首页 > 最新文献

Educational Psychology Review最新文献

英文 中文
The Association Between Parent–Child Attachment and Academic Adjustment: A Multilevel Meta-Analysis 亲子依恋与学业适应之间的关系:多层次元分析
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09920-y
Yan Yang, Song Li, Fang Xie, Xu Chen

Academic adjustment is an important task for students, traditionally encompassing three major constructs: academic motivation (e.g., academic self-efficacy), engagement (e.g., persistence with schoolwork), and achievement (e.g., grade point average). Although theoretical links between parent–child attachment and academic adjustment have been proposed, comprehensive meta-analyses investigating this relationship are lacking. To achieve a more thorough understanding, we incorporated data from children and adolescents, considering both categorical and continuous attachment measures. Through a series of multilevel meta-analyses, we examined the magnitude of the relationship between parent–child attachment and academic achievement, motivation, and engagement. We retrieved 178 effect sizes from 45 studies assessing 47 independent samples (53,619 students) through a systematic literature search. The key findings are as follows: attachment security was positively related to academic achievement (r = .132), while attachment insecurity, whether avoidant (r = − .154) or anxious (r = − .081) attachment, was negatively associated with academic achievement. Moreover, attachment security was positively related to motivation (r = .161) and engagement (r = .229). Notably, the type of attachment measures moderated the association between attachment security and academic achievement. In addition, gender was found to moderate the association between attachment security and academic motivation. Our findings are crucial for understanding the association between parent–child attachment and academic adjustment.

学业适应是学生的一项重要任务,传统上包括三个主要方面:学业动机(如学业自我效能)、参与(如坚持完成学业)和成绩(如平均学分)。尽管有人提出了亲子依恋与学业适应之间的理论联系,但缺乏对这种关系进行全面调查的荟萃分析。为了获得更透彻的理解,我们纳入了儿童和青少年的数据,并考虑了分类和连续的依恋测量。通过一系列多层次荟萃分析,我们研究了亲子依恋与学业成绩、学习动机和参与度之间的关系。通过系统的文献检索,我们从评估 47 个独立样本(53619 名学生)的 45 项研究中检索到 178 个效应大小。主要发现如下:依恋安全感与学业成绩呈正相关(r = .132),而依恋不安全感,无论是回避型依恋(r = - .154)还是焦虑型依恋(r = - .081),都与学业成绩呈负相关。此外,依恋安全感与学习动机(r = .161)和参与度(r = .229)呈正相关。值得注意的是,依恋测量的类型调节了依恋安全感与学习成绩之间的关系。此外,性别也对依恋安全感与学习动机之间的关联有调节作用。我们的研究结果对于理解亲子依恋与学业适应之间的关系至关重要。
{"title":"The Association Between Parent–Child Attachment and Academic Adjustment: A Multilevel Meta-Analysis","authors":"Yan Yang, Song Li, Fang Xie, Xu Chen","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09920-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09920-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Academic adjustment is an important task for students, traditionally encompassing three major constructs: academic motivation (e.g., academic self-efficacy), engagement (e.g., persistence with schoolwork), and achievement (e.g., grade point average). Although theoretical links between parent–child attachment and academic adjustment have been proposed, comprehensive meta-analyses investigating this relationship are lacking. To achieve a more thorough understanding, we incorporated data from children and adolescents, considering both categorical and continuous attachment measures. Through a series of multilevel meta-analyses, we examined the magnitude of the relationship between parent–child attachment and academic achievement, motivation, and engagement. We retrieved 178 effect sizes from 45 studies assessing 47 independent samples (53,619 students) through a systematic literature search. The key findings are as follows: attachment security was positively related to academic achievement (<i>r</i> = .132), while attachment insecurity, whether avoidant (<i>r</i> = − .154) or anxious (<i>r</i> = − .081) attachment, was negatively associated with academic achievement. Moreover, attachment security was positively related to motivation (<i>r</i> = .161) and engagement (<i>r</i> = .229). Notably, the type of attachment measures moderated the association between attachment security and academic achievement. In addition, gender was found to moderate the association between attachment security and academic motivation. Our findings are crucial for understanding the association between parent–child attachment and academic adjustment.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"376 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141974311","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Meta-Analysis on Teachers’ Growth Mindset 关于教师成长心态的元分析
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09925-7
Lisa Bardach, Keiko C. P. Bostwick, Tim Fütterer, Myriel Kopatz, Daniel Memarpour Hobbi, Robert M. Klassen, Jakob Pietschnig

