首页 > 最新文献

Ethology Ecology & Evolution最新文献

英文 中文
Interacting primates: the biological roots of human communication 互动灵长类动物:人类交流的生物学根源
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-04-13 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2059788
E. Demuru, C. Giacoma
The most striking aspect of human communication – and also the one that attracts most of the attention – is certainly language. Language is a complex, adaptive and multimodal communicative system requiring the convergence and integration of multiple mechan-isms (Fitch 2010; Levinson & Holler 2014). Several theories have been proposed to reconstruct the evolutionary pathways that led to this complex human trait, but a unifying framework is still lacking (Christiansen & Kirby 2003). In this volume, Meguerditchian (2022) contributes to the debate on the gestural origins of language by providing a review on baboons’ manual and body gestures that have been shown to share key properties of language. Among these, Meguerditchian (2022) also focuses on later-alisation and hemispheric specialisation in the baboon brain. Concerning the neural basis of primate communication, Sliwa et al. (2022) present evidence that primates possess specialised neural pathways coordinating a diverse range of communication systems for organising their complex kin and friendship bonds.
人类交流中最引人注目的方面——也是最吸引人注意的方面——当然是语言。语言是一个复杂的、自适应的、多模态的交际系统,需要多种机制的汇聚和融合(Fitch 2010;Levinson & Holler 2014)。已经提出了几种理论来重建导致这种复杂人类特征的进化途径,但仍然缺乏统一的框架(Christiansen & Kirby 2003)。在本卷中,Meguerditchian(2022)通过对狒狒的手势和身体手势的回顾,为语言的手势起源的辩论做出了贡献,这些手势和身体手势已被证明具有语言的关键特性。其中,Meguerditchian(2022)也关注狒狒大脑的后发化和半球特化。关于灵长类动物交流的神经基础,Sliwa等人(2022)提供的证据表明,灵长类动物拥有专门的神经通路,协调各种各样的交流系统,以组织其复杂的亲属和友谊关系。
{"title":"Interacting primates: the biological roots of human communication","authors":"E. Demuru, C. Giacoma","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2059788","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2059788","url":null,"abstract":"The most striking aspect of human communication – and also the one that attracts most of the attention – is certainly language. Language is a complex, adaptive and multimodal communicative system requiring the convergence and integration of multiple mechan-isms (Fitch 2010; Levinson & Holler 2014). Several theories have been proposed to reconstruct the evolutionary pathways that led to this complex human trait, but a unifying framework is still lacking (Christiansen & Kirby 2003). In this volume, Meguerditchian (2022) contributes to the debate on the gestural origins of language by providing a review on baboons’ manual and body gestures that have been shown to share key properties of language. Among these, Meguerditchian (2022) also focuses on later-alisation and hemispheric specialisation in the baboon brain. Concerning the neural basis of primate communication, Sliwa et al. (2022) present evidence that primates possess specialised neural pathways coordinating a diverse range of communication systems for organising their complex kin and friendship bonds.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75183092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of epigynal setae in mate choice in the orb-weaver Leucauge argyra 球形编织银蛛附生刚毛在择偶中的作用
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-04-13 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2059786
Linda Hernández Duran, G. Barrantes
Male and female genital morphology may have strong effects on fitness in spiders. Females of some species of Leucauge produce mating plugs that partially or totally block the entrances to their insemination ducts on the epigynum, forming barriers that reduce the probabilities of future inseminations. While in some species both females and males participate in plug formation, the mating plugs in L. argyra are produced exclusively by females. The epigynum of L. argyra (Walckenaer 1841) is unusual in having a prominent ventral process covered on its anterior side and around its base with long, thick setae. We tested whether male stimulation of these epigynal setae affected female plug formation by removing them from some females but not others. Females lacking setae produced mating plugs immediately after copulation less often than unmanipulated females. However, 24 hr later the mating plugs had disappeared in many females of the treatment group. Setae removal did not affect the amount of sperm stored in the female spermatheca, or the frequency of female attacks. The rate of male genital inflations correlated positively, but mating duration was negatively correlated with sperm stored in female’s spermathecae. Females attempted to attack small males more often than large males. These findings contribute to our understanding of the function of the setae associated with the female genitalia, and the effect of their tactile stimulation on plug formation, and transference of spermatozoids. HIGHLIGHTS • Removal of female epigynal setae affects the frequency of female plug formation • Male size influences female attack decisions during mating
