首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
A three-component model of relational responding in the transposition paradigm. 转置范式中关系反应的三分量模型。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000004
Olga F Lazareva, Michael E Young, Edward A Wasserman

We present a new model of transposition behavior that involves 3 predictors: (a) the disparity in generalized associative strength from the previously reinforced and nonreinforced stimuli (g) to the stimuli in the testing pair; (b) relational disparity (r), the difference in the logarithmically scaled sensory values of the testing stimuli; and (c) familiarity (f), the inverse of the Euclidean distance from the testing pair to the nearest training pair in 2-dimensional stimulus space. We evaluated the model with pigeons as subjects and with circle diameter (Experiment 1) and speed of motion (Experiment 2) as sensory dimensions. In each experiment, we presented 1, 2, or 3 training pairs as well as a wide range of testing pairs, including those comprising nonadjacent training stimuli. The control that was exerted by g did not depend on the number of training pairs and predicted behavior better than r and f after 1-pair training. In contrast, the influence of r increased dramatically with an increase in the number of training pairs. The contribution of f depended on the stimulus domain: When circle area was used (Experiment 1), the influence of f was similar to r; however, when speed of motion was used (Experiment 2), f had no discernible effect on pigeons' behavior. In sum, our results suggest that pigeons' transposition behavior is affected by both reinforcement history (g) and the relation between the experimental stimuli (r and f); our model provides a principled means for assessing the relative contribution of each predictor to choice behavior.

我们提出了一个新的移位行为模型,该模型涉及3个预测因素:(a)从先前强化和未强化的刺激(g)到测试对中的刺激的广义联想强度的差异;(b)关系差异(r),测试刺激的对数尺度感官值的差异;(c)熟悉度(f),在二维刺激空间中测试对到最近的训练对的欧氏距离的倒数。我们以鸽子为实验对象,以圆周直径(实验1)和运动速度(实验2)为感官维度对模型进行评价。在每个实验中,我们提供了1、2或3个训练配对以及广泛的测试配对,包括那些包含非相邻训练刺激的配对。g所施加的控制不依赖于训练对的数量,并且在1对训练后比r和f更好地预测行为。相反,r的影响随着训练对数量的增加而显著增加。f的贡献取决于刺激域:当使用圆面积时(实验1),f的影响与r相似;然而,当使用运动速度(实验2)时,f对鸽子的行为没有明显的影响。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,鸽子的换位行为受强化历史(g)和实验刺激之间的关系(r和f)的影响;我们的模型为评估每个预测器对选择行为的相对贡献提供了一个原则性的方法。
{"title":"A three-component model of relational responding in the transposition paradigm.","authors":"Olga F Lazareva,&nbsp;Michael E Young,&nbsp;Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.1037/xan0000004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present a new model of transposition behavior that involves 3 predictors: (a) the disparity in generalized associative strength from the previously reinforced and nonreinforced stimuli (g) to the stimuli in the testing pair; (b) relational disparity (r), the difference in the logarithmically scaled sensory values of the testing stimuli; and (c) familiarity (f), the inverse of the Euclidean distance from the testing pair to the nearest training pair in 2-dimensional stimulus space. We evaluated the model with pigeons as subjects and with circle diameter (Experiment 1) and speed of motion (Experiment 2) as sensory dimensions. In each experiment, we presented 1, 2, or 3 training pairs as well as a wide range of testing pairs, including those comprising nonadjacent training stimuli. The control that was exerted by g did not depend on the number of training pairs and predicted behavior better than r and f after 1-pair training. In contrast, the influence of r increased dramatically with an increase in the number of training pairs. The contribution of f depended on the stimulus domain: When circle area was used (Experiment 1), the influence of f was similar to r; however, when speed of motion was used (Experiment 2), f had no discernible effect on pigeons' behavior. In sum, our results suggest that pigeons' transposition behavior is affected by both reinforcement history (g) and the relation between the experimental stimuli (r and f); our model provides a principled means for assessing the relative contribution of each predictor to choice behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000004","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31786537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 17
Dissociations in the effect of delay on object recognition: evidence for an associative model of recognition memory. 延迟对物体识别的影响中的解离:识别记忆的联想模型的证据。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000003
Shu K E Tam, Jasper Robinson, Dómhnall J Jennings, Charlotte Bonardi

