首页 > 最新文献

Infant Behavior & Development最新文献

英文 中文
When crying turns to hitting: Examining maternal responses to negative affect 当哭泣变成殴打研究母亲对负面情绪的反应
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918
Brooke Edelman , Tamara Del Vecchio

Physical aggression in toddlerhood is empirically linked to anger and often conceptualized as a byproduct of frustration and related negative affect. Further, parenting is the major environmental construct implicated in the development of aggressive behaviors. Given parents’ role as “external regulators,” parents’ responses to their toddlers’ negative affect may serve to escalate or de-escalate their toddlers’ affective experience, thereby impacting the likelihood of subsequent aggression. In the present study, we examined whether parents’ negative affect, harsh, soothing, and distracting responses to their toddlers’ negative affect mediated the relation between toddlers’ negative affect and their aggressive behavior in brief conflict episodes. During a laboratory visit, a community sample of 69 mother-toddler dyads was observed in a structured interaction task. We found that child negative affect is associated with subsequent aggressive behavior by way of maternal harsh responses to negative affect. Negative emotional expression, soothing, and distraction neither facilitated or hindered children’s escalation from negative affect to aggression. Our findings support a dyadic intervention in which patterns of coercive parent-child interactions are targets for prevention and intervention of toddler aggression.

根据经验,幼儿期的身体攻击行为与愤怒有关,通常被认为是挫折和相关负面情绪的副产品。此外,父母的教养方式是与攻击行为的发展相关的主要环境因素。鉴于父母作为 "外部调节者 "的角色,父母对幼儿负面情绪的反应可能会使幼儿的情绪体验升级或降级,从而影响其随后发生攻击行为的可能性。在本研究中,我们考察了父母对幼儿负面情绪的反应、严厉反应、安抚反应和分散注意力反应是否对幼儿的负面情绪与他们在短暂冲突事件中的攻击行为之间的关系起到了中介作用。在一次实验室访问中,我们对 69 个社区样本中的母亲和幼儿组合进行了结构化互动任务观察。我们发现,通过母亲对负面情绪的严厉反应,儿童的负面情绪与随后的攻击行为有关。负面情绪表达、安抚和转移注意力既不会促进也不会阻碍儿童从负面情绪升级到攻击行为。我们的研究结果支持以胁迫性亲子互动模式作为预防和干预幼儿攻击行为的目标的二元干预。
{"title":"When crying turns to hitting: Examining maternal responses to negative affect","authors":"Brooke Edelman ,&nbsp;Tamara Del Vecchio","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101918","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Physical aggression in toddlerhood is empirically linked to anger and often conceptualized as a byproduct of frustration and related negative affect. Further, parenting is the major environmental construct implicated in the development of aggressive behaviors. Given parents’ role as “external regulators,” parents’ responses to their toddlers’ negative affect may serve to escalate or de-escalate their toddlers’ affective experience, thereby impacting the likelihood of subsequent aggression. In the present study, we examined whether parents’ negative affect, harsh, soothing, and distracting responses to their toddlers’ negative affect mediated the relation between toddlers’ negative affect and their aggressive behavior in brief conflict episodes. During a laboratory visit, a community sample of 69 mother-toddler dyads was observed in a structured interaction task. We found that child negative affect is associated with subsequent aggressive behavior by way of maternal harsh responses to negative affect. Negative emotional expression, soothing, and distraction neither facilitated or hindered children’s escalation from negative affect to aggression. Our findings support a dyadic intervention in which patterns of coercive parent-child interactions are targets for prevention and intervention of toddler aggression.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101918"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139399318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preterm toddlers’ joint attention characteristics during dyadic interactions with their mothers and fathers compared to full-term toddlers at age 2 years 早产幼儿与两岁足月幼儿在与母亲和父亲的二人互动中的共同注意特点比较
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915
Merve Ataman-Devrim , Jean Quigley , Elizabeth Nixon

The current study investigates Joint Attention (JA) characteristics (duration, frequency, source of initiation, type of JA, agent of termination, missed and unsuccessful episodes) in preterm and full-term toddlers’ interactions with their mothers and fathers, separately. Thirty-one singleton full-term (Mage = 24.07 months, SD = 1.45; 13 boys) and 17 singleton preterm toddlers (Madjustedage = 24.72 months, SD = 3.39; 12 boys) participated in the study with both parents. JA episodes were examined during dyadic five-minute free play sessions, were coded second-by-second, and were analysed using two-way mixed ANOVAs. Although the total amount of time spent in JA was not significantly different between the preterm and the full-term groups, JA episodes were more frequent, specifically supported JA episodes, and were more often terminated by the child during parent-preterm toddler interactions. Moreover, preterm toddlers missed their fathers’ attempts for JA more often than their mothers’ and more often than full-term toddlers missed their fathers’ and mothers’ bids for JA. Further, regardless of the birth status, toddlers initiated more JA with mothers than fathers, and fathers redirected their child’s attention to initiate JA more than mothers. Findings indicate that preterm toddlers may struggle to respond to JA bids, especially with their fathers, and to sustain their attention on a specific object or event during interactions. Preterm toddlers may need more support to engage in JA relative to their full-term peers, and redirecting attention strategy may not be optimal for them. Also, toddlers’ JA interactions may be different with their mothers and fathers. Findings contribute to the literature by demonstrating preterm toddlers’ JA characteristics with both parents compared to full-term toddlers at age two.

