Pub Date : 2024-09-03DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101995
Ching-Huei Chen , Kun Huang
Although there is controversy surrounding rewards and competition in education, they are key elements in digital game-based learning (DGBL). Research on the influence of rewards and competition in DGBL often yields mixed results, suggesting a need to investigate individual game elements and their interactions. This study implemented DGBL focusing on computational thinking (CT) in 7th-grade classes. Both quantitative and behavioral data were collected to understand the impact of rewards (power-up or customization) and competition (with or without) on students’ intrinsic motivation, game performance, behaviors, and CT skills. The findings uncovered significant interactions between rewards and competition. Students with both competition and power-up rewards achieved the highest game points but the lowest CT performance, whereas those exposed to the same power-up rewards without competition attained the lowest game points but the best CT performance. Implications are drawn for DGBL research and practice.
{"title":"The interplay of rewards and competition in digital game-based learning: Effects on intrinsic motivation, game performance and behaviors, and computational thinking","authors":"Ching-Huei Chen , Kun Huang","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101995","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101995","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although there is controversy surrounding rewards and competition in education, they are key elements in digital game-based learning (DGBL). Research on the influence of rewards and competition in DGBL often yields mixed results, suggesting a need to investigate individual game elements and their interactions. This study implemented DGBL focusing on computational thinking (CT) in 7th-grade classes. Both quantitative and behavioral data were collected to understand the impact of rewards (power-up or customization) and competition (with or without) on students’ intrinsic motivation, game performance, behaviors, and CT skills. The findings uncovered significant interactions between rewards and competition. Students with both competition and power-up rewards achieved the highest game points but the lowest CT performance, whereas those exposed to the same power-up rewards without competition attained the lowest game points but the best CT performance. Implications are drawn for DGBL research and practice.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 101995"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142129934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-09-02DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102003
Eadaoin J. Slattery , Deirdre Butler , Kevin Marshall , Michael Barrett , Neeve Hyland , Michael O'Leary , Laura P. McAvinue
Background
Spatial thinking plays a crucial role in STEM success. Minecraft Education, a digital game-based learning platform, provides opportunities for developing students’ spatial thinking while supporting curricular content.
Aim
This study evaluated the effectiveness of a Minecraft Education intervention, integrated into the curriculum, in improving spatial thinking and creativity.
Sample
Fifth and sixth year primary school students (Mage = 11.53, SDage = 0.58) and their teachers.
Methods
The study used a mixed-methods, two-level cluster randomised-controlled trial design. Thirty-two classes (N = 885 students) were randomly assigned to the intervention or education-as-usual control group. Assessments of spatial thinking and creativity were completed at pre-test, post-test and 6-week follow up. Teacher interviews were conducted to understand their experiences of the intervention.
Results
While no overall intervention-related improvements in spatial thinking were found, subgroup analyses revealed that 5th year demonstrated improved spatial thinking compared to 6th year at post-test. This may be attributable to 5th year students’ higher levels of engagement in the intervention. Unexpectedly, creativity scores were higher in the control group at post-test. Qualitative interviews identified four themes: 1) Improving spatial thinking, 2) Improving creativity, 3) Engagement, and 4) Difficulties implementing the programme.
Conclusion
This study highlights the importance of increasing student engagement in spatial interventions to improve their spatial skills. Finally, the study suggests that sustained practice is required to maintain the benefits of spatial instruction/training.
