首页 > 最新文献

Cerebral cortex communications最新文献

英文 中文
Mechanism Exploration of 3-Hinge Gyral Formation and Pattern Recognition. 三铰旋回形成与模式识别机理探讨。
Pub Date : 2021-07-03 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab044
Mir Jalil Razavi, Tianming Liu, Xianqiao Wang

The 3-hinge gyral folding is the conjunction of gyrus crest lines from three different orientations. Previous studies have not explored the possible mechanisms of formation of such 3-hinge gyri, which are preserved across species in primate brains. We develop a biomechanical model to mimic the formation of 3-hinge patterns on a real brain and determine how special types of 3-hinge patterns form in certain areas of the model. Our computational and experimental imaging results show that most tertiary convolutions and exact locations of 3-hinge patterns after growth and folding are unpredictable, but they help explain the consistency of locations and patterns of certain 3-hinge patterns. Growing fibers within the white matter is posited as a determining factor to affect the location and shape of these 3-hinge patterns. Even if the growing fibers do not exert strong enough forces to guide gyrification directly, they still may seed a heterogeneous growth profile that leads to the formation of 3-hinge patterns in specific locations. A minor difference in initial morphology between two growing model brains can lead to distinct numbers and locations of 3-hinge patterns after folding.

三铰旋回折叠是三种不同方向的脑回嵴线的连接。先前的研究尚未探索这种3铰脑回形成的可能机制,这种机制在灵长类动物大脑中跨物种保存。我们开发了一个生物力学模型来模拟真实大脑上3-hinge模式的形成,并确定特殊类型的3-hinge模式如何在模型的某些区域形成。我们的计算和实验成像结果表明,生长和折叠后3-hinge模式的大多数三级卷积和确切位置是不可预测的,但它们有助于解释某些3-hinge模式的位置和模式的一致性。在白质中生长的纤维被认为是影响这些3铰链模式的位置和形状的决定性因素。即使生长的纤维没有施加足够强的力来直接引导旋转,它们仍然可能形成一种不均匀的生长曲线,导致在特定位置形成3铰模式。两个生长模型大脑在初始形态上的微小差异可以导致折叠后3-hinge模式的不同数量和位置。
{"title":"Mechanism Exploration of 3-Hinge Gyral Formation and Pattern Recognition.","authors":"Mir Jalil Razavi,&nbsp;Tianming Liu,&nbsp;Xianqiao Wang","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab044","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab044","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The 3-hinge gyral folding is the conjunction of gyrus crest lines from three different orientations. Previous studies have not explored the possible mechanisms of formation of such 3-hinge gyri, which are preserved across species in primate brains. We develop a biomechanical model to mimic the formation of 3-hinge patterns on a real brain and determine how special types of 3-hinge patterns form in certain areas of the model. Our computational and experimental imaging results show that most tertiary convolutions and exact locations of 3-hinge patterns after growth and folding are unpredictable, but they help explain the consistency of locations and patterns of certain 3-hinge patterns. Growing fibers within the white matter is posited as a determining factor to affect the location and shape of these 3-hinge patterns. Even if the growing fibers do not exert strong enough forces to guide gyrification directly, they still may seed a heterogeneous growth profile that leads to the formation of 3-hinge patterns in specific locations. A minor difference in initial morphology between two growing model brains can lead to distinct numbers and locations of 3-hinge patterns after folding.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8343593/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39300032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Neural Dynamics of Inhibitory Control in Musicians with Absolute Pitch: Theta Synchrony as an Oscillatory Signature of Information Conflict. 绝对音高音乐家抑制性控制的神经动力学:Theta同步作为信息冲突的振荡特征。
Pub Date : 2021-07-03 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab043
Vivek V Sharma, Michael Thaut, Frank A Russo, Claude Alain