The concept of growth mindset—an individual’s beliefs that basic characteristics such as intelligence are malleable—has gained immense popularity in research, the media, and educational practice. Even though it is assumed that teachers need a growth mindset and that both teachers and their students benefit when teachers adopt a growth mindset, systematic syntheses of the potential advantages of a growth mindset in teachers are lacking. Therefore, in this article, we present the first meta-analysis on teachers’ growth mindset and its relationships with multiple outcomes (50 studies, 81 effect sizes; N = 19,555). Multilevel analyses showed a small effect across outcomes. Statistically significant small-to-typical positive associations between teachers’ growth mindset and their motivation in terms of self-efficacy and mastery goals were observed in subgroup analyses. No statistically significant relationships were found with teachers’ performance-approach goals, teachers’ performance-avoidance goals, teachers’ performance on achievement tests, or student achievement. Teachers’ growth mindset was related to instructional practices in terms of mastery goal structures but unrelated to performance goal structures. Moderator analyses indicated that the dimensionality of the mindset measure (recoded from a fixed mindset to a growth mindset measure vs. assessed as a growth mindset), item referent and content of the mindset measure, publication status (published vs. unpublished), world region, educational level, and study quality influenced the strengths of some of the relationships. Overall, our findings extend knowledge about teachers’ mindset and add to the evidence base on teacher characteristics and their links to relevant outcomes.

成长型思维模式的概念--个人认为智力等基本特征是可塑的--在研究、媒体和教育实践中广受欢迎。尽管人们认为教师需要成长型思维模式,而且教师采用成长型思维模式后,教师和学生都会受益,但目前还缺乏对教师成长型思维模式潜在优势的系统总结。因此,我们在本文中首次对教师的成长型思维模式及其与多种结果的关系进行了荟萃分析(50 项研究,81 个效应大小;N=19,555)。多层次分析表明,不同结果之间的影响较小。在分组分析中观察到,教师的成长心态与他们的自我效能感和掌握目标动机之间存在统计学意义上的小到典型的正相关。在统计意义上,教师的成长心态与教师的绩效目标、绩效回避目标、教师在成绩测试中的表现以及学生的成绩都没有明显的关系。在掌握目标结构方面,教师的成长心态与教学实践有关,但与绩效目标结构无关。调节因素分析表明,思维模式测量的维度(从固定思维模式重新编码为成长思维模式测量与作为成长思维模式评估)、思维模式测量的项目参照和内容、出版状态(已出版与未出版)、世界地区、教育水平和研究质量影响了某些关系的强度。总之,我们的研究结果扩展了有关教师心态的知识,并增加了教师特征及其与相关结果之间联系的证据基础。
{"title":"A Meta-Analysis on Teachers’ Growth Mindset","authors":"Lisa Bardach, Keiko C. P. Bostwick, Tim Fütterer, Myriel Kopatz, Daniel Memarpour Hobbi, Robert M. Klassen, Jakob Pietschnig","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09925-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09925-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The concept of growth mindset—an individual’s beliefs that basic characteristics such as intelligence are malleable—has gained immense popularity in research, the media, and educational practice. Even though it is assumed that teachers need a growth mindset and that both teachers and their students benefit when teachers adopt a growth mindset, systematic syntheses of the potential advantages of a growth mindset in teachers are lacking. Therefore, in this article, we present the first meta-analysis on teachers’ growth mindset and its relationships with multiple outcomes (50 studies, 81 effect sizes; <i>N</i> = 19,555). Multilevel analyses showed a small effect across outcomes. Statistically significant small-to-typical positive associations between teachers’ growth mindset and their motivation in terms of self-efficacy and mastery goals were observed in subgroup analyses. No statistically significant relationships were found with teachers’ performance-approach goals, teachers’ performance-avoidance goals, teachers’ performance on achievement tests, or student achievement. Teachers’ growth mindset was related to instructional practices in terms of mastery goal structures but unrelated to performance goal structures. Moderator analyses indicated that the dimensionality of the mindset measure (recoded from a fixed mindset to a growth mindset measure vs. assessed as a growth mindset), item referent and content of the mindset measure, publication status (published vs. unpublished), world region, educational level, and study quality influenced the strengths of some of the relationships. Overall, our findings extend knowledge about teachers’ mindset and add to the evidence base on teacher characteristics and their links to relevant outcomes.\u0000</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141904510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Control-Value Theory: From Achievement Emotion to a General Theory of Human Emotions 控制-价值理论:从成就情绪到人类情绪的一般理论
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09909-7
Reinhard Pekrun