雄性和雌性生殖器官的形态可能对蜘蛛的适应性有很强的影响。某些种类的长尾雀的雌性会产生交配塞,部分或完全阻塞其上阴部的授精管道入口,形成屏障,降低未来授精的可能性。虽然在一些物种中雌性和雄性都参与塞的形成,但银腹蛛的交配塞完全由雌性产生。L. argyra的上唇(Walckenaer 1841)的不寻常之处是有一个突出的腹突覆盖在其前部和基部周围,有长而厚的刚毛。我们测试了雄性对这些附睾刚毛的刺激是否会影响雌性的栓形成,方法是将它们从一些雌性身上移除,而不是从其他雌性身上移除。缺少刚毛的雌虫在交配后立即产生交配塞的次数少于未被操纵的雌虫。然而,24小时后,治疗组的许多雌性交配塞消失了。去除刚毛不会影响储存在雌性精子囊中的精子数量,也不会影响雌性攻击的频率。雄性生殖器官膨胀率与雌性精子囊中精子存储量呈正相关,而交配持续时间与精子存储量呈负相关。雌性攻击小型雄性的次数多于大型雄性。这些发现有助于我们了解与女性生殖器相关的刚毛的功能,以及它们的触觉刺激对塞形成和精子转移的影响。•去除雌性附毛会影响雌性塞形成的频率•雄性的大小影响雌性在交配期间的攻击决定
{"title":"The role of epigynal setae in mate choice in the orb-weaver Leucauge argyra","authors":"Linda Hernández Duran, G. Barrantes","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2059786","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2059786","url":null,"abstract":"Male and female genital morphology may have strong effects on fitness in spiders. Females of some species of Leucauge produce mating plugs that partially or totally block the entrances to their insemination ducts on the epigynum, forming barriers that reduce the probabilities of future inseminations. While in some species both females and males participate in plug formation, the mating plugs in L. argyra are produced exclusively by females. The epigynum of L. argyra (Walckenaer 1841) is unusual in having a prominent ventral process covered on its anterior side and around its base with long, thick setae. We tested whether male stimulation of these epigynal setae affected female plug formation by removing them from some females but not others. Females lacking setae produced mating plugs immediately after copulation less often than unmanipulated females. However, 24 hr later the mating plugs had disappeared in many females of the treatment group. Setae removal did not affect the amount of sperm stored in the female spermatheca, or the frequency of female attacks. The rate of male genital inflations correlated positively, but mating duration was negatively correlated with sperm stored in female’s spermathecae. Females attempted to attack small males more often than large males. These findings contribute to our understanding of the function of the setae associated with the female genitalia, and the effect of their tactile stimulation on plug formation, and transference of spermatozoids. HIGHLIGHTS • Removal of female epigynal setae affects the frequency of female plug formation • Male size influences female attack decisions during mating","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81875468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sublethal levels of organophosphate insecticides alter behaviour in the juveniles of the Neotropical crab, Poppiana dentata (Randall 1840) 亚致死水平的有机磷杀虫剂改变了新热带蟹幼蟹的行为(Randall 1840)
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-04-03 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2044389
D. Singh, L. Rostant, A. Mohammed, Alyssa S. Jairam, Jyotsna J. Sahatoo, Raquel Khan Ali, F. Mohammed
HIGHLIGHTS This is the first description of OP-induced behavioural alterations for juvenile P. dentata Aggression and inactivity were induced by the OPs The insecticides reduced feeding (e.g. eating) in exposed crabs Impacts were also associated with vulnerability and hampered survivorship Behavioural alterations induced by xenobiotics, like pesticides, have not been assessed for many Neotropical crab species. This study sought to evaluate such effects on juvenile Poppiana dentata, exposed to two organophosphate (OP) pesticides under laboratory conditions. Treatments involved a control, solvent control (xylene), malathion and diazinon commercial insecticides, with the OPs administered in three logarithmically increasing concentrations of 0.1, 1 and 10 µg/L. A pre-established ethogram was used to document behavioural acts for treatments; foraging, eating, grooming, locomotion, quiescence (inactive), prolonged agonism, non-agonistic contact and moulting. Each treatment cohort was continuously recorded over a 72 hr period. The time durations for each act were determined from the video footage (n = 2,304 hr) and used to determine time spent on each behavioural act, over a 24 hr time period. Time spent by OP-exposed crabs on each act differed significantly from those of the controls. Diazinon-exposed crabs spent most of their daily time in heightened agonistic behaviour (43.4–71.6%). Malathion-exposed crabs spent the majority of their time being inactive (46.7–79.0%) and xylene-exposed crabs exhibited similar behavioural alterations of heightened aggression (53.6%) and inactivity (39.3%). OP-induced changes in essential behaviours of juvenile P. dentata can increase vulnerability, hamper survival and increase risk to predators via impaired locomotion, induced inactivity and limited energy availability, through reduced food intake. These findings provide a better understanding of OP-induced behavioural alterations in juvenile P. dentata and the long-term consequences for essential processes like growth, reproduction and survival.
