首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Visual search and attention in blue jays (Cyanocitta cristata): Associative cuing and sequential priming. 冠蓝鸦的视觉搜索和注意:联想线索和顺序启动。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000019
Kazuhiro Goto, Alan B Bond, Marianna Burks, Alan C Kamil

Visual search for complex natural targets requires focal attention, either cued by predictive stimulus associations or primed by a representation of the most recently detected target. Because both processes can focus visual attention, cuing and priming were compared in an operant search task to evaluate their relative impacts on performance and to determine the nature of their interaction in combined treatments. Blue jays were trained to search for pairs of alternative targets among distractors. Informative or ambiguous color cues were provided before each trial, and targets were presented either in homogeneous blocked sequences or in constrained random order. Initial task acquisition was facilitated by priming in general, but was significantly retarded when targets were both cued and primed, indicating that the two processes interfered with each other during training. At asymptote, attentional effects were manifested mainly in inhibition, increasing latency in miscued trials and decreasing accuracy on primed trials following an unexpected target switch. A combination of cuing and priming was found to interfere with performance in such unexpected trials, apparently a result of the limited capacity of working memory. Because the ecological factors that promote priming or cuing are rather disparate, it is not clear whether they ever simultaneously contribute to natural predatory search.

对复杂的自然目标的视觉搜索需要集中注意力,或者由预测刺激关联提示,或者由最近检测到的目标的表示启动。由于这两个过程都可以集中视觉注意力,我们在一个操作性搜索任务中比较了提示和启动,以评估它们对表现的相对影响,并确定它们在联合治疗中相互作用的性质。蓝鸦被训练在干扰物中寻找成对的替代目标。在每次试验前提供信息或模糊的颜色线索,并以均匀的阻塞序列或约束随机顺序呈现目标。初始任务习得在一般情况下受到启动的促进,但当目标同时受到提示和启动时,初始任务习得受到显著阻碍,表明两个过程在训练过程中相互干扰。在渐近线上,注意效应主要表现为误用试验的抑制作用、延迟时间的增加以及意外目标转换后启动试验的准确性降低。在这种意想不到的实验中,提示和启动的组合被发现会干扰表现,这显然是工作记忆容量有限的结果。由于促进启动或提示的生态因素相当不同,因此尚不清楚它们是否同时有助于自然的掠夺性搜索。
{"title":"Visual search and attention in blue jays (Cyanocitta cristata): Associative cuing and sequential priming.","authors":"Kazuhiro Goto, Alan B Bond, Marianna Burks, Alan C Kamil","doi":"10.1037/xan0000019","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xan0000019","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual search for complex natural targets requires focal attention, either cued by predictive stimulus associations or primed by a representation of the most recently detected target. Because both processes can focus visual attention, cuing and priming were compared in an operant search task to evaluate their relative impacts on performance and to determine the nature of their interaction in combined treatments. Blue jays were trained to search for pairs of alternative targets among distractors. Informative or ambiguous color cues were provided before each trial, and targets were presented either in homogeneous blocked sequences or in constrained random order. Initial task acquisition was facilitated by priming in general, but was significantly retarded when targets were both cued and primed, indicating that the two processes interfered with each other during training. At asymptote, attentional effects were manifested mainly in inhibition, increasing latency in miscued trials and decreasing accuracy on primed trials following an unexpected target switch. A combination of cuing and priming was found to interfere with performance in such unexpected trials, apparently a result of the limited capacity of working memory. Because the ecological factors that promote priming or cuing are rather disparate, it is not clear whether they ever simultaneously contribute to natural predatory search.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000019","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Rats respond for information: Metacognition in a rodent? 大鼠对信息的反应:啮齿动物的元认知?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000018
Chelsea R Kirk, Neil McMillan, William A Roberts

In 2 experiments, rats were trained to press a centrally located lever that delivered immediate food reinforcement and turned on a light signal that indicated the location of a further food reward. After rats learned to press the lever and use the light cue to find food, immediate reinforcement for lever pressing was discontinued. In Experiment 1, rats continued to press the lever for information about the location of reward in a T-maze, but control groups yoked to the experimental group for amount of reward, and conditioned reinforcement showed complete extinction of lever pressing. Rats tested on an 8-arm radial maze in Experiment 2 also continued to press a lever that did not yield immediate reinforcement but provided a light cue indicating which randomly chosen arm of the maze contained food; lever pressing declined significantly, however, when the same arm contained food on every trial. Comparisons of testing conditions between and within experiments suggested that probability of lever pressing increased as the amount of information gained increased. The comparative implications of these findings for metacognition are discussed.