In its original version, control-value theory describes and explains achievement emotions. More recently, the theory has been expanded to also explain epistemic, social, and existential emotions. In this article, I outline the development of the theory, from preliminary work in the 1980s to early versions of the theory and the recent generalized control-value theory. I provide summaries of the theory’s evidence-based propositions on antecedents, outcomes, and regulation of emotions, including the fundamentally important role of control and value appraisals across different types of human emotions that are relevant to education (and beyond). The theory includes descriptive taxonomies of emotions as well as propositions explaining (a) the influence of individual factors, social environments, and socio-cultural contexts on emotions; (b) the effects of emotions on learning, performance, and health; (c) reciprocal causation linking emotions, outcomes, and antecedents; (d) ways to regulate emotions; and (e) strategies for intervention. Subsequently, I outline the relevance of the theory for educational practice, including individual and large-scale assessments of emotions; students’, teachers’, and parents’ understanding of emotions; and change of educational practices. In conclusion, I discuss strengths of the theory, open questions, and future directions.

在最初的版本中,控制价值理论描述并解释了成就情绪。最近,该理论被扩展到也可以解释认识情绪、社会情绪和存在情绪。在本文中,我概述了该理论的发展历程,从 20 世纪 80 年代的初步工作到该理论的早期版本,再到最近的广义控制价值理论。我总结了该理论关于情绪的前因、结果和调节的循证命题,包括控制和价值评估在与教育(及其他)相关的不同类型人类情绪中的重要作用。该理论包括情绪的描述性分类法以及解释以下方面的命题:(a) 个人因素、社会环境和社会文化背景对情绪的影响;(b) 情绪对学习、表现和健康的影响;(c) 情绪、结果和前因之间的相互因果关系;(d) 调节情绪的方法;以及 (e) 干预策略。随后,我概述了该理论与教育实践的相关性,包括个人和大规模的情绪评估;学生、教师和家长对情绪的理解;以及教育实践的改变。最后,我讨论了该理论的优势、开放性问题和未来方向。
{"title":"Control-Value Theory: From Achievement Emotion to a General Theory of Human Emotions","authors":"Reinhard Pekrun","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09909-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09909-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In its original version, control-value theory describes and explains achievement emotions. More recently, the theory has been expanded to also explain epistemic, social, and existential emotions. In this article, I outline the development of the theory, from preliminary work in the 1980s to early versions of the theory and the recent generalized control-value theory. I provide summaries of the theory’s evidence-based propositions on antecedents, outcomes, and regulation of emotions, including the fundamentally important role of control and value appraisals across different types of human emotions that are relevant to education (and beyond). The theory includes descriptive taxonomies of emotions as well as propositions explaining (a) the influence of individual factors, social environments, and socio-cultural contexts on emotions; (b) the effects of emotions on learning, performance, and health; (c) reciprocal causation linking emotions, outcomes, and antecedents; (d) ways to regulate emotions; and (e) strategies for intervention. Subsequently, I outline the relevance of the theory for educational practice, including individual and large-scale assessments of emotions; students’, teachers’, and parents’ understanding of emotions; and change of educational practices. In conclusion, I discuss strengths of the theory, open questions, and future directions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"73 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141877581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ten Years of Dimensional Comparison Theory: On the Development of a Theory from Educational Psychology 维度比较理论十年:论教育心理学理论的发展
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09918-6
Jens Möller

Dimensional comparison theory (DCT; Möller & Marsh 2013:Psychological Review, 120(3), 544–560), first formulated 10 years ago, describes individuals’ internal comparison processes applied between different areas of their lives. Dimensional comparisons explain the seemingly counterintuitive phenomenon that students’ verbal and mathematical self-concepts are almost uncorrelated, even though mathematical and verbal performances correlate highly positively: When students compare their performance in verbal and mathematical domains, dimensional comparisons lead to a contrast effect: students overestimate their ability in the intra-individually better domain and underestimate their ability in their intra-individually weaker domain, leading to near-zero correlations between the respective self-concepts.

This paper describes diverse extensions of the classic DCT into a variety of predictors (especially various school subjects, beyond math and native language) and criteria (especially task value components, in addition to subject-specific self-concepts) and extensions of the applicability of the DCT as an educational psychological theory to contexts such as clinical and health psychology. Recent findings on the psychological processes that trigger, accompany, and follow dimensional comparisons are summarized, before an overview of different methods to capture dimensional comparisons is given, and further research directions are discussed.