这是第一次描述由op引起的幼年齿状螯蟹的行为改变。杀虫剂减少了暴露的螃蟹的摄食(如进食),影响也与脆弱性和生存障碍有关,许多新热带螃蟹物种尚未评估由外源物(如杀虫剂)引起的行为改变。本研究旨在评价在实验室条件下暴露于两种有机磷(OP)农药对齿状Poppiana幼鱼的影响。处理包括对照、溶剂对照(二甲苯)、马拉硫磷和二嗪农商用杀虫剂,施用的有机磷浓度分别为0.1、1和10µg/L,呈对数递增。预先建立的心电图用于记录治疗的行为行为;觅食、进食、梳理、运动、静止(不活动)、长时间的激动作用、非激动作用接触和蜕皮。每个治疗组在72小时内连续记录。每个行为的持续时间是从视频片段(n = 2304小时)中确定的,并用于确定在24小时内每个行为行为所花费的时间。暴露于op的螃蟹在每次行动中花费的时间与对照组有显著差异。暴露于二氮肼的蟹在大部分时间内均表现出强烈的激动行为(43.4-71.6%)。暴露于马拉硫磷的螃蟹大部分时间处于不活动状态(46.7-79.0%),暴露于二甲苯的螃蟹表现出类似的行为改变,攻击性增强(53.6%)和不活动(39.3%)。由op引起的幼体齿形矮齿羚基本行为的改变会增加其脆弱性,阻碍其生存,并通过减少食物摄入而导致运动受损、诱导不活动和能量供应有限,从而增加对捕食者的风险。这些发现提供了一个更好的理解op诱导的幼年齿形牙鼠行为改变及其对生长、繁殖和生存等基本过程的长期影响。
{"title":"Sublethal levels of organophosphate insecticides alter behaviour in the juveniles of the Neotropical crab, Poppiana dentata (Randall 1840)","authors":"D. Singh, L. Rostant, A. Mohammed, Alyssa S. Jairam, Jyotsna J. Sahatoo, Raquel Khan Ali, F. Mohammed","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2044389","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2044389","url":null,"abstract":"HIGHLIGHTS This is the first description of OP-induced behavioural alterations for juvenile P. dentata Aggression and inactivity were induced by the OPs The insecticides reduced feeding (e.g. eating) in exposed crabs Impacts were also associated with vulnerability and hampered survivorship Behavioural alterations induced by xenobiotics, like pesticides, have not been assessed for many Neotropical crab species. This study sought to evaluate such effects on juvenile Poppiana dentata, exposed to two organophosphate (OP) pesticides under laboratory conditions. Treatments involved a control, solvent control (xylene), malathion and diazinon commercial insecticides, with the OPs administered in three logarithmically increasing concentrations of 0.1, 1 and 10 µg/L. A pre-established ethogram was used to document behavioural acts for treatments; foraging, eating, grooming, locomotion, quiescence (inactive), prolonged agonism, non-agonistic contact and moulting. Each treatment cohort was continuously recorded over a 72 hr period. The time durations for each act were determined from the video footage (n = 2,304 hr) and used to determine time spent on each behavioural act, over a 24 hr time period. Time spent by OP-exposed crabs on each act differed significantly from those of the controls. Diazinon-exposed crabs spent most of their daily time in heightened agonistic behaviour (43.4–71.6%). Malathion-exposed crabs spent the majority of their time being inactive (46.7–79.0%) and xylene-exposed crabs exhibited similar behavioural alterations of heightened aggression (53.6%) and inactivity (39.3%). OP-induced changes in essential behaviours of juvenile P. dentata can increase vulnerability, hamper survival and increase risk to predators via impaired locomotion, induced inactivity and limited energy availability, through reduced food intake. These findings provide a better understanding of OP-induced behavioural alterations in juvenile P. dentata and the long-term consequences for essential processes like growth, reproduction and survival.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90039048","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the gestural origins of language: what baboons’ gestures and brain have told us after 15 years of research 语言的手势起源:经过15年的研究,狒狒的手势和大脑告诉了我们什么
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-31 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2044388
A. Meguerditchian
Nonhuman primates mostly communicate not only with a rich vocal repertoire but also with manual and body gestures. In contrast to great apes, this latter communicative gestural system has been poorly investigated in monkeys. In the last 15 years, the gestural research we conducted in the baboons Papio anubis, an Old World monkey species, have shown potential direct evolutionary continuities with some key properties of language such as intentionality, referentiality, learning flexibility as well as its underlying lateralisation and hemispheric specialisation of the brain. According to these collective findings, which are congruent with the ones reported in great apes, it is thus not excluded that features of gestural communication shared between humans, great apes and baboons, may have played a critical role in the phylogenetic roots of language and dated back, not to the Hominidae evolution, but rather to their much older catarrhine common ancestor 25–40 million years ago.