{"title":"Effectiveness of a minecraft education intervention for improving spatial thinking in primary school children: A mixed methods two-level cluster randomised trial","authors":"Eadaoin J. Slattery , Deirdre Butler , Kevin Marshall , Michael Barrett , Neeve Hyland , Michael O'Leary , Laura P. McAvinue","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102003","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Spatial thinking plays a crucial role in STEM success. Minecraft Education, a digital game-based learning platform, provides opportunities for developing students’ spatial thinking while supporting curricular content.</p></div><div><h3>Aim</h3><p>This study evaluated the effectiveness of a Minecraft Education intervention, integrated into the curriculum, in improving spatial thinking and creativity.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>Fifth and sixth year primary school students (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 11.53, <em>SD</em><sub>age</sub> = 0.58) and their teachers.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The study used a mixed-methods, two-level cluster randomised-controlled trial design. Thirty-two classes (<em>N</em> = 885 students) were randomly assigned to the intervention or education-as-usual control group. Assessments of spatial thinking and creativity were completed at pre-test, post-test and 6-week follow up. Teacher interviews were conducted to understand their experiences of the intervention.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>While no overall intervention-related improvements in spatial thinking were found, subgroup analyses revealed that 5th year demonstrated improved spatial thinking compared to 6th year at post-test. This may be attributable to 5th year students’ higher levels of engagement in the intervention. Unexpectedly, creativity scores were higher in the control group at post-test. Qualitative interviews identified four themes: 1) Improving spatial thinking, 2) Improving creativity, 3) Engagement, and 4) Difficulties implementing the programme.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>This study highlights the importance of increasing student engagement in spatial interventions to improve their spatial skills. Finally, the study suggests that sustained practice is required to maintain the benefits of spatial instruction/training.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 102003"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959475224001300/pdfft?md5=a2192ade107e47e97080d0d645eeca0f&pid=1-s2.0-S0959475224001300-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142129933","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-31DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101991
Julia Götzfried, Lea Nemeth, Victoria Bleck, Frank Lipowsky
Background
The learning styles myth is prevalent among (pre-service) teachers. Research shows that texts and podcasts refuting misconceptions and contrasting them with scientific concepts can effectively dissolve such misconceptions through cognitive conflicts, encouraging the reflection of one's beliefs. However, it is unclear if texts and podcasts differ in refuting pre-service teachers' misconceptions.
Aims
We investigated if conceptual change texts and podcasts have different potential for inducing conceptual change among pre-service teachers regarding their beliefs in the learning styles myth, compared to an expository information presentation immediately and up to eight weeks after the intervention.
Sample
116 pre-service teachers participated in the short term, and 100 in the long term.
Methods
The experimental study used a 2 x 2 x 4 mixed-design (manner of information presentation: conceptual change vs expository; medium: podcast vs text; time: immediately before and after the treatment as well as four and eight weeks later). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions. Students' beliefs were measured at all four measurement points.
Results
The conceptual change conditions led to a stronger decrease in students' beliefs than the expository conditions in the short term (p < 0.01, η2 = 0.08) and the long term (p = 0.03, η2 = 0.04). Text and podcast were equally effective in the short term (p = 0.62, η2 = 0.00, BF01 = 9.13) and the long term (p = 0.67, η2 = 0.00, BF01 = 39.94).
Conclusion
Podcasts and texts using conceptual change strategies can effectively dispel pedagogical misconceptions among pre-service teachers up to eight weeks post-intervention.
{"title":"Learning styles unmasked: Conceptual change among pre-service teachers using podcasts and texts","authors":"Julia Götzfried, Lea Nemeth, Victoria Bleck, Frank Lipowsky","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101991","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101991","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>The learning styles myth is prevalent among (pre-service) teachers. Research shows that texts and podcasts refuting misconceptions and contrasting them with scientific concepts can effectively dissolve such misconceptions through cognitive conflicts, encouraging the reflection of one's beliefs. However, it is unclear if texts and podcasts differ in refuting pre-service teachers' misconceptions.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>We investigated if conceptual change texts and podcasts have different potential for inducing conceptual change among pre-service teachers regarding their beliefs in the learning styles myth, compared to an expository information presentation immediately and up to eight weeks after the intervention.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>116 pre-service teachers participated in the short term, and 100 in the long term.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The experimental study used a 2 x 2 x 4 mixed-design (manner of information presentation: conceptual change vs expository; medium: podcast vs text; time: immediately before and after the treatment as well as four and eight weeks later). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions. Students' beliefs were measured at all four measurement points.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The conceptual change conditions led to a stronger decrease in students' beliefs than the expository conditions in the short term (<em>p</em> < 0.01, η<sup>2</sup> = 0.08) and the long term (<em>p</em> = 0.03, η<sup>2</sup> = 0.04). Text and podcast were equally effective in the short term (<em>p</em> = 0.62, η<sup>2</sup> = 0.00, BF<sub>01</sub> = 9.13) and the long term (<em>p</em> = 0.67, η<sup>2</sup> = 0.00, BF<sub>01</sub> = 39.94).</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Podcasts and texts using conceptual change strategies can effectively dispel pedagogical misconceptions among pre-service teachers up to eight weeks post-intervention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 101991"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S095947522400118X/pdfft?md5=5ceec285d6ffb6c544233e6a51645c68&pid=1-s2.0-S095947522400118X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142099514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-30DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102005
Anne Heinschel , Sofie Henschel , Camilla Rjosk
Background
As instructional quality is crucial for successful learning, its conditional factors are of particular interest for research and practice. Research showed that distal as well as proximal class composition factors are related to instructional quality. However, it is still unclear to what extent these aspects explain differences in instructional quality between classes of the same teacher.