在两个实验中,老鼠被训练去按一个位于中央的杠杆,这个杠杆会立即提供食物强化,并打开一个光信号,指示下一步食物奖励的位置。在老鼠学会按压杠杆并利用光线线索寻找食物后,立即加强按压杠杆的行为就停止了。在实验1中,大鼠在t迷宫中继续按压杠杆以获取奖励位置信息,而对照组则依附于实验组以获取奖励数量,条件强化表现为完全消除按压杠杆的行为。在实验2中,在8臂放射状迷宫中测试的大鼠也继续按下一个杠杆,这个杠杆不会立即产生强化,但会提供一个灯光提示,表明迷宫中随机选择的哪条手臂含有食物;然而,在每次试验中,当同一手臂含有食物时,杠杆按压明显下降。实验之间和实验内的测试条件比较表明,杠杆按压的概率随着获得的信息量的增加而增加。讨论了这些发现对元认知的比较意义。
{"title":"Rats respond for information: Metacognition in a rodent?","authors":"Chelsea R Kirk,&nbsp;Neil McMillan,&nbsp;William A Roberts","doi":"10.1037/xan0000018","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000018","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In 2 experiments, rats were trained to press a centrally located lever that delivered immediate food reinforcement and turned on a light signal that indicated the location of a further food reward. After rats learned to press the lever and use the light cue to find food, immediate reinforcement for lever pressing was discontinued. In Experiment 1, rats continued to press the lever for information about the location of reward in a T-maze, but control groups yoked to the experimental group for amount of reward, and conditioned reinforcement showed complete extinction of lever pressing. Rats tested on an 8-arm radial maze in Experiment 2 also continued to press a lever that did not yield immediate reinforcement but provided a light cue indicating which randomly chosen arm of the maze contained food; lever pressing declined significantly, however, when the same arm contained food on every trial. Comparisons of testing conditions between and within experiments suggested that probability of lever pressing increased as the amount of information gained increased. The comparative implications of these findings for metacognition are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000018","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 31
Not so bird-brained: Pigeons show what-where-when memory both as time of day and how long ago. 鸽子的脑子可不那么笨:鸽子的“何时何地”记忆既包括一天的时间,也包括很久以前的时间。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000016
Julia E Meyers-Manor, J Bruce Overmier, Daniel W Hatfield, Jo Croswell

In an ever-changing world, the ability to track what significant events occur and where and when is beneficial to a variety of animal species. The purpose of the present studies was to assess the presence of this ability to track what-where-when memory in pigeons based both on when during the day the events occurred and how long ago events occurred. In these studies, pigeons were trained to discriminate between two foods that differed in quality (what), making one more "attractive" than the other. The birds were required to alter their choice of keylights (where) to get these differential foods based on the time of day (Experiments 1-2) or how long ago (Experiments 3-5) they were in a session (when). Pigeons were able to correctly choose the key that yielded the "attractive" food using both time of day and how long ago, indicating a what-where-when memory. However, the pigeons failed to transfer this knowledge to a novel situation, showing limited flexibility in use of the learned what-where-when information. These findings suggest that pigeons have abilities to track what-where-when events as do caching birds and other animal species, but perhaps represented in a more rigid manner.