非人灵长类动物大多不仅用丰富的声音交流,还用手势和肢体语言交流。与类人猿相比,后者的交流手势系统在猴子身上的研究很少。在过去的15年里,我们对狒狒(一种旧大陆的猴子物种)进行的手势研究表明,语言的一些关键特性,如意向性、指称性、学习灵活性以及潜在的偏侧化和大脑半球专门化,具有潜在的直接进化连续性。根据这些与类人猿一致的集体发现,因此不能排除人类、类人猿和狒狒之间共有的手势交流特征,可能在语言的系统发育根源中发挥了关键作用,并且可以追溯到2500万到4000万年前,而不是人科进化,而是他们更古老的卡塔林共同祖先。
{"title":"On the gestural origins of language: what baboons’ gestures and brain have told us after 15 years of research","authors":"A. Meguerditchian","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2044388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2044388","url":null,"abstract":"Nonhuman primates mostly communicate not only with a rich vocal repertoire but also with manual and body gestures. In contrast to great apes, this latter communicative gestural system has been poorly investigated in monkeys. In the last 15 years, the gestural research we conducted in the baboons Papio anubis, an Old World monkey species, have shown potential direct evolutionary continuities with some key properties of language such as intentionality, referentiality, learning flexibility as well as its underlying lateralisation and hemispheric specialisation of the brain. According to these collective findings, which are congruent with the ones reported in great apes, it is thus not excluded that features of gestural communication shared between humans, great apes and baboons, may have played a critical role in the phylogenetic roots of language and dated back, not to the Hominidae evolution, but rather to their much older catarrhine common ancestor 25–40 million years ago.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90171340","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
A pilot study of calling patterns and vocal turn-taking in wild bonobos Pan paniscus 野生倭黑猩猩呼叫模式和声音转换的初步研究
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-28 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2044387
C. Cornec, Muzungu Ngofuna, A. Lemasson, Claude Monghiemo, Victor Narat, F. Levréro
Highlights This pilot study shows that wild bonobos display the fundamental temporal rules of vocal turn-taking Occurrences of calling patterns are in line with the unique observation collected from a captive group Calling patterns do not differ significantly with age and sex Calling patterns appear context-dependent In several species of non-human primates, non-agonistic vocal exchanges can be seen as a primitive form of conversation, as they respect basic temporal rules (i.e. turn-taking, overlap avoidance), the same as those that guide human conversations. Conversational rules have recently been suggested in captive great ape species, yet the only study investigating vocal turn-taking in wild great apes did not find any evidence of such vocal roles. Whether the environmental conditions (captivity versus free ranging) or the social organisation of a given species shape temporally ruled vocal exchanges remain open questions. Here, we investigated general calling patterns of peaceful vocal exchanges in a wild bonobo community. This pilot study revealed that wild bonobos respect the fundamental temporal rules of vocal turn-taking, namely the avoidance of overlapping and the presence of short call-intervals between interlocutors on the order of 2 sec, corroborating findings from captive bonobos. Despite the limited sample size, our finding suggests that vocal exchanges appear context-dependent but neither age nor sex seem to influence their occurrence. While further studies are needed to confirm these observations, this study helps to fill a major gap in research on the vocal communication of wild great apes, paving the way for more extensive comparative studies, representing a further step towards a better understanding of how vocal turn-taking arose in humans.