Rats were administered 3 versions of an object recognition task: In the spontaneous object recognition task (SOR) animals discriminated between a familiar object and a novel object; in the temporal order task they discriminated between 2 familiar objects, 1 of which had been presented more recently than the other; and, in the object-in-place task, they discriminated among 4 previously presented objects, 2 of which were presented in the same locations as in preexposure and 2 in different but familiar locations. In each task animals were tested at 2 delays (5 min and 2 hr) between the sample and test phases in the SOR and object-in-place task, and between the 2 sample phases in the temporal order task. Performance in the SOR was poorer with the longer delay, whereas in the temporal order task performance improved with delay. There was no effect of delay on object-in-place performance. In addition the performance of animals with neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus was selectively impaired in the object-in-place task at the longer delay. These findings are interpreted within the framework of Wagner's (1981) model of memory.

给大鼠三个版本的物体识别任务:在自发物体识别任务(SOR)中,动物区分熟悉的物体和新物体;在时间顺序任务中,他们区分两个熟悉的物体,其中一个比另一个出现得更近;在物体就位任务中,他们区分了4个先前呈现的物体,其中2个与曝光前呈现的位置相同,另外2个呈现在不同但熟悉的位置。在每个任务中,动物在SOR和原位物体任务的样本阶段和测试阶段之间以及时间顺序任务的两个样本阶段之间进行2次延迟(5分钟和2小时)测试。SOR任务的表现随延迟的延长而变差,而时间顺序任务的表现随延迟的延长而提高。延迟对对象就地性能没有影响。此外,海马背侧神经毒性损伤的动物在物体就位任务中表现选择性受损。这些发现是在瓦格纳(1981)记忆模型的框架内解释的。
{"title":"Dissociations in the effect of delay on object recognition: evidence for an associative model of recognition memory.","authors":"Shu K E Tam,&nbsp;Jasper Robinson,&nbsp;Dómhnall J Jennings,&nbsp;Charlotte Bonardi","doi":"10.1037/xan0000003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000003","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rats were administered 3 versions of an object recognition task: In the spontaneous object recognition task (SOR) animals discriminated between a familiar object and a novel object; in the temporal order task they discriminated between 2 familiar objects, 1 of which had been presented more recently than the other; and, in the object-in-place task, they discriminated among 4 previously presented objects, 2 of which were presented in the same locations as in preexposure and 2 in different but familiar locations. In each task animals were tested at 2 delays (5 min and 2 hr) between the sample and test phases in the SOR and object-in-place task, and between the 2 sample phases in the temporal order task. Performance in the SOR was poorer with the longer delay, whereas in the temporal order task performance improved with delay. There was no effect of delay on object-in-place performance. In addition the performance of animals with neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus was selectively impaired in the object-in-place task at the longer delay. These findings are interpreted within the framework of Wagner's (1981) model of memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31703606","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 17
Relative salience versus relative validity: cue salience influences blocking in human associative learning. 相对显著性与相对效度:提示显著性影响人类联想学习中的阻塞。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000006
Mike E Le Pelley, Tom Beesley, Oren Griffiths

Two studies of human contingency learning investigated the influence of stimulus salience on the cue competition effect of blocking. These studies demonstrated that blocking (defined as a difference in responding to blocked and control cues) was greater for target cues that had high "semantic salience" than those of lower salience. Moreover participants showed weaker responding to high salience blocked cues than low salience blocked cues, but a corresponding difference was not observed for control cues. These findings suggest that the influence of relative salience on associative learning depends on the relative validity of the cues in question. Use of eye tracking in Experiment 2 demonstrated that participants' overt attention to cues was also influenced by both relative salience and relative validity. We describe three associative learning models, based on the attentional theory proposed by Mackintosh (1975), that are able to account for our key findings.