在一个不断变化的世界里,追踪重大事件发生的地点和时间的能力对各种动物物种都是有益的。目前研究的目的是评估鸽子这种根据事件发生的时间和事件发生的时间来追踪“何时何地”记忆的能力。在这些研究中,鸽子被训练来区分两种质量不同的食物,使一种比另一种更“有吸引力”。这些鸟被要求根据一天中的时间(实验1-2)或多久前(实验3-5)改变它们对钥匙的选择(在哪里)来获得这些不同的食物。鸽子能够通过一天中的时间和多久以前正确地选择产生“吸引人的”食物的钥匙,这表明它们有“何时何地”的记忆。然而,鸽子无法将这些知识转移到一个新的环境中,在使用学习到的“何时何地”信息方面表现出有限的灵活性。这些发现表明,鸽子有能力像缓存鸟类和其他动物一样,在什么时间、什么地点追踪事件,但可能以一种更严格的方式表现出来。
{"title":"Not so bird-brained: Pigeons show what-where-when memory both as time of day and how long ago.","authors":"Julia E Meyers-Manor,&nbsp;J Bruce Overmier,&nbsp;Daniel W Hatfield,&nbsp;Jo Croswell","doi":"10.1037/xan0000016","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000016","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an ever-changing world, the ability to track what significant events occur and where and when is beneficial to a variety of animal species. The purpose of the present studies was to assess the presence of this ability to track what-where-when memory in pigeons based both on when during the day the events occurred and how long ago events occurred. In these studies, pigeons were trained to discriminate between two foods that differed in quality (what), making one more \"attractive\" than the other. The birds were required to alter their choice of keylights (where) to get these differential foods based on the time of day (Experiments 1-2) or how long ago (Experiments 3-5) they were in a session (when). Pigeons were able to correctly choose the key that yielded the \"attractive\" food using both time of day and how long ago, indicating a what-where-when memory. However, the pigeons failed to transfer this knowledge to a novel situation, showing limited flexibility in use of the learned what-where-when information. These findings suggest that pigeons have abilities to track what-where-when events as do caching birds and other animal species, but perhaps represented in a more rigid manner.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000016","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31988637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Perceptual learning and inversion effects: Recognition of prototype-defined familiar checkerboards. 知觉学习和反转效应:原型定义的熟悉棋盘的识别。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000013
Ciro Civile, Di Zhao, Yixuan Ku, Heike Elchlepp, Aureliu Lavric, I P L McLaren

The face inversion effect is a defection in performance in recognizing inverted faces compared with faces presented in their usual upright orientation typically believed to be specific for facial stimuli. McLaren (1997) was able to demonstrate that (a) an inversion effect could be obtained with exemplars drawn from a familiar category, such that upright exemplars were better discriminated than inverted exemplars; and (b) that the inversion effect required that the familiar category be prototype-defined. In this article, we replicate and extend these findings. We show that the inversion effect can be obtained in a standard old/new recognition memory paradigm, demonstrate that it is contingent on familiarization with a prototype-defined category, and establish that the effect is made up of two components. We confirm the advantage for upright exemplars drawn from a familiar, prototype-defined category, and show that there is a disadvantage for inverted exemplars drawn from this category relative to suitable controls. We also provide evidence that there is an N170 event-related potential signature for this effect. These results allow us to integrate a theory of perceptual learning originally proposed by McLaren, Kaye, and Mackintosh (1989) with explanations of the face inversion effect, first reported by Yin.

人脸倒立效应是识别倒立人脸的一种缺陷,与通常被认为是特定于面部刺激的直立方向的人脸相比。McLaren(1997)能够证明:(a)从熟悉的类别中提取的样本可以获得反转效应,即直立的样本比倒置的样本更容易被识别;(b)反转效应要求熟悉的类别是原型定义的。在本文中,我们复制并扩展了这些发现。我们证明倒置效应可以在标准的新旧识别记忆范式中获得,证明它取决于对原型定义的类别的熟悉程度,并确定该效应由两个组成部分组成。我们确认了从熟悉的、原型定义的类别中提取的直立样本的优势,并表明相对于合适的控制,从该类别中提取的倒置样本存在劣势。我们还提供了证据,表明这种效应存在与N170事件相关的潜在特征。这些结果使我们能够将最初由McLaren, Kaye和Mackintosh(1989)提出的感知学习理论与Yin首次报道的面部反转效应的解释结合起来。
{"title":"Perceptual learning and inversion effects: Recognition of prototype-defined familiar checkerboards.","authors":"Ciro Civile,&nbsp;Di Zhao,&nbsp;Yixuan Ku,&nbsp;Heike Elchlepp,&nbsp;Aureliu Lavric,&nbsp;I P L McLaren","doi":"10.1037/xan0000013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000013","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The face inversion effect is a defection in performance in recognizing inverted faces compared with faces presented in their usual upright orientation typically believed to be specific for facial stimuli. McLaren (1997) was able to demonstrate that (a) an inversion effect could be obtained with exemplars drawn from a familiar category, such that upright exemplars were better discriminated than inverted exemplars; and (b) that the inversion effect required that the familiar category be prototype-defined. In this article, we replicate and extend these findings. We show that the inversion effect can be obtained in a standard old/new recognition memory paradigm, demonstrate that it is contingent on familiarization with a prototype-defined category, and establish that the effect is made up of two components. We confirm the advantage for upright exemplars drawn from a familiar, prototype-defined category, and show that there is a disadvantage for inverted exemplars drawn from this category relative to suitable controls. We also provide evidence that there is an N170 event-related potential signature for this effect. These results allow us to integrate a theory of perceptual learning originally proposed by McLaren, Kaye, and Mackintosh (1989) with explanations of the face inversion effect, first reported by Yin.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000013","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31977726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
Scene gist categorization by pigeons. 现场要点按鸽子分类。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000014
Kimberly Kirkpatrick, Tannis Bilton, Bruce C Hansen, Lester C Loschky