这项初步研究表明,野生倭黑猩猩表现出声音轮流的基本时间规则,呼叫模式的出现与从圈养群体中收集到的独特观察结果一致,呼叫模式与年龄和性别没有显著差异,呼叫模式似乎依赖于环境。在几种非人类灵长类动物中,非竞争的声音交换可以被视为一种原始的对话形式,因为它们尊重基本的时间规则(即轮流,避免重叠),与指导人类对话的原则相同。最近在圈养的类人猿物种中提出了对话规则,然而唯一一项调查野生类人猿声音轮流的研究并没有发现任何证据表明存在这种声音角色。环境条件(圈养还是自由放养)或特定物种的社会组织是否会暂时决定声音交流,这仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。在这里,我们研究了野生倭黑猩猩群体中和平声音交流的一般呼叫模式。这项初步研究表明,野生倭黑猩猩尊重声音轮流的基本时间规则,即避免重叠,对话者之间的呼叫间隔较短,约为2秒,证实了圈养倭黑猩猩的发现。尽管样本量有限,但我们的发现表明,声音交流似乎与环境有关,但年龄和性别似乎都不会影响它们的发生。虽然需要进一步的研究来证实这些观察结果,但这项研究有助于填补野生类人猿声音交流研究的一个主要空白,为更广泛的比较研究铺平了道路,代表着更好地理解人类声音轮流产生的进一步步骤。
{"title":"A pilot study of calling patterns and vocal turn-taking in wild bonobos Pan paniscus","authors":"C. Cornec, Muzungu Ngofuna, A. Lemasson, Claude Monghiemo, Victor Narat, F. Levréro","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2044387","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2044387","url":null,"abstract":"Highlights This pilot study shows that wild bonobos display the fundamental temporal rules of vocal turn-taking Occurrences of calling patterns are in line with the unique observation collected from a captive group Calling patterns do not differ significantly with age and sex Calling patterns appear context-dependent In several species of non-human primates, non-agonistic vocal exchanges can be seen as a primitive form of conversation, as they respect basic temporal rules (i.e. turn-taking, overlap avoidance), the same as those that guide human conversations. Conversational rules have recently been suggested in captive great ape species, yet the only study investigating vocal turn-taking in wild great apes did not find any evidence of such vocal roles. Whether the environmental conditions (captivity versus free ranging) or the social organisation of a given species shape temporally ruled vocal exchanges remain open questions. Here, we investigated general calling patterns of peaceful vocal exchanges in a wild bonobo community. This pilot study revealed that wild bonobos respect the fundamental temporal rules of vocal turn-taking, namely the avoidance of overlapping and the presence of short call-intervals between interlocutors on the order of 2 sec, corroborating findings from captive bonobos. Despite the limited sample size, our finding suggests that vocal exchanges appear context-dependent but neither age nor sex seem to influence their occurrence. While further studies are needed to confirm these observations, this study helps to fill a major gap in research on the vocal communication of wild great apes, paving the way for more extensive comparative studies, representing a further step towards a better understanding of how vocal turn-taking arose in humans.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88057757","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Static allometry of a threat device that is not a weapon: wing spots in male Heterandrium fallax (Hymenoptera Agaonidae) 一种非武器的威胁装置的静态异速测量:雄性黄腹大蜂(膜翅目蜂科)的翅膀斑点
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-22 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2021.2024269
W. Eberhard, R. L. Rodríguez
Males of the agaonid wasp Heterandrium fallax have wing spots that they display during aggressive encounters near the ostioles of syconia of the fig Ficus pertusa, apparently in competition for the opportunity to copulate with emerging females. In accord with predictions of the “functional allometry hypothesis” for the allometry of structures that are under sexual selection as threat devices and that do not function as weapons, larger males tended to have proportionally larger wing spots. Some smaller males lacked spots, and smaller males were less likely to be collected near female emergence sites or to show antennal damage that probably results from male fights.