Absolute pitch (AP) is the ability to identify an auditory pitch without prior context. Current theories posit AP involves automatic retrieval of referents. We tested interference in well-matched AP musicians, non-AP musicians, and nonmusicians with three auditory Stroop tasks. Stimuli were one of two sung pitches with congruent or incongruent verbal cues. The tasks used different lexicons: binary concrete adjectives (i.e., words: Low/High), syllables with no obvious semantic properties (i.e., solmization: Do/So), and abstract semiotic labels (i.e., orthographic: C/G). Participants were instructed to respond to pitch regardless of verbal information during electroencephalographic recording. Incongruent stimuli of words and solmization tasks increased errors and slowed response times (RTs), which was reversed in nonmusicians for the orthographic task. AP musicians made virtually no errors, but their RTs slowed for incongruent stimuli. Frontal theta (4-7 Hz) event-related synchrony was significantly enhanced during incongruence between 350 and 550 ms poststimulus onset in AP, regardless of lexicon or behavior. This effect was found in non-AP musicians and nonmusicians for word task, while orthographic task showed a reverse theta congruency effect. Findings suggest theta synchrony indexes conflict detection in AP. High beta (21-29 Hz) desynchrony indexes response conflict detection in non-AP musicians. Alpha (8-12 Hz) synchrony may reflect top-down attention.

绝对音高(AP)是在没有事先背景的情况下识别听觉音高的能力。目前的理论认为,AP涉及到指称物的自动检索。我们用三个听觉Stroop任务测试了匹配良好的AP音乐家、非AP音乐家和非音乐家的干扰。刺激物是两种具有一致或不一致语言提示的歌唱音高之一。这些任务使用了不同的词汇:二元具体形容词(即单词:Low/High)、没有明显语义属性的音节(即solmization: Do/So)和抽象符号标签(即正字法:C/G)。在脑电图记录中,参与者被指示对音高做出反应,而不考虑口头信息。单词和记忆任务的不一致刺激增加了错误并减慢了反应时间(RTs),而非音乐家在正字法任务中则相反。AP音乐家几乎没有犯错误,但他们的rt在不一致的刺激下减慢了。在刺激后350 - 550 ms,无论词汇或行为如何,前额叶θ波(4-7 Hz)事件相关的同步性显著增强。在单词任务中,非ap乐手和非乐手表现出这种效应,而正字法任务则表现出相反的θ一致性效应。研究结果表明,theta同步性是AP音乐家冲突检测的指标,而高β (21-29 Hz)非AP音乐家冲突检测的指标。α (8- 12hz)同步性可能反映自上而下的注意力。
{"title":"Neural Dynamics of Inhibitory Control in Musicians with Absolute Pitch: Theta Synchrony as an Oscillatory Signature of Information Conflict.","authors":"Vivek V Sharma,&nbsp;Michael Thaut,&nbsp;Frank A Russo,&nbsp;Claude Alain","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab043","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Absolute pitch (AP) is the ability to identify an auditory pitch without prior context. Current theories posit AP involves automatic retrieval of referents. We tested interference in well-matched AP musicians, non-AP musicians, and nonmusicians with three auditory Stroop tasks. Stimuli were one of two sung pitches with congruent or incongruent verbal cues. The tasks used different lexicons: binary concrete adjectives (i.e., words: <i>Low</i>/<i>High</i>), syllables with no obvious semantic properties (i.e., solmization: <i>Do</i>/<i>So</i>), and abstract semiotic labels (i.e., orthographic: <i>C</i>/<i>G</i>). Participants were instructed to respond to pitch regardless of verbal information during electroencephalographic recording. Incongruent stimuli of words and solmization tasks increased errors and slowed response times (RTs), which was reversed in nonmusicians for the orthographic task. AP musicians made virtually no errors, but their RTs slowed for incongruent stimuli. Frontal theta (4-7 Hz) event-related synchrony was significantly enhanced during incongruence between 350 and 550 ms poststimulus onset in AP, regardless of lexicon or behavior. This effect was found in non-AP musicians and nonmusicians for word task, while orthographic task showed a reverse theta congruency effect. Findings suggest theta synchrony indexes conflict detection in AP. High beta (21-29 Hz) desynchrony indexes response conflict detection in non-AP musicians. Alpha (8-12 Hz) synchrony may reflect top-down attention.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8423588/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39410242","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Functional Imaging of the Cerebellum during Action Execution and Observation. 动作执行和观察过程中小脑的功能成像。
Pub Date : 2021-06-30 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab041
Vassilis Raos, Helen E Savaki

