024.101922","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To determine if the United States reference values of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, version III motor scale are suitable for Surinamese infants, we assessed 151 healthy infants at 3, 12, 24 and 36 months of age. The mean fine motor, gross motor, and composite scores of the total group did not significantly differ from the US norms, although some significant but not clinically relevant differences were found (lower fine motor scores at 12 months, lower gross motor and total composite scores at 24 months, and higher scores for gross motor and composite scores at 3 months).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101922"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638324000018/pdfft?md5=da7a645ccaf0b04cf70d1c36e8840437&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638324000018-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139436617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of caregiver inhibitory control on infant visual working memory 照顾者的抑制控制对婴儿视觉工作记忆的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921
Christina Davidson, Aimee Theyer, Ghada Amaireh, Sobanawartiny Wijeakumar

Visual working memory (VWM) emerges in the first year of life and has far-reaching implications for academic and later life outcomes. Given that caregivers play a significant role in shaping cognitive function in children, it is important to understand how they might impact VWM development as early as infancy. The current study investigated whether caregivers’ efficiency of regulating inhibitory control was associated with VWM function in their infants. Eighty-eight caregivers were presented with a Go-NoGo task to assess inhibitory control. An efficiency score was calculated using their behavioural responses. Eighty-six 6-to-10-month-old infants were presented with a preferential looking task to assess VWM function. VWM load was manipulated across one (low load), two (medium load) and three (high load) items. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy was used to record brain activation from caregivers and their infants. We found no direct association between caregiver efficiency and infant VWM behaviour. However, we found an indirect association - caregiver efficiency was linked to infant VWM through left-lateralized fronto-parietal engagement. Specifically, infants with low efficiency caregivers showed decreasing left-lateralized parietal engagement with increasing VWM performance at the medium and high loads compared to infants with high efficiency caregivers, who did not show any load- or performance-dependent modulation. Our findings contribute to a growing body of literature examining the role that caregivers play in early neurocognitive development.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)出现于儿童出生后的第一年,对其学业和日后的生活有着深远的影响。鉴于照顾者在塑造儿童认知功能方面扮演着重要角色,了解他们如何影响婴儿时期的视觉工作记忆发展就显得尤为重要。本研究调查了照顾者调节抑制控制的效率是否与婴儿的VWM功能有关。研究人员向 88 名照顾者布置了一项 Go-NoGo 任务,以评估其抑制控制能力。根据他们的行为反应计算出效率分数。86 名 6 至 10 个月大的婴儿接受了优先寻找任务,以评估 VWM 功能。VWM 负荷在一个(低负荷)、两个(中等负荷)和三个(高负荷)项目中进行调节。我们使用功能性近红外光谱记录了照顾者及其婴儿的大脑激活情况。我们发现,照顾者的效率与婴儿的 VWM 行为之间没有直接联系。但是,我们发现了一种间接的关联--照顾者的效率通过左侧前顶叶的参与与婴儿的暴力记忆行为相关联。具体来说,在中负荷和高负荷下,低效能照料者的婴儿表现出左侧顶叶参与度的下降,而高能效照料者的婴儿则没有表现出任何与负荷或表现相关的调节。越来越多的文献研究了照料者在婴儿早期神经认知发展中所扮演的角色,我们的研究结果为这些文献的研究做出了贡献。
{"title":"The impact of caregiver inhibitory control on infant visual working memory","authors":"Christina Davidson,&nbsp;Aimee Theyer,&nbsp;Ghada Amaireh,&nbsp;Sobanawartiny Wijeakumar","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101921","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Visual working memory (VWM) emerges in the first year of life and has far-reaching implications for academic and later life outcomes. Given that caregivers play a significant role in shaping cognitive function in children, it is important to understand how they might impact VWM development as early as infancy. The current study investigated whether caregivers’ efficiency of regulating inhibitory control was associated with VWM function in their infants. Eighty-eight caregivers were presented with a Go-NoGo task to assess inhibitory control. An efficiency score was calculated using their behavioural responses. Eighty-six 6-to-10-month-old infants were presented with a preferential looking task to assess VWM function. VWM load was manipulated across one (low load), two (medium load) and three (high load) items. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy was used to record brain activation from caregivers and their infants. We found no direct association between caregiver efficiency and infant VWM behaviour. However, we found an indirect association - caregiver efficiency was linked to infant VWM through left-lateralized fronto-parietal engagement. Specifically, infants with low efficiency caregivers showed decreasing left-lateralized parietal engagement with increasing VWM performance at the medium and high loads compared to infants with high efficiency caregivers, who did not show any load- or performance-dependent modulation. Our findings contribute to a growing body of literature examining the role that caregivers play in early neurocognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101921"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001133/pdfft?md5=0c3ba700c3bd37373bd6f652babbfd64&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001133-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139419070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Finding a secure base: Exploring children’s attachment behaviors with professional caregivers during the first months of daycare 寻找安全基地探索儿童在入托前几个月与专业保育员的依恋行为
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919
Alessia Macagno, Paola Molina