Scene gist categorization in humans is rapid, accurate, and tuned to the statistical regularities in the visual world. However, no studies have investigated whether scene gist categorization is a general process shared across species, or whether it may be influenced by species-specific adaptive specializations relying on specific low-level scene statistical regularities of the environment. Although pigeons form many types of categorical judgments, little research has examined pigeons' scene categorization, and no studies have examined pigeons' ability to do so rapidly. In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained to discriminate between either 2 basic-level categories (beach vs. mountain) or a superordinate-level natural versus a manmade scene category distinction (beach vs. street). The birds learned both tasks to a high degree of accuracy and transferred their discrimination to novel images. Furthermore, the pigeons successfully discriminated stimuli presented in the 0.2- to 0.35-s duration range. Therefore, pigeons, a highly divergent species from humans, are also capable of rapid scene categorization, but they require longer stimulus durations than humans. Experiment 2 examined whether pigeons make use of complex statistical regularities during scene gist categorization across multiple viewpoints. Pigeons were trained with the 2 natural categories from Experiment 1 (beach vs. mountain) with zenith (90°), bird's eye (45°), and terrestrial (0°) viewpoints. A sizable portion of the variability in pigeon categorization performance was explained by the systematic variation in scene category-specific statistical regularities, as with humans. Thus, rapid scene categorization is a process that is shared across pigeons and humans, but shows a degree of adaptive specialization.

人类对场景主旨的分类是快速、准确的,并且顺应了视觉世界的统计规律。然而,目前还没有研究调查场景依据分类是否是物种间共有的一般过程,或者它是否可能受到依赖于特定低水平场景环境统计规律的物种特异性适应性特化的影响。尽管鸽子形成了许多类型的分类判断,但很少有研究考察鸽子的场景分类,也没有研究考察鸽子快速分类的能力。在实验1中,鸽子被训练区分两个基本级别的类别(海滩与山)或一个高级级别的自然与人工场景类别区分(海滩与街道)。这些鸟以很高的准确度学会了这两项任务,并将它们的辨别能力转移到了新的图像上。此外,在0.2 ~ 0.35秒的持续时间范围内,鸽子能够成功地识别刺激。因此,作为与人类高度分化的物种,鸽子也具有快速场景分类的能力,但它们需要比人类更长的刺激时间。实验2考察了鸽子在多视点场景主旨分类中是否利用了复杂的统计规律。实验1中的2个自然类别(海滩vs山)训练鸽子,分别为天顶(90°)、鸟眼(45°)和陆地(0°)视点。与人类一样,鸽子分类表现的很大一部分可变性可以用场景类别特定统计规律的系统变化来解释。因此,快速场景分类是鸽子和人类共同的过程,但表现出一定程度的适应性专业化。
{"title":"Scene gist categorization by pigeons.","authors":"Kimberly Kirkpatrick,&nbsp;Tannis Bilton,&nbsp;Bruce C Hansen,&nbsp;Lester C Loschky","doi":"10.1037/xan0000014","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000014","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Scene gist categorization in humans is rapid, accurate, and tuned to the statistical regularities in the visual world. However, no studies have investigated whether scene gist categorization is a general process shared across species, or whether it may be influenced by species-specific adaptive specializations relying on specific low-level scene statistical regularities of the environment. Although pigeons form many types of categorical judgments, little research has examined pigeons' scene categorization, and no studies have examined pigeons' ability to do so rapidly. In Experiment 1, pigeons were trained to discriminate between either 2 basic-level categories (beach vs. mountain) or a superordinate-level natural versus a manmade scene category distinction (beach vs. street). The birds learned both tasks to a high degree of accuracy and transferred their discrimination to novel images. Furthermore, the pigeons successfully discriminated stimuli presented in the 0.2- to 0.35-s duration range. Therefore, pigeons, a highly divergent species from humans, are also capable of rapid scene categorization, but they require longer stimulus durations than humans. Experiment 2 examined whether pigeons make use of complex statistical regularities during scene gist categorization across multiple viewpoints. Pigeons were trained with the 2 natural categories from Experiment 1 (beach vs. mountain) with zenith (90°), bird's eye (45°), and terrestrial (0°) viewpoints. A sizable portion of the variability in pigeon categorization performance was explained by the systematic variation in scene category-specific statistical regularities, as with humans. Thus, rapid scene categorization is a process that is shared across pigeons and humans, but shows a degree of adaptive specialization.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000014","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31977724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Pigeons learn signal-food intervals independently in a multiple peak procedure. 鸽子在一个多峰过程中独立地学习信号-食物间隔。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000011
Nathaniel Rice, Randolph C Grace, Elizabeth G E Kyonka