人类偶然性学习的两项研究探讨了刺激显著性对线索竞争阻滞效应的影响。这些研究表明,对于具有高“语义显著性”的目标线索,阻滞(定义为对被阻断线索和控制线索的反应差异)比显著性较低的目标线索更大。此外,受试者对高显著性障碍提示的反应弱于对低显著性障碍提示的反应,而对控制提示则没有相应的差异。这些发现表明,相对显著性对联想学习的影响取决于相关线索的相对有效性。实验2的眼动追踪表明,被试对线索的显性注意也受到相对显著性和相对效度的影响。我们描述了三种基于Mackintosh(1975)提出的注意理论的联想学习模型,它们能够解释我们的主要发现。
{"title":"Relative salience versus relative validity: cue salience influences blocking in human associative learning.","authors":"Mike E Le Pelley,&nbsp;Tom Beesley,&nbsp;Oren Griffiths","doi":"10.1037/xan0000006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000006","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two studies of human contingency learning investigated the influence of stimulus salience on the cue competition effect of blocking. These studies demonstrated that blocking (defined as a difference in responding to blocked and control cues) was greater for target cues that had high \"semantic salience\" than those of lower salience. Moreover participants showed weaker responding to high salience blocked cues than low salience blocked cues, but a corresponding difference was not observed for control cues. These findings suggest that the influence of relative salience on associative learning depends on the relative validity of the cues in question. Use of eye tracking in Experiment 2 demonstrated that participants' overt attention to cues was also influenced by both relative salience and relative validity. We describe three associative learning models, based on the attentional theory proposed by Mackintosh (1975), that are able to account for our key findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000006","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31786538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 21
The effect of a prior investment on choice: the sunk cost effect. 先前投资对选择的影响:沉没成本效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000007
Paula Magalhães, K Geoffrey White

We investigated the effect of prior investment on choice in pigeons, namely, the sunk cost effect, which is a tendency to continue an endeavor once a prior investment has been made, despite a better option being available. In a concurrent-chains procedure, pigeons chose between left and right keys in the choice phase leading to different work requirements in the outcome phase. Within each session, two components were signaled by red or green keys in the choice phase. Components were identical, except that in red components, the choice was preceded by a prior investment of 20 pecks on the left key, whereas in green components, the investment of 20 pecks was on the right key. Preference for the key with the prior investment was studied in a series of experiments in which we manipulated the absence or presence of the investment (Experiments 1a and 1b) and size of the investment (Experiments 2 and 3). We also investigated whether the bias observed was a result of carryover effects or of the sunk cost effect (Experiment 4). Overall, the results showed that choice was biased toward the alternative associated with the prior investment, consistent with the sunk cost effect, an effect that can be understood in terms of within-trial contrast and the delay-reduction hypothesis.

我们研究了先验投资对鸽子选择的影响,即沉没成本效应,即一旦有了先验投资,尽管有更好的选择,鸽子仍倾向于继续努力。在并发链过程中,鸽子在选择阶段在左键和右键之间进行选择,导致结果阶段的工作要求不同。在每个会话中,选择阶段的两个组成部分由红色或绿色键表示。组件是相同的,除了在红色组件中,在选择之前先在左键上投资20个啄,而在绿色组件中,在右键上投资20个啄。在一系列实验中,我们研究了对先前投资的关键偏好,在这些实验中,我们操纵了投资的缺失或存在(实验1a和1b)以及投资的规模(实验2和3)。我们还调查了观察到的偏见是由于延续效应还是沉没成本效应(实验4)。总体而言,结果表明,选择偏向于与先前投资相关的替代方案。与沉没成本效应一致,这种效应可以用试验内对比和延迟减少假设来理解。
{"title":"The effect of a prior investment on choice: the sunk cost effect.","authors":"Paula Magalhães,&nbsp;K Geoffrey White","doi":"10.1037/xan0000007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000007","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated the effect of prior investment on choice in pigeons, namely, the sunk cost effect, which is a tendency to continue an endeavor once a prior investment has been made, despite a better option being available. In a concurrent-chains procedure, pigeons chose between left and right keys in the choice phase leading to different work requirements in the outcome phase. Within each session, two components were signaled by red or green keys in the choice phase. Components were identical, except that in red components, the choice was preceded by a prior investment of 20 pecks on the left key, whereas in green components, the investment of 20 pecks was on the right key. Preference for the key with the prior investment was studied in a series of experiments in which we manipulated the absence or presence of the investment (Experiments 1a and 1b) and size of the investment (Experiments 2 and 3). We also investigated whether the bias observed was a result of carryover effects or of the sunk cost effect (Experiment 4). Overall, the results showed that choice was biased toward the alternative associated with the prior investment, consistent with the sunk cost effect, an effect that can be understood in terms of within-trial contrast and the delay-reduction hypothesis.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000007","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Persistence in extinction: the sunk time effect. 灭绝中的坚持:沉没时间效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000009
Paula Magalhães, K Geoffrey White