雄蜂在无花果无花果(Ficus pertusa)的syconia孔附近遭遇攻击时,雄蜂的翅膀上有斑点,显然是在争夺与新出现的雌蜂交配的机会。与“功能异速假说”的预测一致,“功能异速假说”是指在性选择下作为威胁手段而不是作为武器的结构异速,体型较大的雄性往往有比例较大的翅膀斑点。一些体型较小的雄性没有斑点,体型较小的雄性不太可能在雌性出现的地方附近被收集,也不太可能显示出可能由雄性打架造成的触角损伤。
{"title":"Static allometry of a threat device that is not a weapon: wing spots in male Heterandrium fallax (Hymenoptera Agaonidae)","authors":"W. Eberhard, R. L. Rodríguez","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2021.2024269","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2021.2024269","url":null,"abstract":"Males of the agaonid wasp Heterandrium fallax have wing spots that they display during aggressive encounters near the ostioles of syconia of the fig Ficus pertusa, apparently in competition for the opportunity to copulate with emerging females. In accord with predictions of the “functional allometry hypothesis” for the allometry of structures that are under sexual selection as threat devices and that do not function as weapons, larger males tended to have proportionally larger wing spots. Some smaller males lacked spots, and smaller males were less likely to be collected near female emergence sites or to show antennal damage that probably results from male fights.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89439692","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Escape angles for solitary animals and groups of the lizard Pholidoscelis fuscatus 独居动物和群蜥蜴的逃生角
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-15 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2044386
D. Eifler, M. Eifler, Grace E. Garrison, Victoria L. Grotbeck
Predation can be an important factor shaping the dynamics of animal groups. The lizard Pholidoscelis fuscatus forms small groups while foraging, providing the opportunity to examine how predation risk influences group dynamics. We studied the escape angles of P. fuscatus during simulated predator approaches, focusing on whether grouped lizards behaved differently from solitary animals and the extent to which group cohesion was maintained during escape. Both solitary and grouped lizards exhibited non-random escape angles, oriented away from the approaching predator. However, lizards in groups exhibited escape angles closer to 0° (i.e., more directly away from the predator) than solitary lizards. Groups maintained their cohesiveness during flight. Grouping by P. fuscatus could allow more effective escape behaviour to occur.
捕食是影响动物群体动态的一个重要因素。蜥蜴Pholidoscelis fuscatus在觅食时形成小群体,为研究捕食风险如何影响群体动态提供了机会。我们研究了P. fuscatus在模拟捕食者接近时的逃跑角度,重点研究了成群蜥蜴的行为是否与独居动物不同,以及在逃跑过程中群体凝聚力的维持程度。独居蜥蜴和群居蜥蜴都表现出非随机的逃跑角度,远离接近的捕食者。然而,成群的蜥蜴比单独的蜥蜴表现出更接近0°的逃跑角度(即更直接地远离捕食者)。群体在飞行过程中保持了凝聚力。fuscatus的分组可以使更有效的逃脱行为发生。
{"title":"Escape angles for solitary animals and groups of the lizard Pholidoscelis fuscatus","authors":"D. Eifler, M. Eifler, Grace E. Garrison, Victoria L. Grotbeck","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2044386","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2044386","url":null,"abstract":"Predation can be an important factor shaping the dynamics of animal groups. The lizard Pholidoscelis fuscatus forms small groups while foraging, providing the opportunity to examine how predation risk influences group dynamics. We studied the escape angles of P. fuscatus during simulated predator approaches, focusing on whether grouped lizards behaved differently from solitary animals and the extent to which group cohesion was maintained during escape. Both solitary and grouped lizards exhibited non-random escape angles, oriented away from the approaching predator. However, lizards in groups exhibited escape angles closer to 0° (i.e., more directly away from the predator) than solitary lizards. Groups maintained their cohesiveness during flight. Grouping by P. fuscatus could allow more effective escape behaviour to occur.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86497600","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What makes us apes? The emotional building blocks of intersubjectivity in hominids 是什么让我们成为类人猿?原始人主体间性的情感基石
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-09 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2022.2044390
E. Demuru, Z. Clay, I. Norscia
Highlights Intersubjectivity has often been lauded as one of the defining features that separates humans and other extant hominids. Intersubjectivity and empathy are different, yet related, phenomena. The study of emotions and empathy-related abilities can provide insights into the ontogeny and evolution of intersubjectivity. Intersubjectivity, which refers to the capacity to create shared value or connection between individuals, is a complex and multi-faceted phenomenon involving both cognitive and affective components. Intersubjectivity has often been lauded as one of the defining features that separates the social minds and existence of humans and non-human animals. Despite the apparently profound importance of inter-subjectivity for the socio-cognitive functioning of our species, we know surprisingly little about its evolution, nor how its evolution relates to the evolution of other related phenomena, such as empathy. In this review, we embrace the “bottom-up” perspective to consider recent theoretical and empirical advances in the fields of non-human animal cognition and emotion and what they can tell us about how complex socio-emotional capacities evolve. In particular, we focus on great ape species. Given their close phylogenetic relationship to us, great apes (the non-human, extant hominids) offer a unique lens to identify which of our capacities may be evolutionarily derived or phylogenetically shared.