本研究分别调查了早产儿和足月幼儿在与母亲和父亲互动时的联合注意(JA)特征(持续时间、频率、启动源、JA类型、终止因素、错过和不成功事件)。31名单胎足月儿(Mage = 24.07个月,SD = 1.45;13名男孩)和17名单胎早产儿(Madjustedage = 24.72个月,SD = 3.39;12名男孩)与父母双方一起参与了研究。JA 事件是在 5 分钟的双亲自由游戏过程中进行的,逐秒进行编码,并使用双向混合方差分析。虽然早产儿和足月儿在联合行动中所花费的总时间没有显著差异,但在父母与早产儿的互动中,早产儿的联合行动更频繁,特别是支持性联合行动,而且更经常被孩子终止。此外,早产幼儿错过父亲尝试 JA 的次数多于母亲,错过父亲和母亲请求 JA 的次数也多于足月幼儿。此外,无论出生状况如何,幼儿主动与母亲进行的联合行动多于主动与父亲进行的联合行动,而且父亲比母亲更多地将孩子的注意力转向主动与母亲进行的联合行动。研究结果表明,早产幼儿可能很难对联合行动的请求做出反应,尤其是对父亲的请求,也很难在互动过程中将注意力持续放在特定的物体或事件上。与足月幼儿相比,早产幼儿可能需要更多的支持才能参与联合行动,而重新引导注意力的策略对他们来说可能不是最佳选择。此外,幼儿与母亲和父亲之间的联合行动互动也可能不同。研究结果表明,与足月幼儿相比,早产幼儿在两岁时与父母双方的联合行动具有不同的特点,从而为相关文献做出了贡献。
{"title":"Preterm toddlers’ joint attention characteristics during dyadic interactions with their mothers and fathers compared to full-term toddlers at age 2 years","authors":"Merve Ataman-Devrim ,&nbsp;Jean Quigley ,&nbsp;Elizabeth Nixon","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101915","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The current study investigates Joint Attention (JA) characteristics (duration, frequency, source of initiation, type of JA, agent of termination, missed and unsuccessful episodes) in preterm and full-term toddlers’ interactions with their mothers and fathers, separately. Thirty-one singleton full-term (<em>M</em>age = 24.07 months, <em>SD</em> = 1.45; 13 boys) and 17 singleton preterm toddlers (<em>M</em>adjustedage = 24.72 months, <em>SD</em> = 3.39; 12 boys) participated in the study with both parents. JA episodes were examined during dyadic five-minute free play sessions, were coded second-by-second, and were analysed using two-way mixed ANOVAs. Although the total amount of time spent in JA was not significantly different between the preterm and the full-term groups, JA episodes were more frequent, specifically supported JA episodes, and were more often terminated by the child during parent-preterm toddler interactions. Moreover, preterm toddlers missed their fathers’ attempts for JA more often than their mothers’ and more often than full-term toddlers missed their fathers’ and mothers’ bids for JA. Further, regardless of the birth status, toddlers initiated more JA with mothers than fathers, and fathers redirected their child’s attention to initiate JA more than mothers. Findings indicate that preterm toddlers may struggle to respond to JA bids, especially with their fathers, and to sustain their attention on a specific object or event during interactions. Preterm toddlers may need more support to engage in JA relative to their full-term peers, and redirecting attention strategy may not be optimal for them. Also, toddlers’ JA interactions may be different with their mothers and fathers. Findings contribute to the literature by demonstrating preterm toddlers’ JA characteristics with both parents compared to full-term toddlers at age two.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101915"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001078/pdfft?md5=9f4c857d956cc031de35ec5e16b6a7c7&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001078-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139072446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of maternal gaze responsiveness on infants’ gaze following and later vocabulary development 母亲的凝视反应能力对婴儿的凝视跟踪和日后词汇发展的影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917
Eugenia Wildt, Katharina J. Rohlfing

Research has shown that infants’ language development is influenced by their gaze following—an ability linked to their cognitive and social development. Following social learning approaches, this pilot study explored whether variations in gaze following and later vocabulary scores relate to early mother–infant interactions by focusing on the role of mothers’ gaze responsiveness in infants’ attentional and language development. We recruited 15 mother–child pairs in Poland and assessed their engagement in joint attention episodes. Results indicate that mothers foster their infants' gaze-following ability by providing them with numerous opportunities to participate in the task. We also confirmed a correlation between infants’ gaze-following ability at 6 months and their vocabulary scores at 24 months. However, combining both infants’ gaze following and mothers’ gaze monitoring as predictors in one model revealed that maternal gaze monitoring was a stronger predictor of infants’ later vocabulary growth. Overall, this study emphasizes that mothers’ gaze responsiveness is a crucial feature of scaffolding that impacts on infants’ gaze following and language development.

研究表明,婴儿的语言发展受其目光追随能力的影响--这种能力与婴儿的认知和社会发展息息相关。根据社会学习方法,这项试验性研究通过关注母亲的注视反应能力在婴儿的注意力和语言发展中的作用,探讨了注视跟随的变化和日后的词汇得分是否与早期的母婴互动有关。我们在波兰招募了 15 对母婴,并评估了他们参与联合注意力事件的情况。结果表明,母亲通过为婴儿提供大量参与任务的机会来培养他们的目光追随能力。我们还证实,婴儿 6 个月时的注视跟踪能力与他们 24 个月时的词汇量得分之间存在相关性。然而,将婴儿的注视跟踪能力和母亲的注视监控能力作为一个预测因素结合在一个模型中,结果显示,母亲的注视监控能力对婴儿日后的词汇量增长有更强的预测作用。总之,本研究强调,母亲的注视反应能力是影响婴儿注视跟随和语言发展的重要支架特征。
{"title":"The impact of maternal gaze responsiveness on infants’ gaze following and later vocabulary development","authors":"Eugenia Wildt,&nbsp;Katharina J. Rohlfing","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101917","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research has shown that infants’ language development is influenced by their gaze following—an ability linked to their cognitive and social development. Following social learning approaches, this pilot study explored whether variations in gaze following and later vocabulary scores relate to early mother–infant interactions by focusing on the role of mothers’ gaze responsiveness in infants’ attentional and language development. We recruited 15 mother–child pairs in Poland and assessed their engagement in joint attention episodes. Results indicate that mothers foster their infants' gaze-following ability by providing them with numerous opportunities to participate in the task. We also confirmed a correlation between infants’ gaze-following ability at 6 months and their vocabulary scores at 24 months. However, combining both infants’ gaze following and mothers’ gaze monitoring as predictors in one model revealed that maternal gaze monitoring was a stronger predictor of infants’ later vocabulary growth. Overall, this study emphasizes that mothers’ gaze responsiveness is a crucial feature of scaffolding that impacts on infants’ gaze following and language development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101917"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001091/pdfft?md5=46fe8f4eae451d764b608b54f4e9b1a7&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001091-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138886828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predictors of executive function among 2 years old from a Thai birth cohort 泰国出生队列中两岁儿童执行功能的预测因素
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916
Pimjuta Nimmapirat , Nancy Fiedler , Panrapee Suttiwan , Margaret Wolan Sullivan , Pamela Ohman-Strickland , Parinya Panuwet , Dana Boyd Barr , Tippawan Prapamontol , Warangkana Naksen , for the SAWASDEE birth cohort investigative team