Recent decades have seen a major rise in demand for daycare services for children aged 0 to 3 years, and this has increased research interest in the child-professional caregiver relationship at daycare centers: How does the relationship between children and their new caregivers develop over time? How long does it take for children to settle in at daycare? What variables can influence the settling-in process? These questions are all of the utmost salience and bear crucial implications for children, parents, and daycare practitioners. In this study, we set out to explore the relationship between infants and their new caregivers over the first two months in daycare, using the Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary. The study involved seven Italian daycare centres and 55 professional caregivers, who observed 148 children (M=17.8 months). The children’s attachment behaviors were assessed at three time-points: when the children started attending daycare (T1), one month later (T2), and two months later (T3). We found that positive attachment behaviors (Secure and Non-Distressed) increased over time, whereas insecure behaviors (Avoidant and Resistant) decreased. Most of the change took place during the first month. Furthermore, children who had attended more daycare more regularly (with fewer days of absence) displayed fewer avoidant behaviors and a more rapid decrease in resistant behaviors than did children who were absent more frequently. The findings suggest that the PCAD may be usefully deployed to observe and analyze children while they are settling into a new daycare setting, especially in relation to their exploratory behaviors.

近几十年来,0 至 3 岁儿童对日托服务的需求大幅增加,这也增加了对日托中心儿童与专业保育员关系的研究兴趣:随着时间的推移,儿童与新保育员之间的关系如何发展?儿童在日托中心安顿下来需要多长时间?哪些变量会影响适应过程?这些问题都极为重要,对儿童、家长和托儿所从业人员都有重要影响。在这项研究中,我们使用 "专业保育员依恋日记"(Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary)来探讨婴儿在入托的头两个月中与新保育员之间的关系。这项研究涉及 7 个意大利日托中心和 55 名专业保育员,他们观察了 148 名儿童(M=17.8 个月)。在三个时间点对儿童的依恋行为进行了评估:儿童开始上托儿所时(T1)、一个月后(T2)和两个月后(T3)。我们发现,随着时间的推移,积极的依恋行为(安全型和非受压型)有所增加,而不安全的行为(回避型和抗拒型)则有所减少。大部分变化发生在第一个月。此外,与缺席次数较多的儿童相比,经常参加日托(缺席天数较少)的儿童表现出的回避行为较少,抵触行为减少得更快。研究结果表明,PCAD 可用于观察和分析儿童在适应新托儿所环境时的表现,尤其是他们的探索行为。
{"title":"Finding a secure base: Exploring children’s attachment behaviors with professional caregivers during the first months of daycare","authors":"Alessia Macagno,&nbsp;Paola Molina","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101919","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent decades have seen a major rise in demand for daycare services for children aged 0 to 3 years, and this has increased research interest in the child-professional caregiver relationship at daycare centers: How does the relationship between children and their new caregivers develop over time? How long does it take for children to settle in at daycare? What variables can influence the settling-in process? These questions are all of the utmost salience and bear crucial implications for children, parents, and daycare practitioners. In this study, we set out to explore the relationship between infants and their new caregivers over the first two months in daycare, using the <em>Professional Caregiver Attachment Diary</em>. The study involved seven Italian daycare centres and 55 professional caregivers, who observed 148 children (<em>M</em>=17.8 months). The children’s attachment behaviors were assessed at three time-points: when the children started attending daycare (T1), one month later (T2), and two months later (T3). We found that positive attachment behaviors (<em>Secure</em> and <em>Non-Distressed</em>) increased over time, whereas insecure behaviors (<em>Avoidant</em> and <em>Resistant</em>) decreased. Most of the change took place during the first month. Furthermore, children who had attended more daycare more regularly (with fewer days of absence) displayed fewer avoidant behaviors and a more rapid decrease in resistant behaviors than did children who were absent more frequently. The findings suggest that the PCAD may be usefully deployed to observe and analyze children while they are settling into a new daycare setting, especially in relation to their exploratory behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101919"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016363832300111X/pdfft?md5=e08774491853108c84b6bf9dcaa2e477&pid=1-s2.0-S016363832300111X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139399317","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant Behavior & Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1