We investigated the sunk time effect, persistence in a nonpreferred option owing to prior investment of time in that option. Pigeons chose between two concurrently available keys-2 fixed-interval (FI) food requirements were arranged on 1 key, and an escape option, which terminated the current trial and started a new one, on a second key. One FI was longer than the other, and the shorter FI was always more probable on any given trial. In most conditions, the different FI schedules were not signaled. In this situation, the optimal behavior would be for pigeons to escape from the long FI once the duration equivalent to the short FI had elapsed without reinforcement. Several variables that could influence persistence or escape behavior were manipulated: the presence and absence of cues signaling the type of trial in effect (Experiments 1 and 2), extinction in the long interval (Experiments 3, 4, and 5), the intertrial-interval duration (Experiments 5 and 6), and the duration of the FI schedules (Experiments 1-6). Overall, the results showed that pigeons tend to persist and finish the current trial, even with extinction in the long interval, a result consistent with the sunk time effect.

我们研究了沉没时间效应,即由于先前在该期权上投入时间而对非首选期权的持久性。鸽子在两个同时可用的键之间进行选择-2固定间隔(FI)食物需求安排在一个键上,并在第二个键上安排逃避选项,该选项终止当前的试验并开始新的试验。一个FI比另一个更长,在任何给定的试验中,较短的FI总是更有可能。在大多数情况下,不同的FI时间表没有信号。在这种情况下,鸽子的最佳行为是在没有强化的情况下,一旦相当于短FI的持续时间过去,鸽子就会逃离长FI。几个可能影响持续或逃避行为的变量被操纵:提示有效试验类型的线索的存在和缺失(实验1和2),长间隔中的消失(实验3、4和5),间隔-间隔持续时间(实验5和6),以及FI时间表的持续时间(实验1-6)。总体而言,结果表明,鸽子倾向于坚持并完成当前的试验,即使在较长的间隔内灭绝,结果与沉没时间效应一致。
{"title":"Persistence in extinction: the sunk time effect.","authors":"Paula Magalhães,&nbsp;K Geoffrey White","doi":"10.1037/xan0000009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000009","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated the sunk time effect, persistence in a nonpreferred option owing to prior investment of time in that option. Pigeons chose between two concurrently available keys-2 fixed-interval (FI) food requirements were arranged on 1 key, and an escape option, which terminated the current trial and started a new one, on a second key. One FI was longer than the other, and the shorter FI was always more probable on any given trial. In most conditions, the different FI schedules were not signaled. In this situation, the optimal behavior would be for pigeons to escape from the long FI once the duration equivalent to the short FI had elapsed without reinforcement. Several variables that could influence persistence or escape behavior were manipulated: the presence and absence of cues signaling the type of trial in effect (Experiments 1 and 2), extinction in the long interval (Experiments 3, 4, and 5), the intertrial-interval duration (Experiments 5 and 6), and the duration of the FI schedules (Experiments 1-6). Overall, the results showed that pigeons tend to persist and finish the current trial, even with extinction in the long interval, a result consistent with the sunk time effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000009","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392989","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Editorial explaining the change in name of this journal to Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition. 解释本刊更名为《实验心理学杂志:动物学习与认知》的社论。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000015
Ralph R Miller

This editorial explains the reasoning behind The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes name change. This Journal started publication in 1975 as a result of a major reorganization of the American Psychological Association's basic science journals. To signal that expansion of interest, the name of the journal has been changed to Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition. This change is not meant to discourage submission of the types of manuscripts that have most frequently appeared in the journal in recent years but to encourage submission of papers across a broader range of topics.

这篇社论解释了《实验心理学杂志:动物行为过程》更名背后的原因。该杂志于1975年开始出版,这是美国心理协会基础科学期刊进行重大重组的结果。为了表明兴趣的扩大,杂志的名称已改为实验心理学杂志:动物学习与认知。这一变化并不是为了阻止近年来最频繁出现在期刊上的手稿类型的提交,而是为了鼓励提交更广泛主题的论文。
{"title":"Editorial explaining the change in name of this journal to Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition.","authors":"Ralph R Miller","doi":"10.1037/xan0000015","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000015","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This editorial explains the reasoning behind The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes name change. This Journal started publication in 1975 as a result of a major reorganization of the American Psychological Association's basic science journals. To signal that expansion of interest, the name of the journal has been changed to Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition. This change is not meant to discourage submission of the types of manuscripts that have most frequently appeared in the journal in recent years but to encourage submission of papers across a broader range of topics.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000015","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Suboptimal choice by pigeons may result from the diminishing effect of nonreinforcement. 鸽子的次优选择可能是非强化效应递减的结果。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000010
Jennifer R Laude, Jessica P Stagner, Thomas R Zentall