Executive function (EF) is a critical skill for academic achievement. Research on the psychosocial and environmental predictors of EF, particularly among Southeast Asian, agricultural, and low income/rural populations, is limited. Our longitudinal study explored the influence of agricultural environmental, psychosocial, and temperamental factors on children’s emerging EF. Three-hundred and nine farm worker women were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy. We evaluated the effects of prenatal insecticide exposure and psychosocial factors on “cool” (i.e., cognitive: A-not-B task, looking version) and “hot” EF (i.e., affective, response inhibition) measures of emerging EF. Maternal urine samples were collected monthly during pregnancy, composited, and analyzed for dialkylphosphate (DAP) metabolites of organophosphate insecticides. Psychosocial factors included socioeconomic status, maternal psychological factors, and quality of mother-child behavioral interactions. Backward stepwise regressions evaluated predictors of children’s EF at 12 (N = 288), 18 (N = 277) and 24 (N = 280) months of age. We observed different predictive models for cool EF, as measured by A-not-B task, vs. hot EF, as measured by response inhibition tasks. Report of housing quality as a surrogate for income was a significant predictor of emerging EF. However, these variables had opposite effects for cool vs. hot EF. More financial resources predicted better cool EF performance but poorer hot EF performance. Qualitative findings indicate that homes with fewer resources were in tribal areas where children must remain close to an adult for safety reasons. This finding suggests that challenging physical environments (e.g., an elevated bamboo home with no electricity or running water), may contribute to development of higher levels of response inhibition through parental socialization methods that emphasize compliance. Children who tended to show more arousal and excitability, and joy reactivity as young infants in the laboratory setting had better cognitive performance. In contrast, maternal emotional availability was a significant predictor of hot EF. As expected, increased maternal exposure to pesticides during pregnancy was associated with worse cognitive performance but was not associated with inhibitory control. Identifying risk factors contributing to the differential developmental pathways of cool and hot EF will inform prevention strategies to promote healthy development in this and other unstudied rural, low income Southeast Asian farming communities.

执行功能(EF)是学术成就的关键技能。关于EF的社会心理和环境预测因素的研究,特别是在东南亚、农业和低收入/农村人口中,是有限的。我们的纵向研究探讨了农业环境、社会心理和气质因素对儿童出现EF的影响。在怀孕的前三个月招募了309名农场女工。我们评估了产前杀虫剂暴露和社会心理因素对新兴EF的“冷”(即认知:A-not-B任务,外观版本)和“热”(即情感,反应抑制)测量的影响。妊娠期间每月采集产妇尿液样本,合成并分析有机磷杀虫剂的二烷基磷酸(DAP)代谢物。社会心理因素包括社会经济地位、母亲心理因素和母子行为互动质量。通过反向逐步回归评估儿童在12 (N = 288)、18 (N = 277)和24 (N = 280)月龄时EF的预测因子。我们观察到冷EF和热EF的不同预测模型,前者通过a -非- b任务测量,后者通过反应抑制任务测量。报告住房质量作为收入的替代品是新兴EF的重要预测因子。然而,这些变量对冷EF和热EF有相反的影响。更多的财政资源预示着较好的冷EF绩效,但较差的热EF绩效。定性调查结果表明,资源较少的家庭位于部落地区,那里的儿童出于安全原因必须与成年人待在一起。这一发现表明,具有挑战性的物理环境(例如,没有电或自来水的高架竹屋)可能有助于通过强调依从性的父母社会化方法发展更高水平的反应抑制。那些在实验室环境中表现出更多兴奋、兴奋和快乐反应的儿童,他们的认知表现更好。母亲情绪可得性是热EF的显著预测因子。正如预期的那样,孕妇在怀孕期间接触杀虫剂的增加与认知能力下降有关,但与抑制控制无关。确定导致冷热EF不同发展途径的风险因素将为预防策略提供信息,以促进该地区和其他未研究的农村低收入东南亚农业社区的健康发展。
{"title":"Predictors of executive function among 2 years old from a Thai birth cohort","authors":"Pimjuta Nimmapirat ,&nbsp;Nancy Fiedler ,&nbsp;Panrapee Suttiwan ,&nbsp;Margaret Wolan Sullivan ,&nbsp;Pamela Ohman-Strickland ,&nbsp;Parinya Panuwet ,&nbsp;Dana Boyd Barr ,&nbsp;Tippawan Prapamontol ,&nbsp;Warangkana Naksen ,&nbsp;for the SAWASDEE birth cohort investigative team","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101916","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Executive function (EF) is a critical skill for academic achievement. Research on the psychosocial and environmental predictors of EF, particularly among </span>Southeast Asian, agricultural, and low income/rural populations, is limited. Our longitudinal study explored the influence of agricultural environmental, psychosocial, and temperamental factors on children’s emerging EF. Three-hundred and nine farm worker women were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy. We evaluated the effects of prenatal insecticide exposure and psychosocial factors on “cool” (i.e., cognitive: A-not-B task, looking version) and “hot” EF (i.e., affective, response inhibition) measures of emerging EF. Maternal urine samples were collected monthly during pregnancy, composited, and analyzed for dialkylphosphate (DAP) metabolites of </span>organophosphate insecticides<span>. Psychosocial factors included socioeconomic status, maternal psychological factors, and quality of mother-child behavioral interactions. Backward stepwise regressions evaluated predictors of children’s EF at 12 (N = 288), 18 (N = 277) and 24 (N = 280) months of age. We observed different predictive models for cool EF, as measured by A-not-B task, vs. hot EF, as measured by response inhibition tasks. Report of housing quality as a surrogate for income was a significant predictor of emerging EF. However, these variables had opposite effects for cool vs. hot EF. More financial resources predicted better cool EF performance but poorer hot EF performance. Qualitative findings indicate that homes with fewer resources were in tribal areas where children must remain close to an adult for safety reasons. This finding suggests that challenging physical environments (e.g., an elevated bamboo home with no electricity or running water), may contribute to development of higher levels of response inhibition through parental socialization<span> methods that emphasize compliance. Children who tended to show more arousal and excitability, and joy reactivity as young infants in the laboratory setting had better cognitive performance. In contrast, maternal emotional availability was a significant predictor of hot EF. As expected, increased maternal exposure to pesticides during pregnancy was associated with worse cognitive performance but was not associated with inhibitory control. Identifying risk factors contributing to the differential developmental pathways of cool and hot EF will inform prevention strategies to promote healthy development in this and other unstudied rural, low income Southeast Asian farming communities.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101916"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138656670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching 触摸屏可促进婴儿的对象-对话者参照切换
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914
Kimberley M. Hudspeth, Charlie Lewis