We employed the 14C-deoxyglucose autoradiographic method to map the activity in the cerebellar cortex of rhesus monkeys that performed forelimb movements either in the light or in the dark and of monkeys that observed forelimb movements executed by a human experimenter. The execution of forelimb movements, both in the light and in the dark, activated the forelimb representations in the cerebellar hemispheric extensions of 1) vermian lobules IV-VI and 2) vermian lobule VIIIB, ipsilaterally to the moving forelimb. Activations in the former forelimb representation involved both a paravermal and a lateral hemispheric region. Also, Crus II posterior in the ansiform lobule (the hemispheric expansion of lobule VIIB) was activated bilaterally by execution of movements in the light but not in the dark. Action observation activated the lateral-most region of the forelimb representation in the lateral hemispheric extension of vermian lobules IV-VI, as well as the crus II posterior, bilaterally. Our results demonstrate that the cerebellar cortex, in addition to its involvement in the generation of movement, is also recruited in the perception of observed movements. Moreover, our findings suggest a modularity gradient in the primate cerebellar cortex, which progresses from unimodal (medially) to multimodal (laterally) functional areas.

我们采用14c -脱氧葡萄糖放射自显影法绘制了在光照或黑暗中进行前肢运动的恒河猴和观察人类实验者进行前肢运动的猴子的小脑皮层活动。无论是在光照下还是在黑暗中,前肢运动的执行都激活了与前肢运动同侧的小脑半球延伸的1)蠕虫小叶IV-VI和2)蠕虫小叶viii - b中的前肢表征。前前肢表征的激活涉及皮层旁和外侧半球区域。此外,在光线下而不是在黑暗中执行运动时,双侧的异状小叶(VIIB小叶的半球扩张)后II脚被激活。动作观察激活了前肢代表的第iv -第vi小叶外侧半球延伸的外侧大部分区域,以及双侧后的第II小腿。我们的研究结果表明,小脑皮层除了参与运动的产生外,还参与观察到的运动的感知。此外,我们的研究结果表明,灵长类小脑皮层存在模块化梯度,从单峰(内侧)到多峰(外侧)功能区。
{"title":"Functional Imaging of the Cerebellum during Action Execution and Observation.","authors":"Vassilis Raos,&nbsp;Helen E Savaki","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab041","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab041","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We employed the <sup>14</sup>C-deoxyglucose autoradiographic method to map the activity in the cerebellar cortex of rhesus monkeys that performed forelimb movements either in the light or in the dark and of monkeys that observed forelimb movements executed by a human experimenter. The execution of forelimb movements, both in the light and in the dark, activated the forelimb representations in the cerebellar hemispheric extensions of 1) vermian lobules IV-VI and 2) vermian lobule VIIIB, ipsilaterally to the moving forelimb. Activations in the former forelimb representation involved both a paravermal and a lateral hemispheric region. Also, Crus II posterior in the ansiform lobule (the hemispheric expansion of lobule VIIB) was activated bilaterally by execution of movements in the light but not in the dark. Action observation activated the lateral-most region of the forelimb representation in the lateral hemispheric extension of vermian lobules IV-VI, as well as the crus II posterior, bilaterally. Our results demonstrate that the cerebellar cortex, in addition to its involvement in the generation of movement, is also recruited in the perception of observed movements. Moreover, our findings suggest a modularity gradient in the primate cerebellar cortex, which progresses from unimodal (medially) to multimodal (laterally) functional areas.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8366719/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39324306","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Dopaminergic Basis of Spatial Deficits in Early Parkinson's Disease. 早期帕金森病空间缺陷的多巴胺能基础。
Pub Date : 2021-06-24 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab042
B Hanna-Pladdy, R Pahwa, K E Lyons