Pigeons prefer an alternative that provides them with a stimulus 20% of the time that predicts 10 pellets of food and a different stimulus 80% of the time that predicts 0 pellets, over an alternative that provides them with a stimulus that always predicts 3 pellets of food, even though the preferred alternative provides them with considerably less food. It appears that the stimulus that predicts 10 pellets acts as a strong conditioned reinforcer, despite the fact that the stimulus that predicts 0 pellets occurs 4 times as often. In the present research, we tested the hypothesis that early in training conditioned inhibition develops to the 0-pellet stimulus, but later in training it dissipates. We trained pigeons with a hue as the 10-pellet stimulus and a vertical line as the 0-pellet stimulus. To assess the inhibitory value of the vertical line, we compared responding to the 10-pellet hue with responding to the compound of the 10-pellet hue and the vertical line early in training and once again late in training, using both a within-subject design (Experiment 1) and a between-groups design (Experiment 2). We found that there was a significant reduction in inhibition between the early test (when pigeons chose optimally) and late test (when choice was suboptimal). Thus, the increase in suboptimal choice may result from the decline in inhibition to the 0-pellet stimulus. Implications for human gambling behavior are considered.

鸽子更喜欢在 20% 的时间里为它们提供预示 10 粒食物的刺激和在 80% 的时间里为它们提供预示 0 粒食物的不同刺激,而不是为它们提供总是预示 3 粒食物的刺激,尽管首选刺激为它们提供的食物要少得多。由此看来,尽管预示 10 粒食物的刺激出现的频率是预示 0 粒食物的刺激出现频率的 4 倍,但预示 10 粒食物的刺激还是起到了很强的条件强化作用。在本研究中,我们测试了这样一个假设:在训练早期,鸽子会对 0 粒食物的刺激产生条件性抑制,但在训练后期,这种抑制会消失。我们用一种色调作为 10 粒刺激物,用一条垂直线作为 0 粒刺激物对鸽子进行训练。为了评估垂直线的抑制价值,我们在训练初期和训练后期,分别采用受试者内设计(实验 1)和组间设计(实验 2),比较了鸽子对 10 粒颗粒色调的反应和对 10 粒颗粒色调与垂直线的复合反应。我们发现,在早期测试(当鸽子做出最佳选择时)和晚期测试(当鸽子做出次佳选择时)之间,抑制作用明显减弱。因此,次优选择的增加可能是由于对0颗粒刺激的抑制作用下降所致。该研究还考虑了对人类赌博行为的影响。
{"title":"Suboptimal choice by pigeons may result from the diminishing effect of nonreinforcement.","authors":"Jennifer R Laude, Jessica P Stagner, Thomas R Zentall","doi":"10.1037/xan0000010","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000010","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pigeons prefer an alternative that provides them with a stimulus 20% of the time that predicts 10 pellets of food and a different stimulus 80% of the time that predicts 0 pellets, over an alternative that provides them with a stimulus that always predicts 3 pellets of food, even though the preferred alternative provides them with considerably less food. It appears that the stimulus that predicts 10 pellets acts as a strong conditioned reinforcer, despite the fact that the stimulus that predicts 0 pellets occurs 4 times as often. In the present research, we tested the hypothesis that early in training conditioned inhibition develops to the 0-pellet stimulus, but later in training it dissipates. We trained pigeons with a hue as the 10-pellet stimulus and a vertical line as the 0-pellet stimulus. To assess the inhibitory value of the vertical line, we compared responding to the 10-pellet hue with responding to the compound of the 10-pellet hue and the vertical line early in training and once again late in training, using both a within-subject design (Experiment 1) and a between-groups design (Experiment 2). We found that there was a significant reduction in inhibition between the early test (when pigeons chose optimally) and late test (when choice was suboptimal). Thus, the increase in suboptimal choice may result from the decline in inhibition to the 0-pellet stimulus. Implications for human gambling behavior are considered.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4330565/pdf/nihms-555621.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Renewal effects in interference between outcomes as measured by a cued response reaction time task: further evidence for associative retrieval models. 由提示反应反应时间任务测量的结果间干扰的更新效应:联想检索模型的进一步证据。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2013-10-01 Epub Date: 2013-07-15 DOI: 10.1037/a0033528
Pedro L Cobos, Estrella González-Martín, Sergio Varona-Moya, Francisco J López