We re-examine whether the type of object played with influences parent-infant joint attention. A within-participants comparison of 24 parent-9-month-old dyads, used head-mounted eye-tracking to measure parental naming and infant attention during play with touchscreen apps on a touchscreen tablet or matched interactive toys. Infants engaged in sustained attention more to the toy than the tablet. Parents named objects less in toy play. Infants exhibited more gaze shifts between the object and their parent during tablet play. Contrasting previous studies, these findings suggest that joint tablet play can be more interactive than with toys, and raise questions about the recommendation that infants should not be exposed at all to such technology.

我们重新研究了玩耍对象的类型是否会影响父母和婴儿的共同注意力。我们对 24 名父母和 9 个月大的婴儿进行了参与者内比较,使用头戴式眼动跟踪仪测量了父母在触摸屏平板电脑上玩触摸屏应用程序或玩匹配的互动玩具时的命名和婴儿的注意力。婴儿对玩具的持续注意力高于平板电脑。在玩具游戏中,父母对物体的命名较少。在平板电脑游戏中,婴儿在物体和父母之间表现出更多的视线转移。与之前的研究相反,这些研究结果表明,与玩具相比,共同玩平板电脑的互动性更强,这也让人们对 "完全不应该让婴儿接触此类技术 "的建议产生了疑问。
{"title":"Touchscreens can promote infant object-interlocutor reference switching","authors":"Kimberley M. Hudspeth,&nbsp;Charlie Lewis","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101914","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We re-examine whether the type of object played with influences parent-infant joint attention. A within-participants comparison of 24 parent-9-month-old dyads, used head-mounted eye-tracking to measure parental naming and infant attention during play with touchscreen apps on a touchscreen tablet or matched interactive toys. Infants engaged in sustained attention more to the toy than the tablet. Parents named objects less in toy play. Infants exhibited more gaze shifts between the object and their parent during tablet play. Contrasting previous studies, these findings suggest that joint tablet play can be more interactive than with toys, and raise questions about the recommendation that infants should not be exposed at all to such technology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101914"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138549703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It takes a village: Caregiver diversity and language contingency in the UK and rural Gambia 需要一个村庄英国和冈比亚农村地区护理人员的多样性和语言应急能力
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913
Laura Katus , Maria M. Crespo-Llado , Bosiljka Milosavljevic , Mariama Saidykhan , Omar Njie , Tijan Fadera , Samantha McCann , Lena Acolatse , Marta Perapoch Amadó , Maria Rozhko , Sophie E. Moore , Clare E. Elwell , Sarah Lloyd-Fox , The BRIGHT Project Team

Introduction

There is substantial diversity within and between contexts globally in caregiving practices and family composition, which may have implications for the early interaction’s infants engage in. We draw on data from the [blinded] project, which longitudinally examined infants in the UK and in rural Gambia, West Africa. In The Gambia, households are commonly characterized by multigenerational, frequently polygamous family structures, which, in part, is reflected in the diversity of caregivers a child spends time with. In this paper, we aim to 1) evaluate and validate the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) for use in the Mandinka speaking families in The Gambia, 2) examine the nature (i.e., prevalence of turn taking) and amount (i.e., adult and child vocalizations) of conversation that infants are exposed to from 12 to 24 months of age and 3) investigate the link between caregiver diversity and child language outcomes, examining the mediating role of contingent turn taking.

Method

We obtained naturalistic seven-hour-long LENA recordings at 12, 18 and 24 months of age from a cohort of N = 204 infants from Mandinka speaking households in The Gambia and N = 61 infants in the UK. We examined developmental changes and site differences in LENA counts of adult word counts (AWC), contingent turn taking (CTT) and child vocalizations (CVC). In the larger and more heterogenous Gambian sample, we also investigated caregiver predictors of turn taking frequency. We hereby examined the number of caregivers present over the recording day and the consistency of caregivers across two subsequent days per age point. We controlled for children’s cognitive development via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL).