Aim
This study investigates differences in instructional quality (cognitive activation, classroom management, student support) between classes taught by the same teacher and how these differences are related to distal (gender ratio, SES, proportion of multilingual learners) and proximal (self-concept, interest, mathematics grade) classroom composition factors.
Sample
Analyses are based on 3566 students in 219 classes taught by 106 teachers in 9th-grade mathematics classrooms in Germany.
Methods
We estimated three-level doubly-latent linear mixed models regressing instructional quality on class composition factors with students at level 1, classes at level 2, and teachers at level 3.
Results
Roughly 7–13 % of the variance in instructional quality perceived by the students can be attributed to class characteristics. Higher class-mean interest and a lower proportion of multilingual learners are related to higher cognitive activation. Class-mean self-concept is negatively and interest is positively related to student support. Higher discipline can be significantly explained by higher mean mathematics grades and more female students in the class.
Conclusions
Our results underline the importance of considering the classroom context when striving for high instructional quality, highlighting the importance of promoting proximal factors such as student interest.
{"title":"Links between instructional quality and classroom composition in classes taught by the same teacher","authors":"Anne Heinschel , Sofie Henschel , Camilla Rjosk","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>As instructional quality is crucial for successful learning, its conditional factors are of particular interest for research and practice. Research showed that distal as well as proximal class composition factors are related to instructional quality. However, it is still unclear to what extent these aspects explain differences in instructional quality between classes of the same teacher.</p></div><div><h3>Aim</h3><p>This study investigates differences in instructional quality (cognitive activation, classroom management, student support) between classes taught by the same teacher and how these differences are related to distal (gender ratio, SES, proportion of multilingual learners) and proximal (self-concept, interest, mathematics grade) classroom composition factors.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>Analyses are based on 3566 students in 219 classes taught by 106 teachers in 9th-grade mathematics classrooms in Germany.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>We estimated three-level doubly-latent linear mixed models regressing instructional quality on class composition factors with students at level 1, classes at level 2, and teachers at level 3.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Roughly 7–13 % of the variance in instructional quality perceived by the students can be attributed to class characteristics. Higher class-mean interest and a lower proportion of multilingual learners are related to higher cognitive activation. Class-mean self-concept is negatively and interest is positively related to student support. Higher discipline can be significantly explained by higher mean mathematics grades and more female students in the class.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our results underline the importance of considering the classroom context when striving for high instructional quality, highlighting the importance of promoting proximal factors such as student interest.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 102005"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142099306","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-28DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102002
Rebecca J. Collie , Tina Hascher
{"title":"Student well-being: Advancing knowledge of the construct and the role of learning and teaching factors","authors":"Rebecca J. Collie , Tina Hascher","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102002","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 102002"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142086936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-26DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102004
Xiaoxue Leng , Wanling Zhu , Richard E. Mayer , Fuxing Wang
Background
The viewing perspective effect refers to the finding that students learn better from instructional videos recorded from first-person perspective than third-person perspective, but little is known about the neural mechanism underlying this effect.
Aims
This study investigates the underlying neural mechanism of the viewing perspective effect.
Samples
Participants were 60 university students in Experiment 1, and 65 university students in Experiment 2.
Methods
Experiment 1 replicated previous studies by using paper folding tasks (i.e., butterfly and four-leaf clover). Participants were randomly assigned to the first-person view group (i.e., watching videos recorded from a first-person view) or the third-person view group (i.e., watching videos recorded from a third-person view). Experiment 2 extended viewing perspective research by using neuroimaging methodology. Participants were randomly assigned to the two groups as in Experiment 1, but with fNIRS probes placed on each student's head. Participants watched the butterfly video (watching phase) and then performed the task (imitation phase).
Results
There was superior performance on the butterfly task with a first-person view over a third-person view in both experiments, replicating the viewing perspective effect. There was higher cortical activation in the first-person view group in the right dlPFC during the watching phase, and higher cortical activation in third-person view group in the left IPC during the imitation phase.
Conclusions
This study replicates viewing perspective effect and investigates the underlying neural mechanism of the viewing perspective effect.