Dopaminergic mechanisms regulating cognitive and motor control were evaluated comparing visuoperceptual and perceptuomotor functions in Parkinson's disease (PD). The performance of PD patients (n = 40) was contrasted with healthy controls (n = 42) across two separate visits (on and off dopaminergic medications) on computerized tasks of perception and aiming to a target at variable stimulus lengths (4, 8, 12 cm). Novel visuoperceptual tasks of length equivalence and width interval estimations without motor demands were compared with tasks estimating spatial deviation in movement termination. The findings support the presence of spatial deficits in early PD, more pronounced with increased discrimination difficulty, and with shorter stimulus lengths of 4 cm for both visuoperceptual and perceptumotor functions. Dopaminergic medication had an adverse impact on visuoperceptual accuracy in particular for length equivalence estimations, in contrast with dopaminergic modulation of perceptuomotor functions that reduced angular displacements toward the target. The differential outcomes for spatial accuracy in perception versus movement termination in PD are consistent with involvement of the direct pathway and models of progressive loss of dopamine through corticostriatal loops. Future research should develop validated and sensitive standardized tests of perception and explore dopaminergic selective deficits in PD to optimize medication titration for motor and cognitive symptoms of the disease.

比较帕金森病(PD)的视觉和感知运动功能,评估多巴胺能调节认知和运动控制的机制。PD患者(n = 40)与健康对照(n = 42)在两次单独访问(使用和不使用多巴胺能药物)时,在计算机化感知任务和针对不同刺激长度(4、8、12厘米)的目标上的表现进行了对比。将无运动需求的长度等效和宽度间隔估计的新型视觉任务与运动终止空间偏差估计任务进行了比较。研究结果支持了早期PD中空间缺陷的存在,随着辨别困难的增加以及视觉和感知运动功能的刺激长度较短(4cm)而更加明显。多巴胺能药物对视觉知觉准确性有不利影响,特别是长度等效估计,与多巴胺能调节感知运动功能相比,减少了向目标的角位移。PD患者感知空间准确性与运动终止的差异结果与多巴胺通过皮质纹状体回路进行性损失的直接途径和模型的参与一致。未来的研究应开发有效的、敏感的标准化感知测试,并探索帕金森病的多巴胺能选择性缺陷,以优化该疾病运动和认知症状的药物滴定。
{"title":"Dopaminergic Basis of Spatial Deficits in Early Parkinson's Disease.","authors":"B Hanna-Pladdy,&nbsp;R Pahwa,&nbsp;K E Lyons","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab042","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dopaminergic mechanisms regulating cognitive and motor control were evaluated comparing visuoperceptual and perceptuomotor functions in Parkinson's disease (PD). The performance of PD patients (n = 40) was contrasted with healthy controls (n = 42) across two separate visits (<i>on</i> and <i>off</i> dopaminergic medications) on computerized tasks of perception and aiming to a target at variable stimulus lengths (4, 8, 12 cm). Novel visuoperceptual tasks of length equivalence and width interval estimations without motor demands were compared with tasks estimating spatial deviation in movement termination. The findings support the presence of spatial deficits in early PD, more pronounced with increased discrimination difficulty, and with shorter stimulus lengths of 4 cm for both visuoperceptual and perceptumotor functions. Dopaminergic medication had an adverse impact on visuoperceptual accuracy in particular for length equivalence estimations, in contrast with dopaminergic modulation of perceptuomotor functions that reduced angular displacements toward the target. The differential outcomes for spatial accuracy in perception versus movement termination in PD are consistent with involvement of the direct pathway and models of progressive loss of dopamine through corticostriatal loops. Future research should develop validated and sensitive standardized tests of perception and explore dopaminergic selective deficits in PD to optimize medication titration for motor and cognitive symptoms of the disease.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8436572/pdf/tgab042.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39592359","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Same Ultra-Rapid Parallel Brain Dynamics Underpin the Production and Perception of Speech. 同样的超快速并行大脑动力支撑着语音的产生和感知。
Pub Date : 2021-06-10 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab040
Amie Fairs, Amandine Michelas, Sophie Dufour, Kristof Strijkers