Results

Our LENA validation showed high internal consistency between the human coders and automated LENA outputs (Cronbach’s alpha’s all >.8). All LENA counts were higher in the UK compared to the Gambian cohort. In The Gambia, controlling for overall neurodevelopment via the MSEL, CTT at 12 and 18 months predicted CVC at 18 and 24 months. Caregiver consistency was associated with CTT counts at 18 and 24 months. The number of caregivers and CTT counts showed an inverted u-shape relationship at 18 and 24 months, with an intermediate number of caregivers being associated with the highest CTT frequencies. Mediation analyses showed a partial mediation by number of caregivers and CTT and 24-month CVC.

Discussion

The LENA provided reliable estimates for the Mandinka language in the home recording context. We showed that turn taking is associated with subsequent child vocalizations and explored contextual caregiving factors contributing to turn taking in the Gambian cohort.

导言:在全球范围内,不同环境下的照顾方式和家庭组成存在很大差异,这可能会对婴儿参与的早期互动产生影响。我们借鉴了[盲法]项目的数据,该项目对英国和西非冈比亚农村地区的婴儿进行了纵向研究。在冈比亚,多代同堂、一夫多妻制的家庭结构是常见的家庭结构特征,这在一定程度上反映在与孩子相处的照顾者的多样性上。在本文中,我们的目标是:1)评估和验证在冈比亚曼丁卡语家庭中使用的语言环境分析(LENA);2)研究谈话的性质(即轮流发言的普遍性)和数量(即成人和儿童的发声)、我们从冈比亚曼丁卡语家庭的 N = 204 名婴儿和英国的 N = 61 名婴儿中获得了 12、18 和 24 个月大时长达 7 小时的 LENA 自然录音。我们研究了成人词数 (AWC)、应急轮流 (CTT) 和儿童发声 (CVC) 的 LENA 计数的发展变化和地点差异。在规模更大、异质性更强的冈比亚样本中,我们还调查了轮流发言频率的预测因素。在此,我们考察了记录当天在场的照料者人数,以及每个年龄点的照料者在随后两天中的一致性。我们通过穆伦早期学习量表(MSEL)对儿童的认知发展进行了控制。结果我们的 LENA 验证表明,人工编码者和自动 LENA 输出之间具有很高的内部一致性(Cronbach's alpha's all >.8)。与冈比亚队列相比,英国的所有 LENA 计数都更高。在冈比亚,在通过 MSEL 控制整体神经发育的情况下,12 个月和 18 个月的 CTT 预测了 18 个月和 24 个月的 CVC。照护者的一致性与 18 和 24 个月时的 CTT 计数有关。在 18 和 24 个月时,照顾者人数与 CTT 计数呈倒 U 型关系,照顾者人数居中与 CTT 频率最高相关。中介分析表明,照顾者人数和 CTT 与 24 个月的 CVC 存在部分中介关系。我们的研究表明,轮流发声与儿童随后的发声有关,并探讨了在冈比亚队列中导致轮流发声的护理因素。
{"title":"It takes a village: Caregiver diversity and language contingency in the UK and rural Gambia","authors":"Laura Katus ,&nbsp;Maria M. Crespo-Llado ,&nbsp;Bosiljka Milosavljevic ,&nbsp;Mariama Saidykhan ,&nbsp;Omar Njie ,&nbsp;Tijan Fadera ,&nbsp;Samantha McCann ,&nbsp;Lena Acolatse ,&nbsp;Marta Perapoch Amadó ,&nbsp;Maria Rozhko ,&nbsp;Sophie E. Moore ,&nbsp;Clare E. Elwell ,&nbsp;Sarah Lloyd-Fox ,&nbsp;The BRIGHT Project Team","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101913","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>There is substantial diversity within and between contexts globally in caregiving practices and family composition, which may have implications for the early interaction’s infants engage in. We draw on data from the [blinded] project, which longitudinally examined infants in the UK and in rural Gambia, West Africa. In The Gambia, households are commonly characterized by multigenerational, frequently polygamous family structures, which, in part, is reflected in the diversity of caregivers a child spends time with. In this paper, we aim to 1) evaluate and validate the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) for use in the Mandinka speaking families in The Gambia, 2) examine the nature (i.e., prevalence of turn taking) and amount (i.e., adult and child vocalizations) of conversation that infants are exposed to from 12 to 24 months of age and 3) investigate the link between caregiver diversity and child language outcomes, examining the mediating role of contingent turn taking.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>We obtained naturalistic seven-hour-long LENA recordings at 12, 18 and 24 months of age from a cohort of N = 204 infants from Mandinka speaking households in The Gambia and N = 61 infants in the UK. We examined developmental changes and site differences in LENA counts of adult word counts (AWC), contingent turn taking (CTT) and child vocalizations (CVC). In the larger and more heterogenous Gambian sample, we also investigated caregiver predictors of turn taking frequency. We hereby examined the number of caregivers present over the recording day and the consistency of caregivers across two subsequent days per age point. We controlled for children’s cognitive development via the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Our LENA validation showed high internal consistency between the human coders and automated LENA outputs (Cronbach’s alpha’s all &gt;.8). All LENA counts were higher in the UK compared to the Gambian cohort. In The Gambia, controlling for overall neurodevelopment via the MSEL, CTT at 12 and 18 months predicted CVC at 18 and 24 months. Caregiver consistency was associated with CTT counts at 18 and 24 months. The number of caregivers and CTT counts showed an inverted u-shape relationship at 18 and 24 months, with an intermediate number of caregivers being associated with the highest CTT frequencies. Mediation analyses showed a partial mediation by number of caregivers and CTT and 24-month CVC.</p></div><div><h3>Discussion</h3><p>The LENA provided reliable estimates for the Mandinka language in the home recording context. We showed that turn taking is associated with subsequent child vocalizations and explored contextual caregiving factors contributing to turn taking in the Gambian cohort.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101913"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001054/pdfft?md5=6d39869a5d18eb72a123a4c5f85c87dd&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001054-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138490834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Referent-oriented interactions in infancy: A naturalistic, longitudinal case study from an English-speaking household 婴儿期指涉互动:来自英语家庭的自然主义纵向案例研究
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101911
Erica H. Wojcik, Meghan C. Pierce, Gracie Stevens, Sarah J. Goulding