{"title":"The viewing perspective effect in learning from instructional videos: A replication and neuroimaging extension","authors":"Xiaoxue Leng , Wanling Zhu , Richard E. Mayer , Fuxing Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>The viewing perspective effect refers to the finding that students learn better from instructional videos recorded from first-person perspective than third-person perspective, but little is known about the neural mechanism underlying this effect.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>This study investigates the underlying neural mechanism of the viewing perspective effect.</p></div><div><h3>Samples</h3><p>Participants were 60 university students in Experiment 1, and 65 university students in Experiment 2.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Experiment 1 replicated previous studies by using paper folding tasks (i.e., butterfly and four-leaf clover). Participants were randomly assigned to the first-person view group (i.e., watching videos recorded from a first-person view) or the third-person view group (i.e., watching videos recorded from a third-person view). Experiment 2 extended viewing perspective research by using neuroimaging methodology. Participants were randomly assigned to the two groups as in Experiment 1, but with fNIRS probes placed on each student's head. Participants watched the butterfly video (watching phase) and then performed the task (imitation phase).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>There was superior performance on the butterfly task with a first-person view over a third-person view in both experiments, replicating the viewing perspective effect. There was higher cortical activation in the first-person view group in the right dlPFC during the watching phase, and higher cortical activation in third-person view group in the left IPC during the imitation phase.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>This study replicates viewing perspective effect and investigates the underlying neural mechanism of the viewing perspective effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 102004"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142076681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-26DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102001
Ines Zeitlhofer , Joerg Zumbach , Judith Schweppe
Background
Problem-solving encourages learners to engage in deep knowledge processing. However, merely providing problem-solving activities without considering task complexity may not optimize learning.
Aims
The present study analyzed the effects of task complexity (consistently high vs. gradually increasing) on performance, germane cognitive load, meta-awareness, and intrinsic interest using a learning task conducted in a laboratory.
Sample
We analyzed data collected from 98 graduate and ungraduated university students (N = 98; Mage = 21.22 years, SD = 3.03).
Methods
The study comprised a main session and a post-test. During the main session participants completed a problem-solving task either with consistently high complexity or gradually increasing complexity. During the post-test all participants solved the task once with high complexity. Metacognition (regulation, knowledge), germane cognitive load, meta-awareness, and intrinsic interest were assessed by self-evaluation questionnaires.
Results
The findings indicated positive effects of consistently high complexity on immediate performance, germane cognitive load, and meta-awareness. Additionally, an important relationship between metacognition (regulation, knowledge) and meta-awareness was identified when learning with gradually increasing complexity. However, no impact of task complexity on intrinsic interest was found.
Conclusions
The findings suggest that exposing learners to consistently high complexity tasks can have beneficial effects on learning without negatively influencing intrinsic interest.
{"title":"Complexity affects performance, cognitive load, and awareness","authors":"Ines Zeitlhofer , Joerg Zumbach , Judith Schweppe","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.102001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Problem-solving encourages learners to engage in deep knowledge processing. However, merely providing problem-solving activities without considering task complexity may not optimize learning.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>The present study analyzed the effects of task complexity (consistently high vs. gradually increasing) on performance, germane cognitive load, meta-awareness, and intrinsic interest using a learning task conducted in a laboratory.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>We analyzed data collected from 98 graduate and ungraduated university students (<em>N</em> = 98; <em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 21.22 years, <em>SD</em> = 3.03).</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The study comprised a main session and a post-test. During the main session participants completed a problem-solving task either with consistently high complexity or gradually increasing complexity. During the post-test all participants solved the task once with high complexity. Metacognition (regulation, knowledge), germane cognitive load, meta-awareness, and intrinsic interest were assessed by self-evaluation questionnaires.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The findings indicated positive effects of consistently high complexity on immediate performance, germane cognitive load, and meta-awareness. Additionally, an important relationship between metacognition (regulation, knowledge) and meta-awareness was identified when learning with gradually increasing complexity. However, no impact of task complexity on intrinsic interest was found.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The findings suggest that exposing learners to consistently high complexity tasks can have beneficial effects on learning without negatively influencing intrinsic interest.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 102001"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959475224001282/pdfft?md5=bc5f61a2675f327841ca66363145190e&pid=1-s2.0-S0959475224001282-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142076680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-26DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101993
Kevin van Loon , Monika Waldis
Background
Image interpretation scaffolds and mind mapping may support students’ historical reasoning. Benefits of such approaches are, until present, not empirically tested.
Aims
This quasi-experimental study compared effects of three conditions: a sequenced image interpretation scaffold, a flexible image interpretation scaffold, and mind mapping on students’ historical reasoning about and with images.
Sample
Participants were adolescents (secondary school students, N = 145, M age 13.9 years).