维度比较理论(Dimensional comparison theory,DCT;Möller & Marsh 2013:Psychological Review,120(3),544-560)于 10 年前首次提出,描述了个人在不同生活领域之间的内部比较过程。维度比较解释了一个看似违反直觉的现象,即学生的语言和数学自我概念几乎不相关,尽管数学和语言成绩高度正相关:当学生比较他们在语言和数学领域的表现时,维度比较会导致一种对比效应:学生会高估他们在个体内部较好领域的能力,而低估他们在个体内部较弱领域的能力,从而导致各自自我概念之间的相关性接近于零。本文介绍了经典 DCT 在各种预测因素(尤其是数学和母语之外的各种学校科目)和标准(尤其是任务价值成分,以及特定科目的自我概念)方面的不同扩展,以及 DCT 作为教育心理学理论在临床和健康心理学等领域的适用性扩展。在概述捕捉维度比较的不同方法之前,还总结了有关引发、伴随和跟随维度比较的心理过程的最新研究成果,并讨论了进一步的研究方向。
{"title":"Ten Years of Dimensional Comparison Theory: On the Development of a Theory from Educational Psychology","authors":"Jens Möller","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09918-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09918-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dimensional comparison theory (DCT; Möller &amp; Marsh 2013:<i>Psychological Review, 120</i>(3), 544–560), first formulated 10 years ago, describes individuals’ internal comparison processes applied between different areas of their lives. Dimensional comparisons explain the seemingly counterintuitive phenomenon that students’ verbal and mathematical self-concepts are almost uncorrelated, even though mathematical and verbal performances correlate highly positively: When students compare their performance in verbal and mathematical domains, dimensional comparisons lead to a contrast effect: students overestimate their ability in the intra-individually better domain and underestimate their ability in their intra-individually weaker domain, leading to near-zero correlations between the respective self-concepts.</p><p>This paper describes diverse extensions of the classic DCT into a variety of predictors (especially various school subjects, beyond math and native language) and criteria (especially task value components, in addition to subject-specific self-concepts) and extensions of the applicability of the DCT as an educational psychological theory to contexts such as clinical and health psychology. Recent findings on the psychological processes that trigger, accompany, and follow dimensional comparisons are summarized, before an overview of different methods to capture dimensional comparisons is given, and further research directions are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141857886","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The association between initial metacognition and subsequent academic achievement: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies 初始元认知与后续学业成绩之间的关系:纵向研究的元分析
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09922-w
Guohao He, Songshan Chen, Hongyi Lin, Aoxue Su

In the present meta-analysis, we systematically examined the association between students’ initial level of metacognition and their academic achievement at least three months later. Using multilevel meta-analysis as well as meta-analytic structural equation modelling, we analysed data from 71,171 students provided by 28 independent studies. The findings indicated a positive relationship between initial metacognition and subsequent academic achievement (r = .22, 95% CI = [0.18, 0.33], p < .001). Meanwhile, age, gender, time lag, educational stage, culture, and the composition and measurement of metacognition were considered as potential moderating variables. Moreover, while previous research has typically viewed high levels of academic achievement as a consequence of high levels of metacognition, the self-determination theory (SDT) suggests that high levels of academic achievement may also be an antecedent of high levels of metacognition. Therefore, we conducted cross-lagged panel analyses, and after accounting for autoregressive effects, the results showed that students’ initial academic achievement was also a significant positive predictor of subsequent metacognitive levels. Finally, theoretical and practical implications are discussed.

在本荟萃分析中,我们系统地研究了学生最初的元认知水平与他们至少三个月后的学业成绩之间的关联。我们采用多层次元分析和元分析结构方程模型,分析了 28 项独立研究提供的 71,171 名学生的数据。结果表明,初始元认知与后续学业成绩之间存在正相关关系(r = .22, 95% CI = [0.18, 0.33], p <.001)。同时,年龄、性别、时滞、教育阶段、文化以及元认知的构成和测量都被视为潜在的调节变量。此外,尽管以往的研究通常将高水平的学业成绩视为高水平元认知的结果,但自我决定理论(SDT)认为,高水平的学业成绩也可能是高水平元认知的前因。因此,我们进行了交叉滞后面板分析,在考虑了自回归效应后,结果表明学生最初的学业成绩也是后续元认知水平的显著正向预测因素。最后,我们讨论了理论和实践意义。
{"title":"The association between initial metacognition and subsequent academic achievement: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies","authors":"Guohao He, Songshan Chen, Hongyi Lin, Aoxue Su","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09922-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09922-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the present meta-analysis, we systematically examined the association between students’ initial level of metacognition and their academic achievement at least three months later. Using multilevel meta-analysis as well as meta-analytic structural equation modelling, we analysed data from 71,171 students provided by 28 independent studies. The findings indicated a positive relationship between initial metacognition and subsequent academic achievement (<i>r</i> = .22, 95% CI = [0.18, 0.33], <i>p</i> &lt; .001). Meanwhile, age, gender, time lag, educational stage, culture, and the composition and measurement of metacognition were considered as potential moderating variables. Moreover, while previous research has typically viewed high levels of academic achievement as a consequence of high levels of metacognition, the self-determination theory (SDT) suggests that high levels of academic achievement may also be an antecedent of high levels of metacognition. Therefore, we conducted cross-lagged panel analyses, and after accounting for autoregressive effects, the results showed that students’ initial academic achievement was also a significant positive predictor of subsequent metacognitive levels. Finally, theoretical and practical implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"45 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141857846","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of Playback Speed and Distractions on the Comprehension of Audio and Audio-Visual Materials 播放速度和干扰因素对理解视听材料的影响
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09917-7
Ashley Chen, Suchita E. Kumar, Rhea Varkhedi, Dillon H. Murphy