Previous research has shown rapid learning of multiple temporal relations between signals and food by pigeons when these relations are changed unpredictably each session (Kyonka & Grace, 2007). The goal of the present study was to test whether contextual temporal cues-that is, an alternative signal-food delay that was a valid predictor of a target signal-food delay-facilitated acquisition by the target contingency. Four pigeons responded in a multiple peak-interval procedure in which red and green keys signaled separate fixed-interval (FI) schedules with occasional extinction probes (peak trials). The schedule parameters of the FIs either summed to 30 s (correlated condition; ρ = -1.0) or were not restricted to sum to 30 s (uncorrelated condition; ρ = 0.0). Comparing stop times obtained from peak trials in the 2 conditions revealed no effect of context: Temporal control of responding was acquired at the same rate and with the same precision regardless of whether the schedule values were correlated. These results suggest that pigeons learn about multiple signal-food delays independently.

先前的研究表明,当信号和食物之间的多重时间关系在每次会话中不可预测地发生变化时,鸽子会快速学习这些关系(Kyonka & Grace, 2007)。本研究的目的是测试上下文时间线索(即,替代信号-食物延迟)是否能有效预测目标信号-食物延迟,从而促进目标偶然性的习得。4只鸽子在多个峰值间隔过程中做出反应,其中红色和绿色键表示单独的固定间隔(FI)时间表,并偶尔进行消光探测(峰值试验)。FIs的调度参数或求和为30s(相关条件);ρ = -1.0)或不限于总和为30s(不相关条件;ρ = 0.0)。比较两种条件下峰值试验的停止时间,结果显示情境没有影响:无论进度值是否相关,反应的时间控制都以相同的速率和相同的精度获得。这些结果表明,鸽子独立地学习了多种信号食物延迟。
{"title":"Pigeons learn signal-food intervals independently in a multiple peak procedure.","authors":"Nathaniel Rice,&nbsp;Randolph C Grace,&nbsp;Elizabeth G E Kyonka","doi":"10.1037/xan0000011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000011","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has shown rapid learning of multiple temporal relations between signals and food by pigeons when these relations are changed unpredictably each session (Kyonka & Grace, 2007). The goal of the present study was to test whether contextual temporal cues-that is, an alternative signal-food delay that was a valid predictor of a target signal-food delay-facilitated acquisition by the target contingency. Four pigeons responded in a multiple peak-interval procedure in which red and green keys signaled separate fixed-interval (FI) schedules with occasional extinction probes (peak trials). The schedule parameters of the FIs either summed to 30 s (correlated condition; ρ = -1.0) or were not restricted to sum to 30 s (uncorrelated condition; ρ = 0.0). Comparing stop times obtained from peak trials in the 2 conditions revealed no effect of context: Temporal control of responding was acquired at the same rate and with the same precision regardless of whether the schedule values were correlated. These results suggest that pigeons learn about multiple signal-food delays independently.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000011","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31977727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Pigeons' tracking of relevant attributes in categorization learning. 分类学习中鸽子对相关属性的跟踪。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000022
Leyre Castro, Edward A Wasserman