Methods
Historical reasoning was assessed with a writing task. Participants interpreted three photographs over three time points. At T1, students had no support. At T2 and T3, students interpreted an image with the randomly assigned condition. Students’ interpretative essays were rated to assess historical reasoning.
Results
Image interpretation scaffolds better supported historical reasoning than mind mapping. Both interpretation scaffolds enhanced historical reasoning. Particularly the flexible image interpretation scaffold appeared to benefit contextualization and the description of the relevance of the image message for the present. However, effects of the flexible scaffold were not stable over time.
Conclusions
Image interpretation scaffolds seem beneficial to support students’ interpretation of historical images. Further research needs to investigate how image interpretation scaffolds support historical reasoning for different types of images.
{"title":"Image interpretation scaffolds support adolescents’ historical reasoning","authors":"Kevin van Loon , Monika Waldis","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101993","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101993","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Image interpretation scaffolds and mind mapping may support students’ historical reasoning. Benefits of such approaches are, until present, not empirically tested.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>This quasi-experimental study compared effects of three conditions: a sequenced image interpretation scaffold, a flexible image interpretation scaffold, and mind mapping on students’ historical reasoning <em>about</em> and <em>with</em> images.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>Participants were adolescents (secondary school students, <em>N</em> = 145, <em>M</em> age 13.9 years).</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Historical reasoning was assessed with a writing task. Participants interpreted three photographs over three time points. At T1, students had no support. At T2 and T3, students interpreted an image with the randomly assigned condition. Students’ interpretative essays were rated to assess historical reasoning.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Image interpretation scaffolds better supported historical reasoning than mind mapping. Both interpretation scaffolds enhanced historical reasoning. Particularly the flexible image interpretation scaffold appeared to benefit contextualization and the description of the relevance of the image message for the present. However, effects of the flexible scaffold were not stable over time.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Image interpretation scaffolds seem beneficial to support students’ interpretation of historical images. Further research needs to investigate how image interpretation scaffolds support historical reasoning for different types of images.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 101993"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142076679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-24DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101998
Linda Froese, Julian Roelle
Background
Providing learners with standards, which represent correct solutions to a task, is a promising means to support self-assessment. To date, however, the evidence in support of standards mainly stems from studies that used tasks for which correct solutions are very similar to each other in terms of both structural and surface features. By contrast, for tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features, empirical evidence is scarce.
Aims
The goals of the present study were to investigate the effects of standards in tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features.
Samples
Participants were NExp1 = 139 and NExp2 = 170 university students.
Methods
Using the task of generating examples that illustrate previously encountered declarative concepts, we varied whether in self-assessing the quality of their examples learners received standards (with vs. without) and structural comparison/processing support (with vs. without) that supported learners in processing the critical structural features of the standards and their own examples.
Results
Standards increased self-assessment accuracy and performance. The effects concerning self-assessment accuracy could be enhanced by additionally providing structural comparison support.
Conclusions
We conclude that standards are a promising means to support self-assessment accuracy in tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features. However, their effectiveness depends on the degree to which learners compare their products and the standards in terms of structural features, which needs to be supported.
{"title":"How to support self-assessment through standards in dissimilar-solution-tasks","authors":"Linda Froese, Julian Roelle","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101998","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101998","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>Providing learners with standards, which represent correct solutions to a task, is a promising means to support self-assessment. To date, however, the evidence in support of standards mainly stems from studies that used tasks for which correct solutions are very similar to each other in terms of both structural and surface features. By contrast, for tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features, empirical evidence is scarce.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>The goals of the present study were to investigate the effects of standards in tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features.</p></div><div><h3>Samples</h3><p>Participants were <em>N</em><sub>Exp1</sub> = 139 and <em>N</em><sub>Exp2</sub> = 170 university students.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Using the task of generating examples that illustrate previously encountered declarative concepts, we varied whether in self-assessing the quality of their examples learners received <em>standards</em> (with vs. without) and <em>structural comparison/processing</em> support (with vs. without) that supported learners in processing the critical structural features of the standards and their own examples.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Standards increased self-assessment accuracy and performance. The effects concerning self-assessment accuracy could be enhanced by additionally providing structural comparison support.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>We conclude that standards are a promising means to support self-assessment accuracy in tasks for which correct solutions are similar to each other only in terms of structural features. However, their effectiveness depends on the degree to which learners compare their products and the standards in terms of structural features, which needs to be supported.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 101998"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959475224001257/pdfft?md5=d66d53fa7067c395a26cc6d03c2ca8c3&pid=1-s2.0-S0959475224001257-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142049977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-08-23DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101992
Carolina Lopera-Oquendo , Anastasiya A. Lipnevich , Ignacio Mañez
Background
In a typical instructional setting, teachers are responsible for making ongoing decisions that involve judgments of students’ capabilities, knowledge, learning needs, and progress toward a certain pre-specified goal. However, there is a significant within-teacher as well as a great between-teacher variability in the actual determination of grades. Grades appear to be an amalgam of characteristics of a student, filtered through a range of teacher personality variables.