In the modern age, we often consume content at faster than its normal speed. Prior research suggests that watching lecture videos at speeds up to 2x does not significantly affect performance, but the mechanisms by which comprehension is preserved at faster playback speeds are not fully understood. Therefore, we sought to investigate whether there is an effect of speed when the content is audio only, varies in modality (audio-only, audio-visual) and content (textual, pictorial), or is accompanied by distractions. In four experiments, we found that: (1) increasing playback speed to 2.5x speed did not impair test performance (though we still do not advise exceeding 2x speed); (2) having a visual aspect (i.e., presentation slides, instructor images) to learning can be advantageous, especially when processing information at faster speeds; (3) there was a small benefit of receiving textual over pictorial presentations, and the effect did not vary by speed; (4) computer-based distractions (i.e., phone calls, text messages, email notifications) did not impact performance at 1x or 2x speed. Hence, students are more adept at learning at faster speeds than conventional wisdom would suggest, even with distractions present, demonstrating intact comprehension at double the natural speed of to-be-learned material. Furthermore, multimedia presentations can help mitigate the negative costs of accelerated speeds, especially when information is processed by separate working memory components (i.e., narration in the auditory channel, text or pictures in the visual channel), which reduces cognitive load.

在现代社会,我们经常以快于正常速度观看内容。先前的研究表明,以最高 2 倍的速度观看讲座视频不会对学习成绩产生明显影响,但在更快的播放速度下保持理解能力的机制尚未完全明了。因此,我们试图研究当内容只有音频、模式(纯音频、视听)和内容(文字、图片)不同或伴有干扰时,速度是否会产生影响。在四个实验中,我们发现(1) 将播放速度提高到 2.5 倍速不会影响测试成绩(尽管我们仍然不建议超过 2 倍速);(2) 视觉方面(即演示幻灯片、教师图像)对学习有好处,尤其是在以较快速度处理信息时;(3) 接收文字演示比接收图像演示有微小好处,而且效果不因速度而异;(4) 基于计算机的分心(即电话、短信、电子邮件通知)不会影响 1 倍速或 2 倍速下的成绩。因此,与传统观念相比,学生更善于以更快的速度学习,即使有分心因素存在,他们也能以两倍于自然速度完整地理解要学习的材料。此外,多媒体演示可以帮助减轻加快速度带来的负面代价,尤其是当信息由不同的工作记忆部分(即听觉通道中的叙述、视觉通道中的文字或图片)处理时,可以减轻认知负荷。
{"title":"The Effect of Playback Speed and Distractions on the Comprehension of Audio and Audio-Visual Materials","authors":"Ashley Chen, Suchita E. Kumar, Rhea Varkhedi, Dillon H. Murphy","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09917-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09917-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the modern age, we often consume content at faster than its normal speed. Prior research suggests that watching lecture videos at speeds up to 2x does not significantly affect performance, but the mechanisms by which comprehension is preserved at faster playback speeds are not fully understood. Therefore, we sought to investigate whether there is an effect of speed when the content is audio only, varies in modality (audio-only, audio-visual) and content (textual, pictorial), or is accompanied by distractions. In four experiments, we found that: (1) increasing playback speed to 2.5x speed did not impair test performance (though we still do not advise exceeding 2x speed); (2) having a visual aspect (i.e., presentation slides, instructor images) to learning can be advantageous, especially when processing information at faster speeds; (3) there was a small benefit of receiving textual over pictorial presentations, and the effect did not vary by speed; (4) computer-based distractions (i.e., phone calls, text messages, email notifications) did not impact performance at 1x or 2x speed. Hence, students are more adept at learning at faster speeds than conventional wisdom would suggest, even with distractions present, demonstrating intact comprehension at double the natural speed of to-be-learned material. Furthermore, multimedia presentations can help mitigate the negative costs of accelerated speeds, especially when information is processed by separate working memory components (i.e., narration in the auditory channel, text or pictures in the visual channel), which reduces cognitive load.\u0000</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"35 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141755195","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Typed Versus Handwritten Lecture Notes and College Student Achievement: A Meta-Analysis 打字笔记与手写笔记和大学生成绩:元分析
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09914-w
Abraham E. Flanigan, Jordan Wheeler, Tiphaine Colliot, Junrong Lu, Kenneth A. Kiewra