Most theories and experimental investigations of discrimination learning and categorization, in both humans and animals, hypothesize that attention must be allocated to the relevant attributes of the training stimuli for learning to occur. Attention has conventionally been inferred after learning has transpired rather than examined while learning is transpiring. We presented pigeons with a visual categorization task in which we monitored their choice accuracy through their responses to different report buttons; critically, we tracked the location of the pigeons' pecks to both the relevant and irrelevant attributes of the training stimuli using touchscreen technology, in order to find out where the birds may have been attending during the course of categorization learning. Pigeons readily mastered the categorization task; most importantly, as training progressed, they increasingly concentrated their pecks on the relevant features of the category exemplars, suggesting that the birds were tracking the relevant information to solve the task. When either new irrelevant features were introduced (Experiment 1) or when new relevant features were introduced and later the discriminative value of these new relevant features was reversed (Experiment 2), pigeons' choice accuracy and peck tracking were strongly affected. These results help elucidate the dynamics and interplay of attention and learning; they also suggest that peck tracking can be a suitable measure of the allocation of attention in pigeons, much as eyetracking is deemed to be a suitable measure of attention in humans.

在人类和动物中,大多数关于辨别学习和分类的理论和实验研究都假设,必须将注意力分配到训练刺激的相关属性上,才能发生学习。注意通常是在学习发生后推断出来的,而不是在学习发生时进行检查。我们给鸽子一个视觉分类任务,通过它们对不同报告按钮的反应来监测它们选择的准确性;至关重要的是,我们使用触摸屏技术跟踪鸽子的啄食位置与训练刺激的相关和不相关属性,以找出鸟类在分类学习过程中可能参加的位置。鸽子很容易就掌握了分类任务;最重要的是,随着训练的进行,它们越来越多地将啄食集中在类别样本的相关特征上,这表明鸟类正在追踪相关信息来解决任务。无论是引入新的不相关特征(实验1),还是引入新的相关特征,然后将这些新相关特征的判别值颠倒(实验2),鸽子的选择准确性和啄跟踪都受到强烈影响。这些结果有助于阐明注意力和学习的动态和相互作用;他们还认为,轻啄跟踪可以作为衡量鸽子注意力分配的合适方法,就像眼睛跟踪被认为是衡量人类注意力分配的合适方法一样。
{"title":"Pigeons' tracking of relevant attributes in categorization learning.","authors":"Leyre Castro,&nbsp;Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.1037/xan0000022","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000022","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most theories and experimental investigations of discrimination learning and categorization, in both humans and animals, hypothesize that attention must be allocated to the relevant attributes of the training stimuli for learning to occur. Attention has conventionally been inferred after learning has transpired rather than examined while learning is transpiring. We presented pigeons with a visual categorization task in which we monitored their choice accuracy through their responses to different report buttons; critically, we tracked the location of the pigeons' pecks to both the relevant and irrelevant attributes of the training stimuli using touchscreen technology, in order to find out where the birds may have been attending during the course of categorization learning. Pigeons readily mastered the categorization task; most importantly, as training progressed, they increasingly concentrated their pecks on the relevant features of the category exemplars, suggesting that the birds were tracking the relevant information to solve the task. When either new irrelevant features were introduced (Experiment 1) or when new relevant features were introduced and later the discriminative value of these new relevant features was reversed (Experiment 2), pigeons' choice accuracy and peck tracking were strongly affected. These results help elucidate the dynamics and interplay of attention and learning; they also suggest that peck tracking can be a suitable measure of the allocation of attention in pigeons, much as eyetracking is deemed to be a suitable measure of attention in humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000022","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
The role of local, distal, and global information in latent spatial learning. 局部、远端和全局信息在潜在空间学习中的作用。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-04-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000017
Kerry E Gilroy, John M Pearce

In 4 experiments that investigated latent spatial learning, rats were repeatedly placed on a submerged platform in a corner of a square swimming pool with walls of different brightness. When they were subsequently released into the pool for a test trial in the absence of the platform, they spent the majority of time in the corner used for placement training-the correct corner. This effect was observed in Experiment 1, even when the test trial took place in a transformed version of the training arena. Experiments 2 and 3 indicated that the correct corner was identified by local cues based on the walls creating the corner. Experiment 4 demonstrated that distal cues created by the two walls that did not surround the platform during placement training could also be used to identify the correct corner. There was no evidence of learning about the relationship between global cues provided by the entire arena and the goal. The absence of the opportunity to develop instrumental, stimulus-response associations during placement training indicates that stimulus-stimulus associations acquired during this training were sufficient to guide rats to the platform when they were eventually released into the pool.