Two experiments demonstrated renewal effects in interference between outcomes in human participants. Experiment 1 revealed a XYX renewal effect, whereas Experiment 2 showed a XYZ renewal effect. The results from both experiments conformed to Bouton's (1993) theory of interference and recovery from interference, and contradicted the predictions derived from alternative accounts. Unlike previous demonstration of renewal effects, a cued response reaction time (RT) task was used, able to detect the effects of fast retrieval processes based on associative activation and that allowed little impact of inferential reasoning.

两个实验证明了人类参与者在结果之间的干扰中的更新效应。实验1显示XYZ更新效应,而实验2显示XYZ更新效应。这两个实验的结果都符合Bouton(1993)的干扰和干扰恢复理论,并与其他说法的预测相矛盾。不同于以往的更新效应的论证,我们使用了一个线索反应时间(RT)任务,能够检测基于联想激活的快速检索过程的影响,并且允许推理推理的影响很小。
{"title":"Renewal effects in interference between outcomes as measured by a cued response reaction time task: further evidence for associative retrieval models.","authors":"Pedro L Cobos,&nbsp;Estrella González-Martín,&nbsp;Sergio Varona-Moya,&nbsp;Francisco J López","doi":"10.1037/a0033528","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033528","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two experiments demonstrated renewal effects in interference between outcomes in human participants. Experiment 1 revealed a XYX renewal effect, whereas Experiment 2 showed a XYZ renewal effect. The results from both experiments conformed to Bouton's (1993) theory of interference and recovery from interference, and contradicted the predictions derived from alternative accounts. Unlike previous demonstration of renewal effects, a cued response reaction time (RT) task was used, able to detect the effects of fast retrieval processes based on associative activation and that allowed little impact of inferential reasoning.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0033528","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31581068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Does constraining field of view prevent extraction of geometric cues for humans during virtual-environment reorientation? 在虚拟环境重定向过程中,限制视场是否会阻止人类提取几何线索?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2013-10-01 Epub Date: 2013-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/a0032543
Bradley R Sturz, Zachary A Kilday, Kent D Bodily

Environment size has been shown to influence the reliance on local and global geometric cues during reorientation. Unless changes in environment size are produced by manipulating length and width proportionally, changes in environment size are confounded by the amount of the environment that is visible from a single vantage point. Yet, the influence of the amount of the environment that is visible from any single vantage point on the use of local and global geometric cues remains unknown. We manipulated the amount of an environment that was visually available to participants by manipulating field of view (FOV) in a virtual environment orientation task. Two groups of participants were trained in a trapezoid-shaped enclosure to find a location that was uniquely specified by both local and global geometric cues. One group (FOV 50°) had visually less of the environment available to them from any one perspective compared to another group (FOV 100°). Following training, we presented both groups with a control test along with three novel-shaped environments. Testing assessed the use of global geometry in isolation, in alignment with local geometry, or in conflict with local geometry. Results (confirmed by a follow-up experiment) indicated that constraining FOV prevented extraction of geometric properties and relationships of space and resulted in an inability to use either global or local geometric cues for reorientation.