Aims
The purpose of this study was to investigate the extent to which pre-service teachers agreed on students’ grades in writing task between holistic and analytic grading approaches and how their individual characteristics and beliefs about features of assessment explained the variability in grading practices.
Sample
Teacher candidates (N = 231, 65% female) enrolled in a training program in 2020 and 2021 cohorts at the University of València, Spain, were asked to read two essays, identified by experts as being of low and high quality, and assign holistic and analytic grades.
Results
although teacher candidates provided grades consistently across the two approaches (intra-individual differences), there was a high variability in the distribution among participants (inter-individual differences). We found that, gender, area of specialization, attitudes toward feedback, and extraversion were significant predictors of grading variability.
Conclusion
This study highlights the considerable variation in grading practices among pre-service teachers, indicating the influence of individual factors such as gender, specialization, feedback receptivity, and extraversion. Despite consistent grading within specific approaches, the inter-individual differences in scores were substantial. Due to the consequential nature of teacher grades, our findings offer important insights and have critical implications for teacher preparation and professional development programs.
背景在典型的教学环境中,教师负责对学生的能力、知识、学习需求以及在实现某个预先指定目标方面的进展情况作出持续的判断。然而,在实际确定成绩的过程中,教师内部和教师之间的差异都很大。本研究的目的是调查职前教师在整体评分法和分析评分法之间对学生写作任务成绩的认同程度,以及他们的个人特点和对评价特点的信念如何解释评分实践中的差异。样本西班牙瓦伦西亚大学(University of València)2020 年和 2021 年入学的师范生(N = 231,65% 为女性)被要求阅读两篇被专家认定为低质量和高质量的作文,并给出整体评分和分析评分。我们发现,性别、专业领域、对反馈的态度和外向性是评分差异的重要预测因素。结论这项研究凸显了职前教师在评分实践中的巨大差异,表明性别、专业领域、反馈接受能力和外向性等个体因素的影响。尽管在特定的方法中评分是一致的,但个体间的分数差异很大。由于教师评分的后果性,我们的研究结果提供了重要的见解,并对教师准备和专业发展计划具有重要意义。
{"title":"Rating writing: Comparison of holistic and analytic grading approaches in pre-service teachers","authors":"Carolina Lopera-Oquendo , Anastasiya A. Lipnevich , Ignacio Mañez","doi":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101992","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.learninstruc.2024.101992","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Background</h3><p>In a typical instructional setting, teachers are responsible for making ongoing decisions that involve judgments of students’ capabilities, knowledge, learning needs, and progress toward a certain pre-specified goal. However, there is a significant within-teacher as well as a great between-teacher variability in the actual determination of grades. Grades appear to be an amalgam of characteristics of a student, filtered through a range of teacher personality variables.</p></div><div><h3>Aims</h3><p>The purpose of this study was to investigate the extent to which pre-service teachers agreed on students’ grades in writing task between holistic and analytic grading approaches and how their individual characteristics and beliefs about features of assessment explained the variability in grading practices.</p></div><div><h3>Sample</h3><p>Teacher candidates (N = 231, 65% female) enrolled in a training program in 2020 and 2021 cohorts at the University of València, Spain, were asked to read two essays, identified by experts as being of low and high quality, and assign holistic and analytic grades.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>although teacher candidates provided grades consistently across the two approaches (intra-individual differences), there was a high variability in the distribution among participants (inter-individual differences). We found that, gender, area of specialization, attitudes toward feedback, and extraversion were significant predictors of grading variability.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>This study highlights the considerable variation in grading practices among pre-service teachers, indicating the influence of individual factors such as gender, specialization, feedback receptivity, and extraversion. Despite consistent grading within specific approaches, the inter-individual differences in scores were substantial. Due to the consequential nature of teacher grades, our findings offer important insights and have critical implications for teacher preparation and professional development programs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48357,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Instruction","volume":"94 ","pages":"Article 101992"},"PeriodicalIF":4.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142049959","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}