Caregivers use a of combination labeling, pointing, object grasping, and gaze to communicate with infants about referents in their environment. By two years of age, children reliably use these referent-oriented cues to communicate and learn. While there is some evidence from lab-based studies that younger infants attend to and use referent-oriented cues during communication, some more naturalistic studies have found that in the first year of life, infants do not robustly leverage these cues during dyadic interactions. The current study examined parent and infant gaze, touching, pointing, and reaching to referents for a wide range of nouns, verbs, adjectives, and other early-learned words during 59 one-hour head-camera recordings sampled from one English-learning infants’ life between 6 and 12 months of age. We found substantial variability across individual words for all cues. Some variability was explained by referent concreteness and the grammatical category of the label. The parent’s touching of labeled referents increased across months, suggesting that parent-infant-referent interactions may change with development. Future studies should investigate the trajectories of specific types of words and contexts, rather than attempting to discover possibly non-existent universal trajectories of parent and infant referent-oriented behaviors.

照顾者使用组合标记,指向,物体抓取和凝视与婴儿沟通他们的环境中的指涉物。到两岁时,孩子们可以可靠地使用这些指向的线索进行交流和学习。虽然有一些基于实验室的研究证据表明,年幼的婴儿在交流中注意并使用指向指向的线索,但一些更自然的研究发现,在生命的第一年,婴儿在二元互动中并没有充分利用这些线索。目前的研究调查了父母和婴儿的凝视、触摸、指向和伸手去指各种各样的名词、动词、形容词和其他早期学过的单词,在59个一小时的头部摄像机录音中,从一个6到12个月大的英语学习婴儿的生活中取样。我们发现所有线索的单个单词都存在很大的差异。所指的具体性和标签的语法范畴解释了一些变化。父母触摸标记物的次数在几个月内增加,这表明父母与婴儿之间的相互作用可能随着发育而改变。未来的研究应该研究特定类型的词语和语境的轨迹,而不是试图发现可能不存在的父母和婴儿指称导向行为的普遍轨迹。
{"title":"Referent-oriented interactions in infancy: A naturalistic, longitudinal case study from an English-speaking household","authors":"Erica H. Wojcik,&nbsp;Meghan C. Pierce,&nbsp;Gracie Stevens,&nbsp;Sarah J. Goulding","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101911","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101911","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Caregivers use a of combination labeling, pointing, object grasping, and gaze to communicate with infants about referents in their environment. By two years of age, children reliably use these referent-oriented cues to communicate and learn. While there is some evidence from lab-based studies that younger infants attend to and use referent-oriented cues during communication, some more naturalistic studies have found that in the first year of life, infants do not robustly leverage these cues during dyadic interactions. The current study examined parent and infant gaze, touching, pointing, and reaching to referents for a wide range of nouns, verbs, adjectives, and other early-learned words during 59 one-hour head-camera recordings sampled from one English-learning infants’ life between 6 and 12 months of age. We found substantial variability across individual words for all cues. Some variability was explained by referent concreteness and the grammatical category of the label. The parent’s touching of labeled referents increased across months, suggesting that parent-infant-referent interactions may change with development. Future studies should investigate the trajectories of specific types of words and contexts, rather than attempting to discover possibly non-existent universal trajectories of parent and infant referent-oriented behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101911"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001030/pdfft?md5=5dd429b341eda33a5beec36d8271918c&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001030-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138483889","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddler negative affectivity and effortful control: Relations with parent-toddler conversation engagement and indirect effects on language 幼儿负性情感与努力控制:亲子对话参与的关系及其对语言的间接影响。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101912
Margaret A. Fields-Olivieri , Crystal E. Thinzar , Caroline K.P. Roben , Pamela M. Cole

Evidence that early parent-child conversation supports early language development suggests a need to understand factors that account for individual differences in parent-child conversation engagement. Whereas most studies focus on demographic factors, we investigated the role of toddler temperament in a longitudinal study of 120 economically strained families. Specifically, we investigated the degree to which toddlers’ negative affectivity and effortful control, considered together as a composite reflecting challenging temperament, accounted for variability in parent-toddler conversation engagement, and whether the frequency of that engagement mediated associations between toddler temperament and toddler expressive language skills. Toddler challenging temperament (i.e., high negative affectivity and low effortful control) and parent-toddler conversation engagement were measured at 18 and 30 months. Toddler expressive language skills were measured at 18, 24, and 36 months. As expected, a path model indicated inverse relations between toddler challenging temperament and concurrent parent-toddler conversation engagement at both 18 and 30 months. Unexpectedly, there were no direct associations between toddler challenging temperament and toddler expressive language skills either concurrently or longitudinally. However, we found indirect effects of toddler challenging temperament on later toddler expressive language skills via parent-toddler conversation engagement. Findings highlight the importance of considering toddler temperamental characteristics in addition to family demographics as important factors that account for variability in parent-toddler conversation engagement.