在4个研究潜在空间学习的实验中,大鼠被反复放置在一个方形游泳池角落的水下平台上,泳池的墙壁亮度不同。当它们随后被释放到没有平台的水池中进行测试时,它们大部分时间都呆在用于安置训练的角落——正确的角落。这种效应在实验1中被观察到,即使是在训练场地的一个转换版本中。实验2和3表明,正确的角是根据形成角的墙壁的局部线索识别的。实验4表明,在定位训练中,由两面墙产生的远端线索也可以用来识别正确的角。没有证据表明学习了整个竞技场提供的全局线索和目标之间的关系。在放置训练中没有机会发展工具性、刺激-反应关联表明,在训练中获得的刺激-刺激关联足以在大鼠最终被释放到池中时引导它们到平台。
{"title":"The role of local, distal, and global information in latent spatial learning.","authors":"Kerry E Gilroy,&nbsp;John M Pearce","doi":"10.1037/xan0000017","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000017","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In 4 experiments that investigated latent spatial learning, rats were repeatedly placed on a submerged platform in a corner of a square swimming pool with walls of different brightness. When they were subsequently released into the pool for a test trial in the absence of the platform, they spent the majority of time in the corner used for placement training-the correct corner. This effect was observed in Experiment 1, even when the test trial took place in a transformed version of the training arena. Experiments 2 and 3 indicated that the correct corner was identified by local cues based on the walls creating the corner. Experiment 4 demonstrated that distal cues created by the two walls that did not surround the platform during placement training could also be used to identify the correct corner. There was no evidence of learning about the relationship between global cues provided by the entire arena and the goal. The absence of the opportunity to develop instrumental, stimulus-response associations during placement training indicates that stimulus-stimulus associations acquired during this training were sufficient to guide rats to the platform when they were eventually released into the pool.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000017","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Contextual control of discriminated operant behavior. 歧视操作行为的语境控制。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000002
Mark E Bouton, Travis P Todd, Samuel P León

Previous research has suggested that changing the context after instrumental (operant) conditioning can weaken the strength of the operant response. That result contrasts with the results of studies of Pavlovian conditioning, in which a context switch often does not affect the response elicited by a conditioned stimulus. To begin to make the methods more similar, Experiments 1-3 tested the effects of a context switch in rats on a discriminated operant response (R; lever pressing or chain pulling) that had been reinforced only in the presence of a 30-s discriminative stimulus (S; tone or light). As in Pavlovian conditioning, responses and reinforcers became confined to presentations of the S during training. However, in Experiment 1, after training in Context A, a switch to Context B caused a decrement in responding during S. In Experiment 2, a switch to Context B likewise decreased responding in S when Context B was equally familiar, equally associated with reinforcement, or equally associated with the training of a discriminated operant (a different R reinforced in a different S). However, there was no decrement if Context B had been associated with the same response that was trained in Context A (Experiments 2 and 3). The effectiveness of S transferred across contexts, whereas the strength of the response did not. Experiment 4 found that a continuously reinforced response was also disrupted by context change when the same response manipulandum was used in both training and testing. Overall, the results suggest that the context can have a robust general role in the control of operant behavior. Mechanisms of contextual control are discussed.