Many college students prefer to type their lecture notes rather than write them by hand. As a result, the number of experimental and quasi-experimental studies comparing these two note-taking mediums has flourished over the past decade. The present meta-analytic research sought to uncover trends in the existing studies comparing achievement and note-taking outcomes among college students. Results from 24 separate studies across 21 articles revealed that taking and reviewing handwritten notes leads to higher achievement (Hedges’ g = 0.248; p < 0.001), even though typing notes benefits note-taking volume (Hedges’ g = 0.919; p < 0.001), among college students. Furthermore, our binomial effect size display shows that taking handwritten lecture notes is expected to produce higher course grades than typing notes among college students. We conclude that handwritten notes are more useful for studying and committing to memory than typed notes, ultimately contributing to higher achievement for college students.

许多大学生喜欢打字而不是手写课堂笔记。因此,在过去十年中,对这两种笔记媒介进行比较的实验和准实验研究数量激增。本荟萃分析研究试图揭示现有研究中比较大学生学习成绩和记笔记结果的趋势。来自 21 篇文章中 24 项独立研究的结果显示,尽管打字笔记有利于提高大学生的笔记量(Hedges' g = 0.919; p <0.001),但手写笔记和复习笔记会提高成绩(Hedges' g = 0.248; p <0.001)。此外,我们的二项式效应大小显示,在大学生中,手写讲课笔记比打字笔记有望获得更高的课程成绩。我们的结论是,手写笔记比打字笔记更有助于学习和记忆,最终有助于提高大学生的成绩。
{"title":"Typed Versus Handwritten Lecture Notes and College Student Achievement: A Meta-Analysis","authors":"Abraham E. Flanigan, Jordan Wheeler, Tiphaine Colliot, Junrong Lu, Kenneth A. Kiewra","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09914-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09914-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many college students prefer to type their lecture notes rather than write them by hand. As a result, the number of experimental and quasi-experimental studies comparing these two note-taking mediums has flourished over the past decade. The present meta-analytic research sought to uncover trends in the existing studies comparing achievement and note-taking outcomes among college students. Results from 24 separate studies across 21 articles revealed that taking and reviewing handwritten notes leads to higher achievement (Hedges’ <i>g</i> = 0.248; <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001), even though typing notes benefits note-taking volume (Hedges’ <i>g</i> = 0.919; <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001), among college students. Furthermore, our binomial effect size display shows that taking handwritten lecture notes is expected to produce higher course grades than typing notes among college students. We conclude that handwritten notes are more useful for studying and committing to memory than typed notes, ultimately contributing to higher achievement for college students.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"39 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141597274","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Associations Between Discrete Emotions and Political Learning: A Cross-Disciplinary Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis 离散情绪与政治学习之间的关联:跨学科系统回顾与元分析
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09893-y
Elisabeth Graf, Johanna L. Donath, Elouise Botes, Martin Voracek, Thomas Goetz

In recent decades, researchers’ interest in the role of emotions in individual political learning has grown. However, it is still unclear whether and how discrete emotions are associated with political learning. Through a cross-disciplinary systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis, we reviewed which discrete emotions have been analyzed in the context of political learning so far and meta-analytically synthesized how these emotions relate to political learning. We addressed this question by synthesizing associations between discrete emotions and various aspects of learning about political matters, such as political attention, information seeking, discussions, knowledge, and knowledge gain. The final dataset included 66 publications with 486 effect sizes, involving more than 100,000 participants. Most of the effect sizes were based on negative-activating emotions (65%; mainly anxiety, 32%, and anger, 19%) and positive-activating emotions (32%; mainly enthusiasm, 15%), while studies on positive-deactivating emotions (e.g., contentment) and negative-deactivating emotions (e.g., sadness) are largely lacking. We uncovered small positive associations (r = .05 to .13) for activating emotions, of both negative (especially anger) and positive valence (e.g., enthusiasm, only in cross-sectional designs), but no associations for negative-deactivating emotions. We discuss theoretical implications and recommend future research to include previously unconsidered emotions in order to extend existing findings.