早期亲子对话支持早期语言发展的证据表明,有必要了解导致亲子对话参与中个体差异的因素。虽然大多数研究都关注人口因素,但我们对120个经济紧张家庭进行了纵向研究,调查了幼儿气质的作用。具体而言,我们调查了幼儿的负面情感和努力控制在多大程度上反映了父母-幼儿对话参与的可变性,以及这种参与的频率是否介导了幼儿气质和幼儿表达语言技能之间的联系。在18个月和30个月时测量幼儿挑战性气质(即高负性情感和低努力控制)和亲子对话参与。在18个月、24个月和36个月时测量幼儿的语言表达能力。正如预期的那样,路径模型显示幼儿挑战性气质与18个月和30个月的亲子同时对话参与呈负相关。出乎意料的是,幼儿挑战性气质和幼儿表达性语言技能之间没有直接的联系,无论是同时的还是纵向的。然而,我们发现幼儿挑战性气质通过亲子对话参与对后来的幼儿表达语言技能有间接影响。研究结果强调,除了家庭人口统计数据外,考虑幼儿的气质特征也是影响亲子对话参与变化的重要因素。
{"title":"Toddler negative affectivity and effortful control: Relations with parent-toddler conversation engagement and indirect effects on language","authors":"Margaret A. Fields-Olivieri ,&nbsp;Crystal E. Thinzar ,&nbsp;Caroline K.P. Roben ,&nbsp;Pamela M. Cole","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101912","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101912","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Evidence that early parent-child conversation supports early language development suggests a need to understand factors that account for individual differences in parent-child conversation engagement. Whereas most studies focus on demographic factors, we investigated the role of toddler temperament in a longitudinal study of 120 economically strained families. Specifically, we investigated the degree to which toddlers’ negative affectivity and effortful control, considered together as a composite reflecting <em>challenging temperament</em>, accounted for variability in parent-toddler conversation engagement, and whether the frequency of that engagement mediated associations between toddler temperament and toddler expressive language skills. Toddler challenging temperament (i.e., high negative affectivity and low effortful control) and parent-toddler conversation engagement were measured at 18 and 30 months. Toddler expressive language skills were measured at 18, 24, and 36 months. As expected, a path model indicated inverse relations between toddler challenging temperament and concurrent parent-toddler conversation engagement at both 18 and 30 months. Unexpectedly, there were no direct associations between toddler challenging temperament and toddler expressive language skills either concurrently or longitudinally. However, we found indirect effects of toddler challenging temperament on later toddler expressive language skills via parent-toddler conversation engagement. Findings highlight the importance of considering toddler temperamental characteristics in addition to family demographics as important factors that account for variability in parent-toddler conversation engagement.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101912"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138479854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A new online paradigm to measure spontaneous pointing in infants and caregivers 一个新的在线范式来测量婴儿和照顾者的自发指向
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101907
Katharina Kaletsch, Ulf Liszkowski

Index-finger pointing is a milestone in the development of referential communication. Previous research has investigated infants’ pointing with a variety of paradigms ranging from parent reports to field observations to experimental settings, suggesting that lab-based semi-natural interactional settings seem especially suited to elicit and measure infant pointing. With the Covid-pandemic the need for a comparable online tool became evident enabling also efficient, low-cost, large-scale, diverse data collection. The current study introduces a remote online paradigm, based on the established live ‘decorated-room’ paradigm. In Experiment 1, 12-months old infants and their caregivers (N = 24) looked at digitally presented stimuli together while being recorded with their webcam. We coded pointing gestures of infants and caregivers as well as caregivers’ responses to infants’ pointing. In Experiment 2 (N = 47), we optimized stimuli and investigated influences of stimulus characteristics. We systematically varied the style of depiction, stimulus complexity, motion, and facial stimuli. Main findings were that infants and caregivers pointed spontaneously, with mean behaviors ranging within the benchmarks of previously reported findings of the live decorated-room paradigm. Further, the social setting was preserved as revealed by significant relations between parents’ responsive points and infants’ pointing frequency. Analyses of stimuli characteristics revealed that infants pointed more to stimuli depicting faces than to other stimuli. The new remote online paradigm proves a useful addition to established paradigms. It offers novel opportunities for simplified assessments, large-scale sampling, and worldwide, diversified data collection.

指指点点是指称交流发展的一个里程碑。先前的研究用各种范式调查了婴儿的指向行为,从父母报告到实地观察再到实验环境,表明基于实验室的半自然互动环境似乎特别适合引发和测量婴儿指向行为。随着covid - 19大流行,对类似在线工具的需求变得明显,从而能够高效、低成本、大规模、多样化地收集数据。目前的研究引入了一种远程在线模式,以现有的现场“装饰房间”模式为基础。在实验1中,12个月大的婴儿和他们的看护人(N = 24)一起观看数字呈现的刺激,同时用他们的网络摄像头记录下来。我们对婴儿和照顾者的手势以及照顾者对婴儿的手势的反应进行编码。在实验2 (N = 47)中,我们对刺激进行了优化,并考察了刺激特征的影响。我们系统地改变了描绘的风格、刺激的复杂性、运动和面部刺激。主要发现是婴儿和照顾者自发地指出,其平均行为范围在先前报道的现场装饰房间范式的研究结果的基准范围内。此外,父母的反应点与婴儿的指指频率之间存在显著的关系,这表明社会环境得到了保留。对刺激特征的分析显示,婴儿更多地指向描绘人脸的刺激,而不是其他刺激。新的远程在线范例被证明是对已建立范例的有用补充。它为简化评估、大规模抽样和全球多样化的数据收集提供了新的机会。
{"title":"A new online paradigm to measure spontaneous pointing in infants and caregivers","authors":"Katharina Kaletsch,&nbsp;Ulf Liszkowski","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101907","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101907","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Index-finger pointing is a milestone in the development of referential communication. Previous research has investigated infants’ pointing with a variety of paradigms ranging from parent reports to field observations to experimental settings, suggesting that lab-based semi-natural interactional settings seem especially suited to elicit and measure infant pointing. With the Covid-pandemic the need for a comparable online tool became evident enabling also efficient, low-cost, large-scale, diverse data collection. The current study introduces a remote online paradigm, based on the established live ‘decorated-room’ paradigm. In Experiment 1, 12-months old infants and their caregivers (N = 24) looked at digitally presented stimuli together while being recorded with their webcam. We coded pointing gestures of infants and caregivers as well as caregivers’ responses to infants’ pointing. In Experiment 2 (N = 47), we optimized stimuli and investigated influences of stimulus characteristics. We systematically varied the style of depiction, stimulus complexity, motion, and facial stimuli. Main findings were that infants and caregivers pointed spontaneously, with mean behaviors ranging within the benchmarks of previously reported findings of the live decorated-room paradigm. Further, the social setting was preserved as revealed by significant relations between parents’ responsive points and infants’ pointing frequency. Analyses of stimuli characteristics revealed that infants pointed more to stimuli depicting faces than to other stimuli. The new remote online paradigm proves a useful addition to established paradigms. It offers novel opportunities for simplified assessments, large-scale sampling, and worldwide, diversified data collection.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101907"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138437856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of acoustic features of maternal infant-directed singing in enhancing infant sensorimotor, language and socioemotional development 母婴定向歌唱的声学特征在促进婴儿感觉运动、语言和社会情感发展中的作用。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101908
Raija-Leena Punamäki , Safwat Y. Diab , Konstantinos Drosos , Samir R. Qouta , Mervi Vänskä