先前的研究表明,在工具性条件反射(操作性条件反射)后改变情境可以削弱操作性反应的强度。这一结果与巴甫洛夫条件反射的研究结果形成了对比。巴甫洛夫条件反射认为,情境转换通常不会影响条件刺激引发的反应。为了使方法更加相似,实验1-3测试了大鼠的语境转换对区分操作反应的影响(R;杠杆按压或拉链),仅在30秒的判别刺激(S;音调或光线)。在巴甫洛夫条件反射中,反应和强化因素在训练过程中被限制在S的呈现中。然而,在实验1中,在情境A训练后,切换到情境B会导致S期间的反应下降。在实验2中,当情境B同样熟悉、同样与强化相关、或同样与歧视操作者的训练相关(不同的R在不同的S中得到强化)时,切换到情境B同样会降低S的反应。如果情境B与情境A中训练的相同反应相关联,则没有减少(实验2和3)。情境S的有效性跨情境转移,而反应的强度则没有。实验4发现,当在训练和测试中同时使用相同的反应操作时,连续强化的反应也会因情境变化而中断。总的来说,研究结果表明,环境在控制操作行为方面具有强大的普遍作用。讨论了上下文控制的机制。
{"title":"Contextual control of discriminated operant behavior.","authors":"Mark E Bouton,&nbsp;Travis P Todd,&nbsp;Samuel P León","doi":"10.1037/xan0000002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000002","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has suggested that changing the context after instrumental (operant) conditioning can weaken the strength of the operant response. That result contrasts with the results of studies of Pavlovian conditioning, in which a context switch often does not affect the response elicited by a conditioned stimulus. To begin to make the methods more similar, Experiments 1-3 tested the effects of a context switch in rats on a discriminated operant response (R; lever pressing or chain pulling) that had been reinforced only in the presence of a 30-s discriminative stimulus (S; tone or light). As in Pavlovian conditioning, responses and reinforcers became confined to presentations of the S during training. However, in Experiment 1, after training in Context A, a switch to Context B caused a decrement in responding during S. In Experiment 2, a switch to Context B likewise decreased responding in S when Context B was equally familiar, equally associated with reinforcement, or equally associated with the training of a discriminated operant (a different R reinforced in a different S). However, there was no decrement if Context B had been associated with the same response that was trained in Context A (Experiments 2 and 3). The effectiveness of S transferred across contexts, whereas the strength of the response did not. Experiment 4 found that a continuously reinforced response was also disrupted by context change when the same response manipulandum was used in both training and testing. Overall, the results suggest that the context can have a robust general role in the control of operant behavior. Mechanisms of contextual control are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"31703605","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 54
I like to get nothing: implicit and explicit evaluation of avoided negative outcomes. 我喜欢什么都得不到:对避免的负面结果的内隐和外显评估。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/xan0000005
Andreas B Eder, David Dignath

This study examined affective consequences of an active avoidance response. Catching a fleeting stimulus with a rapid key press secured a monetary reward in a reward condition or avoided a monetary loss in an avoidance condition. Outcomes of wins, missed wins, losses, and avoided losses were signaled with color patches that were evaluated explicitly (via evaluative rating) and implicitly (via an affective priming task). Liking scores in each condition were compared with those in yoked-control conditions in which wins and avoided losses were presented without the requirement of an active response. In the explicit measure, colors associated with an avoided loss were rated positively and colors associated with a missed win were judged negatively, irrespective of whether the outcome was self-generated. In the implicit measure, outcomes of missed wins and avoided losses were evaluated differently only when they were self-generated. The results confirm a qualitative affective equivalence between an avoided loss and an achieved win. Implications for avoidance theories are discussed.

本研究考察了主动回避反应的情感后果。在奖励条件下,通过快速按键捕捉转瞬即逝的刺激确保了金钱奖励;在回避条件下,避免了金钱损失。获胜、错过胜利、失败和避免失败的结果用色块表示,这些色块被明确地(通过评估评级)和隐性地(通过情感启动任务)评估。研究人员将每一种情况下的喜欢分数与不要求积极反应的“轭控”情况下的喜欢分数进行了比较。在明确的测量中,与避免失败相关的颜色被评价为积极的,与错过胜利相关的颜色被评价为消极的,无论结果是否是自我产生的。在隐性测量中,错过的胜利和避免的损失的结果只有在它们是自我产生时才会被不同地评估。结果证实了避免损失和取得胜利之间在质量上的情感等价。讨论了回避理论的含义。
{"title":"I like to get nothing: implicit and explicit evaluation of avoided negative outcomes.","authors":"Andreas B Eder,&nbsp;David Dignath","doi":"10.1037/xan0000005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xan0000005","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined affective consequences of an active avoidance response. Catching a fleeting stimulus with a rapid key press secured a monetary reward in a reward condition or avoided a monetary loss in an avoidance condition. Outcomes of wins, missed wins, losses, and avoided losses were signaled with color patches that were evaluated explicitly (via evaluative rating) and implicitly (via an affective priming task). Liking scores in each condition were compared with those in yoked-control conditions in which wins and avoided losses were presented without the requirement of an active response. In the explicit measure, colors associated with an avoided loss were rated positively and colors associated with a missed win were judged negatively, irrespective of whether the outcome was self-generated. In the implicit measure, outcomes of missed wins and avoided losses were evaluated differently only when they were self-generated. The results confirm a qualitative affective equivalence between an avoided loss and an achieved win. Implications for avoidance theories are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/xan0000005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"32392990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1