近几十年来,研究人员对情绪在个人政治学习中的作用的兴趣与日俱增。然而,离散情绪是否以及如何与政治学习相关,目前仍不清楚。通过跨学科的系统综述和多层次的荟萃分析,我们回顾了迄今为止在政治学习背景下分析过的离散情绪,并对这些情绪与政治学习的关系进行了荟萃分析。针对这一问题,我们综合了离散情绪与政治学习的各个方面(如政治关注、信息寻求、讨论、知识和知识获得)之间的关联。最终的数据集包括 66 篇出版物和 486 个效应大小,涉及 10 万多名参与者。大多数效应大小都是基于消极激活情绪(65%;主要是焦虑,32%;愤怒,19%)和积极激活情绪(32%;主要是热情,15%),而关于积极消极情绪(如满足感)和消极消极情绪(如悲伤)的研究则非常缺乏。我们发现,激活情绪与消极情绪(尤其是愤怒)和积极情绪(如热情,仅在横断面设计中)之间存在微小的正相关(r = 0.05 至 0.13),但与消极激活情绪之间没有任何关联。我们讨论了这一研究的理论意义,并建议今后的研究将以前未考虑的情绪纳入其中,以扩展现有的研究结果。
{"title":"The Associations Between Discrete Emotions and Political Learning: A Cross-Disciplinary Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis","authors":"Elisabeth Graf, Johanna L. Donath, Elouise Botes, Martin Voracek, Thomas Goetz","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09893-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09893-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In recent decades, researchers’ interest in the role of emotions in individual political learning has grown. However, it is still unclear whether and how discrete emotions are associated with political learning. Through a cross-disciplinary systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis, we reviewed which discrete emotions have been analyzed in the context of political learning so far and meta-analytically synthesized how these emotions relate to political learning. We addressed this question by synthesizing associations between discrete emotions and various aspects of learning about political matters, such as political attention, information seeking, discussions, knowledge, and knowledge gain. The final dataset included 66 publications with 486 effect sizes, involving more than 100,000 participants. Most of the effect sizes were based on negative-activating emotions (65%; mainly anxiety, 32%, and anger, 19%) and positive-activating emotions (32%; mainly enthusiasm, 15%), while studies on positive-deactivating emotions (e.g., contentment) and negative-deactivating emotions (e.g., sadness) are largely lacking. We uncovered small positive associations (<i>r</i> = .05 to .13) for activating emotions, of both negative (especially anger) and positive valence (e.g., enthusiasm, only in cross-sectional designs), but no associations for negative-deactivating emotions. We discuss theoretical implications and recommend future research to include previously unconsidered emotions in order to extend existing findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"103 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141584214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stimming as Thinking: a Critical Reevaluation of Self-Stimulatory Behavior as an Epistemic Resource for Inclusive Education 刺激即思考:对作为全纳教育认识资源的自我刺激行为的批判性再评价
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09904-y
Sofia Tancredi, Dor Abrahamson

Peripheral sensorimotor stimming activity, such as rocking and fidgeting, is widely considered irrelevant to and even distracting from learning. In this critical-pedagogy conceptual paper, we argue that stimming is an intrinsic part of adaptive functioning, interaction, and cognitive dynamics. We submit that when cultural resources build from students’ own sensorimotor dynamics, rather than subjugating them to hegemonic corporeal norms, learners’ intrinsic sensorimotor behaviors may be embraced and empowered as mental activity. This call for transformative inclusive pedagogy is of particular importance for neurodivergent children whose sensorimotor engagements have historically been ostracized as disruptive. Following a conceptual analysis of stimming that builds on a range of neuro-cognitive empirical studies drawing on post-cognitivist embodied cognition theory, we imagine inclusive educational futures that disrupt sedentary instructional design to elevate minoritized learners’ sensorimotor activity. As proof of concept, we present an example inclusive embodied activity, balance board math, a pedagogical tool designed to elicit stimming as thinking. We propose a set of design heuristics for realizing stimming’s pedagogical potential.