主体间性经常被称赞为区分人类和其他现存原始人的决定性特征之一。主体间性和共情是不同但又相关的现象。情绪和共情相关能力的研究可以为主体间性的个体发生和演化提供见解。主体间性是指个体之间创造共同价值或联系的能力,是一种复杂的、多方面的现象,涉及认知和情感两方面。主体间性经常被称赞为区分人类和非人类动物的社会思想和存在的决定性特征之一。尽管主体间性对人类的社会认知功能有着深远的重要性,但我们对它的进化知之甚少,也不知道它的进化与其他相关现象(如移情)的进化之间的关系。在这篇综述中,我们采用“自下而上”的观点来考虑最近在非人类动物认知和情感领域的理论和实证进展,以及它们可以告诉我们复杂的社会情感能力是如何进化的。我们特别关注类人猿。考虑到类人猿(现存的非人类原始人)与我们在系统发育上的密切关系,它们提供了一个独特的视角来识别我们的哪些能力可能是进化衍生的,或者是在系统发育上共享的。
{"title":"What makes us apes? The emotional building blocks of intersubjectivity in hominids","authors":"E. Demuru, Z. Clay, I. Norscia","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2022.2044390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2022.2044390","url":null,"abstract":"Highlights Intersubjectivity has often been lauded as one of the defining features that separates humans and other extant hominids. Intersubjectivity and empathy are different, yet related, phenomena. The study of emotions and empathy-related abilities can provide insights into the ontogeny and evolution of intersubjectivity. Intersubjectivity, which refers to the capacity to create shared value or connection between individuals, is a complex and multi-faceted phenomenon involving both cognitive and affective components. Intersubjectivity has often been lauded as one of the defining features that separates the social minds and existence of humans and non-human animals. Despite the apparently profound importance of inter-subjectivity for the socio-cognitive functioning of our species, we know surprisingly little about its evolution, nor how its evolution relates to the evolution of other related phenomena, such as empathy. In this review, we embrace the “bottom-up” perspective to consider recent theoretical and empirical advances in the fields of non-human animal cognition and emotion and what they can tell us about how complex socio-emotional capacities evolve. In particular, we focus on great ape species. Given their close phylogenetic relationship to us, great apes (the non-human, extant hominids) offer a unique lens to identify which of our capacities may be evolutionarily derived or phylogenetically shared.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82032551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Urban junco flight initiation distances correlate with approach velocities of anthropogenic sounds 城市junco飞行起始距离与人为声音的接近速度相关
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-02-27 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2021.2024263
Kara Lukas, Hayley M. Stansell, P. Yeh, P. Nonacs
Urban-dwelling birds face novel visual cues and soundscapes. To thrive in these challenging environments, individuals must correctly identify and calibrate threats posed by humans and their activities. We showed that Dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) residing in an urban habitat responded differently to the sounds that approaching people and objects make. A person approached juncos simultaneously playing the sounds of object types that normally move at different relative velocities: faster (bicycles), intermediate (skateboards and scooters), or slower (people walking). Juncos responded at significantly greater distances and moved further in relation to what sound cues would normally imply about the velocity of approach. Absolute stimulus volume was not a significant predictor of response across object type. The responses occurred without the presence of visual cues, suggesting that an auditory cue alone and without visual confirmation can produce an appropriate response. Overall, this shows that this population of urban juncos has the ability to respond appropriately to novel anthropogenic sound cues. The question remains as to how universal such abilities are across species, different urban situations, and in natural habitats.
城市鸟类面临着新的视觉线索和声景。为了在这些充满挑战的环境中茁壮成长,个人必须正确识别和校准人类及其活动构成的威胁。我们发现,生活在城市栖息地的黑眼juncos (Junco hyemalis)对接近的人和物体发出的声音有不同的反应。一个人在接近juncos的同时播放不同物体类型的声音,这些物体通常以不同的相对速度移动:较快的(自行车)、中等的(滑板和踏板车)或较慢的(人走路)。相对于声音线索通常暗示的接近速度,Juncos在更远的距离上做出反应,移动得更远。绝对刺激量对不同对象类型的反应没有显著的预测作用。这些反应是在没有视觉提示的情况下发生的,这表明单独的听觉提示和没有视觉确认可以产生适当的反应。总的来说,这表明城市juncos群体有能力对新的人为声音线索做出适当的反应。问题仍然是,这种能力在不同的物种、不同的城市环境和自然栖息地中有多普遍。