The quality of infant-directed speech (IDS) and infant-directed singing (IDSi) are considered vital to children, but empirical studies on protomusical qualities of the IDSi influencing infant development are rare. The current prospective study examines the role of IDSi acoustic features, such as pitch variability, shape and movement, and vocal amplitude vibration, timbre, and resonance, in associating with infant sensorimotor, language, and socioemotional development at six and 18 months. The sample consists of 236 Palestinian mothers from Gaza Strip singing to their six-month-olds a song by their own choice. Maternal IDSi was recorded and analyzed by the OpenSMILE- tool to depict main acoustic features of pitch frequencies, variations, and contours, vocal intensity, resonance formants, and power. The results are based on completed 219 maternal IDSi. Mothers reported about their infants’ sensorimotor, language-vocalization, and socioemotional skills at six months, and psychologists tested these skills by Bayley Scales for Infant Development at 18 months. Results show that maternal IDSi characterized by wide pitch variability and rich and high vocal amplitude and vibration were associated with infants’ optimal sensorimotor, language vocalization, and socioemotional skills at six months, and rich and high vocal amplitude and vibration predicted these optimal developmental skills also at 18 months. High resonance and rhythmicity formants were associated with optimal language and vocalization skills at six months. To conclude, the IDSi is considered important in enhancing newborn and risk infants’ wellbeing, and the current findings argue that favorable acoustic singing qualities are crucial for optimal multidomain development across infancy.

婴儿定向语(IDS)和婴儿定向歌唱(IDSi)的质量被认为对儿童至关重要,但关于婴儿定向语的原音乐质量影响婴儿发育的实证研究很少。当前的前瞻性研究考察了IDSi声学特征的作用,如音高变异性、形状和运动、声音振幅振动、音色和共振,与婴儿6个月和18个月的感觉运动、语言和社会情感发展有关。样本由来自加沙地带的236名巴勒斯坦母亲组成,她们给六个月大的孩子唱一首自己选择的歌。通过OpenSMILE工具记录和分析母体的IDSi,以描绘音调频率、变化和轮廓、声音强度、共振共振峰和功率的主要声学特征。结果基于完成的219例母体IDSi。母亲们在婴儿6个月大的时候报告了他们的感觉运动、语言发声和社会情感技能,心理学家在婴儿18个月大的时候通过贝利·斯卡尔斯对这些技能进行了测试。结果表明,音高变幅大、声音振幅和振动高与婴儿6月龄时的最佳感觉运动、语言发声和社会情感技能相关,而声音振幅和振动高与婴儿18月龄时的最佳发展技能相关。高共振和韵律共振峰与六个月时的最佳语言和发声技能有关。综上所述,IDSi在提高新生儿和风险婴儿的健康方面被认为是重要的,目前的研究结果表明,良好的声学歌唱质量对于婴儿期的最佳多领域发展至关重要。
{"title":"The role of acoustic features of maternal infant-directed singing in enhancing infant sensorimotor, language and socioemotional development","authors":"Raija-Leena Punamäki ,&nbsp;Safwat Y. Diab ,&nbsp;Konstantinos Drosos ,&nbsp;Samir R. Qouta ,&nbsp;Mervi Vänskä","doi":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101908","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101908","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The quality of infant-directed speech (IDS) and infant-directed singing (IDSi) are considered vital to children, but empirical studies on protomusical qualities of the IDSi influencing infant development are rare. The current prospective study examines the role of IDSi acoustic features, such as pitch variability, shape and movement, and vocal amplitude vibration, timbre, and resonance, in associating with infant sensorimotor, language, and socioemotional development at six and 18 months. The sample consists of 236 Palestinian mothers from Gaza Strip singing to their six-month-olds a song by their own choice. Maternal IDSi was recorded and analyzed by the OpenSMILE- tool to depict main acoustic features of pitch frequencies, variations, and contours, vocal intensity, resonance formants, and power. The results are based on completed 219 maternal IDSi. Mothers reported about their infants’ sensorimotor, language-vocalization, and socioemotional skills at six months, and psychologists tested these skills by Bayley Scales for Infant Development at 18 months. Results show that maternal IDSi characterized by wide pitch variability and rich and high vocal amplitude and vibration were associated with infants’ optimal sensorimotor, language vocalization, and socioemotional skills at six months, and rich and high vocal amplitude and vibration predicted these optimal developmental skills also at 18 months. High resonance and rhythmicity formants were associated with optimal language and vocalization skills at six months. To conclude, the IDSi is considered important in enhancing newborn and risk infants’ wellbeing, and the current findings argue that favorable acoustic singing qualities are crucial for optimal multidomain development across infancy.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48222,"journal":{"name":"Infant Behavior & Development","volume":"74 ","pages":"Article 101908"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0163638323001005/pdfft?md5=8ba2488e03e838fdcbd82f3c4d01737b&pid=1-s2.0-S0163638323001005-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138296864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant Behavior & Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1