人们普遍认为,摇晃和躁动等外周感觉运动刺激活动与学习无关,甚至会分散学习注意力。在这篇批判教育学概念性论文中,我们认为刺激是适应功能、互动和认知动力的内在组成部分。我们认为,当文化资源建立在学生自身的感官运动动力之上,而不是将其屈从于霸权的肉体规范时,学习者固有的感官运动行为就有可能作为心理活动而被接纳和赋权。这种对变革性全纳教学法的呼唤,对于神经变异儿童尤为重要,因为他们的感官运动参与历来被视为破坏性活动而受到排斥。我们在后认知主义体现认知理论的基础上进行了一系列神经认知实证研究,并在此基础上对 "刺激"(stimming)进行了概念分析,我们设想了包容性教育的未来,这种未来将打破沉闷的教学设计,提升少数群体学习者的感官运动活动。作为概念的证明,我们提出了一个包容性具身活动的例子--平衡板数学,这是一种旨在激发刺激思维的教学工具。我们提出了一套设计启发式方法,以实现刺激教学的潜力。
{"title":"Stimming as Thinking: a Critical Reevaluation of Self-Stimulatory Behavior as an Epistemic Resource for Inclusive Education","authors":"Sofia Tancredi, Dor Abrahamson","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09904-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09904-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Peripheral sensorimotor stimming activity, such as rocking and fidgeting, is widely considered irrelevant to and even distracting from learning. In this critical-pedagogy conceptual paper, we argue that stimming is an intrinsic part of adaptive functioning, interaction, and cognitive dynamics. We submit that when cultural resources build from students’ own sensorimotor dynamics, rather than subjugating them to hegemonic corporeal norms, learners’ intrinsic sensorimotor behaviors may be embraced and empowered as mental activity. This call for transformative inclusive pedagogy is of particular importance for neurodivergent children whose sensorimotor engagements have historically been ostracized as disruptive. Following a conceptual analysis of stimming that builds on a range of neuro-cognitive empirical studies drawing on post-cognitivist embodied cognition theory, we imagine inclusive educational futures that disrupt sedentary instructional design to elevate minoritized learners’ sensorimotor activity. As proof of concept, we present an example inclusive embodied activity, balance board math, a pedagogical tool designed to elicit stimming as thinking. We propose a set of design heuristics for realizing stimming’s pedagogical potential.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"12 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141561564","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Meeting John Bransford 会见约翰-布兰福德
IF 10.1 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-07-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10648-024-09912-y
Sashank Varma

John D. Bransford died in 2022. He was an intellectual giant. His seminal work in educational psychology was recognized by Division 15 of the American Psychological Association with their Career Achievement Award in 2001. This paper is an introduction to the man. It traces his intellectual development from cognitive psychology to educational psychology to the learning sciences. It is written as a first-person narrative to echo the perspective he often adopted in his surprisingly accessible papers. This paper also describes the intellectual community he built around himself, one that was remarkably successful in generating novel ideas, supporting collaborative research, and training generations of young researchers who would go on to make their own marks. Finally, it portrays what it was like to work alongside a man who saw just a little further down the road than the rest of us.

约翰-布兰斯福德于 2022 年去世。他是一位思想巨人。他在教育心理学方面的开创性工作于 2001 年被美国心理学会第 15 分部授予职业成就奖。本文是对他的介绍。它追溯了他从认知心理学到教育心理学再到学习科学的智力发展历程。本文以第一人称叙述的方式撰写,以呼应他在其平易近人的论文中经常采用的视角。本文还描述了他围绕自己建立起来的知识界,这个知识界在产生新思想、支持合作研究以及培养一代又一代年轻研究人员方面取得了巨大成功,这些年轻研究人员将继续在各自的领域大显身手。最后,本文还描述了与一位比我们其他人看得更远的人并肩工作的感受。
{"title":"Meeting John Bransford","authors":"Sashank Varma","doi":"10.1007/s10648-024-09912-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09912-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>John D. Bransford died in 2022. He was an intellectual giant. His seminal work in educational psychology was recognized by Division 15 of the American Psychological Association with their Career Achievement Award in 2001. This paper is an introduction to the man. It traces his intellectual development from cognitive psychology to educational psychology to the learning sciences. It is written as a first-person narrative to echo the perspective he often adopted in his surprisingly accessible papers. This paper also describes the intellectual community he built around himself, one that was remarkably successful in generating novel ideas, supporting collaborative research, and training generations of young researchers who would go on to make their own marks. Finally, it portrays what it was like to work alongside a man who saw just a little further down the road than the rest of us.</p>","PeriodicalId":48344,"journal":{"name":"Educational Psychology Review","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":10.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141553379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Educational Psychology Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1