环境大小已被证明会影响重新定向过程中对局部和全局几何线索的依赖。除非通过按比例操纵长度和宽度来产生环境大小的变化,否则环境大小的变化会与从单个有利位置可见的环境数量相混淆。然而,从任何单一有利位置可见的环境数量对使用局部和全局几何线索的影响仍然未知。在虚拟环境定向任务中,我们通过操纵视场(FOV)来操纵参与者在视觉上可用的环境数量。两组参与者在一个梯形的圈地中接受训练,以找到一个由局部和全局几何线索唯一指定的位置。与另一组(视场视角为100°)相比,一组(视场视角为50°)从任何一个角度看都能看到更少的环境。训练结束后,我们给两组人都做了一个对照测试和三个新形状的环境。测试评估了全局几何在孤立、与局部几何对齐或与局部几何冲突情况下的使用情况。结果(通过后续实验证实)表明,限制视场阻碍了几何属性和空间关系的提取,导致无法使用全局或局部几何线索进行重新定向。
{"title":"Does constraining field of view prevent extraction of geometric cues for humans during virtual-environment reorientation?","authors":"Bradley R Sturz,&nbsp;Zachary A Kilday,&nbsp;Kent D Bodily","doi":"10.1037/a0032543","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032543","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Environment size has been shown to influence the reliance on local and global geometric cues during reorientation. Unless changes in environment size are produced by manipulating length and width proportionally, changes in environment size are confounded by the amount of the environment that is visible from a single vantage point. Yet, the influence of the amount of the environment that is visible from any single vantage point on the use of local and global geometric cues remains unknown. We manipulated the amount of an environment that was visually available to participants by manipulating field of view (FOV) in a virtual environment orientation task. Two groups of participants were trained in a trapezoid-shaped enclosure to find a location that was uniquely specified by both local and global geometric cues. One group (FOV 50°) had visually less of the environment available to them from any one perspective compared to another group (FOV 100°). Following training, we presented both groups with a control test along with three novel-shaped environments. Testing assessed the use of global geometry in isolation, in alignment with local geometry, or in conflict with local geometry. Results (confirmed by a follow-up experiment) indicated that constraining FOV prevented extraction of geometric properties and relationships of space and resulted in an inability to use either global or local geometric cues for reorientation.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0032543","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31546797","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
Pigeons use low rather than high spatial frequency information to make visual category discriminations. 鸽子使用低而不是高的空间频率信息来进行视觉类别区分。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2013-10-01 Epub Date: 2013-07-08 DOI: 10.1037/a0033104
Stephen E G Lea, Guido De Filippo, Ruth Dakin, Christina Meier

Pigeons were trained to discriminate photographs of cat faces from dog faces. They were then presented with test stimuli involving high- and low-pass spatial frequency filtering. Discrimination was maintained with both types of filtered stimuli, though it was increasingly impaired the more information was filtered out, and high-pass filtering impaired discrimination more than low-pass filtering. The pigeons were then exposed to hybrid stimuli in which high-pass filtered dog faces were combined with low-pass filtered cat faces, and vice versa. Response to hybrid stimuli was determined more by the low spatial frequency content than by the high-frequency content, whereas humans viewing the same stimuli at corresponding viewing distance respond more strongly to the high-frequency content. These results are unexpected given that, compared with humans, pigeons' behavior tends to be controlled by the local details of visual stimuli rather than their global appearance.

鸽子被训练来区分猫脸和狗脸的照片。然后,他们接受了包括高通和低通空间频率滤波的测试刺激。两种类型的过滤刺激都保持了辨别能力,但随着信息被过滤掉的越多,辨别能力受到的损害越大,高通过滤比低通过滤对辨别能力的损害更大。然后将鸽子暴露在混合刺激下,其中高通过滤的狗脸与低通过滤的猫脸相结合,反之亦然。低空间频率内容对混合刺激的响应比对高空间频率内容的影响更大,而在相同空间距离观看相同刺激的人对高空间频率内容的响应更强。这些结果是出乎意料的,因为与人类相比,鸽子的行为往往是由视觉刺激的局部细节控制的,而不是它们的全局外观。
{"title":"Pigeons use low rather than high spatial frequency information to make visual category discriminations.","authors":"Stephen E G Lea,&nbsp;Guido De Filippo,&nbsp;Ruth Dakin,&nbsp;Christina Meier","doi":"10.1037/a0033104","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033104","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pigeons were trained to discriminate photographs of cat faces from dog faces. They were then presented with test stimuli involving high- and low-pass spatial frequency filtering. Discrimination was maintained with both types of filtered stimuli, though it was increasingly impaired the more information was filtered out, and high-pass filtering impaired discrimination more than low-pass filtering. The pigeons were then exposed to hybrid stimuli in which high-pass filtered dog faces were combined with low-pass filtered cat faces, and vice versa. Response to hybrid stimuli was determined more by the low spatial frequency content than by the high-frequency content, whereas humans viewing the same stimuli at corresponding viewing distance respond more strongly to the high-frequency content. These results are unexpected given that, compared with humans, pigeons' behavior tends to be controlled by the local details of visual stimuli rather than their global appearance.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2013-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0033104","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31563363","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1