{"title":"Urban junco flight initiation distances correlate with approach velocities of anthropogenic sounds","authors":"Kara Lukas, Hayley M. Stansell, P. Yeh, P. Nonacs","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2021.2024263","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2021.2024263","url":null,"abstract":"Urban-dwelling birds face novel visual cues and soundscapes. To thrive in these challenging environments, individuals must correctly identify and calibrate threats posed by humans and their activities. We showed that Dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) residing in an urban habitat responded differently to the sounds that approaching people and objects make. A person approached juncos simultaneously playing the sounds of object types that normally move at different relative velocities: faster (bicycles), intermediate (skateboards and scooters), or slower (people walking). Juncos responded at significantly greater distances and moved further in relation to what sound cues would normally imply about the velocity of approach. Absolute stimulus volume was not a significant predictor of response across object type. The responses occurred without the presence of visual cues, suggesting that an auditory cue alone and without visual confirmation can produce an appropriate response. Overall, this shows that this population of urban juncos has the ability to respond appropriately to novel anthropogenic sound cues. The question remains as to how universal such abilities are across species, different urban situations, and in natural habitats.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77721124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Web building behavior in a wall spider (Oecobiidae) suggests a close relationship with orb-weavers 墙蛛(壁蛛科)的造网行为表明它与圆蛛有密切的关系
IF 1.2 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-02-09 DOI: 10.1080/03949370.2021.2024264
Diego Solano-Brenes, D. G. Muniz, L. Sandoval, Olman Alvarado-Rodríguez, G. Barrantes
Behavior is often phylogenetically informative and detailed descriptions of behavior have been used to support taxonomic relationships in several groups, such as birds, lizards, and arthropods. Web building behavior has provided informative traits to several spider families, but observations are lacking for other families, such as wall spiders Oecobiidae. Recent phylogenetic hypotheses based on molecular traits placed oecobiids either close to cribellate orb weavers (Uloboridae) or to long-spinneret spiders (Hersiliidae). Here, we describe for first time the web construction behavior and details of the web design in the oecobiid Oecobius concinnus Simon 1893 (Oecobiidae). We compare them with uloborid and araneoid orb weavers. If Oecobiidae and Uloboridae are closely related, we expect that O. concinnus share some web construction behaviors with these orb weavers. Video recordings and analyses of web structures suggest the sequence of web construction (radii, then sticky spiral construction) and the arrangement of cribellate threads are possible homologies between Oecobiidae and orb weavers, supporting a close relationship of Oecobiidae with Uloboridae.
行为通常是系统发育方面的信息,行为的详细描述已被用来支持一些类群的分类关系,如鸟类、蜥蜴和节肢动物。造网行为为一些蜘蛛家族提供了信息特征,但对其他家族(如壁虎蛛)缺乏观察。最近基于分子特征的系统发育假说认为,生态蜘蛛要么接近于蹼状球形织蛛(蹼状织蛛科),要么接近于长纺蛛(蛛蛛科)。本文首次描述了壁蜂Oecobius concinus Simon 1893(壁蜂科)的造网行为和设计细节。我们将它们与球状和蛛形织球者进行比较。如果Oecobiidae和Uloboridae有密切的关系,我们预计oecobiidus与这些织球动物有一些共同的织网行为。视频记录和对网结构的分析表明,网的构造顺序(半径,然后是粘螺旋结构)和网纹线的排列可能是圆织虫科和圆织虫科的同源性,支持圆织虫科和圆织虫科的密切关系。
{"title":"Web building behavior in a wall spider (Oecobiidae) suggests a close relationship with orb-weavers","authors":"Diego Solano-Brenes, D. G. Muniz, L. Sandoval, Olman Alvarado-Rodríguez, G. Barrantes","doi":"10.1080/03949370.2021.2024264","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2021.2024264","url":null,"abstract":"Behavior is often phylogenetically informative and detailed descriptions of behavior have been used to support taxonomic relationships in several groups, such as birds, lizards, and arthropods. Web building behavior has provided informative traits to several spider families, but observations are lacking for other families, such as wall spiders Oecobiidae. Recent phylogenetic hypotheses based on molecular traits placed oecobiids either close to cribellate orb weavers (Uloboridae) or to long-spinneret spiders (Hersiliidae). Here, we describe for first time the web construction behavior and details of the web design in the oecobiid Oecobius concinnus Simon 1893 (Oecobiidae). We compare them with uloborid and araneoid orb weavers. If Oecobiidae and Uloboridae are closely related, we expect that O. concinnus share some web construction behaviors with these orb weavers. Video recordings and analyses of web structures suggest the sequence of web construction (radii, then sticky spiral construction) and the arrangement of cribellate threads are possible homologies between Oecobiidae and orb weavers, supporting a close relationship of Oecobiidae with Uloboridae.","PeriodicalId":55163,"journal":{"name":"Ethology Ecology & Evolution","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-02-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79639130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Ethology Ecology & Evolution
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1