The temporal dynamics by which linguistic information becomes available is one of the key properties to understand how language is organized in the brain. An unresolved debate between different brain language models is whether words, the building blocks of language, are activated in a sequential or parallel manner. In this study, we approached this issue from a novel perspective by directly comparing the time course of word component activation in speech production versus perception. In an overt object naming task and a passive listening task, we analyzed with mixed linear models at the single-trial level the event-related brain potentials elicited by the same lexico-semantic and phonological word knowledge in the two language modalities. Results revealed that both word components manifested simultaneously as early as 75 ms after stimulus onset in production and perception; differences between the language modalities only became apparent after 300 ms of processing. The data provide evidence for ultra-rapid parallel dynamics of language processing and are interpreted within a neural assembly framework where words recruit the same integrated cell assemblies across production and perception. These word assemblies ignite early on in parallel and only later on reverberate in a behavior-specific manner.

语言信息的时间动态变化是了解大脑如何组织语言的关键特性之一。不同大脑语言模型之间的一个悬而未决的争论点是,语言的组成部分--单词是以顺序还是并行的方式被激活的。在这项研究中,我们从一个新颖的角度探讨了这一问题,直接比较了语音生成和感知中单词成分激活的时间过程。在一个公开的物体命名任务和一个被动的听力任务中,我们使用混合线性模型在单次试验水平上分析了在两种语言模式中相同的词汇语义和语音单词知识所引起的事件相关脑电位。结果显示,早在刺激开始后 75 毫秒,这两个词的成分就同时出现在生产和感知中;语言模式之间的差异只有在处理 300 毫秒后才变得明显。这些数据为语言处理的超快速并行动力学提供了证据,并在神经集合框架内进行了解释。这些单词集合在早期就开始并行启动,之后才以特定行为的方式产生反响。
{"title":"The Same Ultra-Rapid Parallel Brain Dynamics Underpin the Production and Perception of Speech.","authors":"Amie Fairs, Amandine Michelas, Sophie Dufour, Kristof Strijkers","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab040","DOIUrl":"10.1093/texcom/tgab040","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The temporal dynamics by which linguistic information becomes available is one of the key properties to understand how language is organized in the brain. An unresolved debate between different brain language models is whether words, the building blocks of language, are activated in a sequential or parallel manner. In this study, we approached this issue from a novel perspective by directly comparing the time course of word component activation in speech production versus perception. In an overt object naming task and a passive listening task, we analyzed with mixed linear models at the single-trial level the event-related brain potentials elicited by the same lexico-semantic and phonological word knowledge in the two language modalities. Results revealed that both word components manifested simultaneously as early as 75 ms after stimulus onset in production and perception; differences between the language modalities only became apparent after 300 ms of processing. The data provide evidence for ultra-rapid parallel dynamics of language processing and are interpreted within a neural assembly framework where words recruit the same integrated cell assemblies across production and perception. These word assemblies ignite early on in parallel and only later on reverberate in a behavior-specific manner.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8262084/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Placebo Analgesia Does Not Reduce Empathy for Naturalistic Depictions of Others' Pain in a Somatosensory Specific Way. 安慰剂镇痛并不会减少以躯体感觉特定方式对他人痛苦的自然描述的同理心。
Pub Date : 2021-06-02 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab039
Helena Hartmann, Federica Riva, Markus Rütgen, Claus Lamm

The shared representations account postulates that sharing another's pain recruits underlying brain functions also engaged during first-hand pain. Critically, direct causal evidence for this was mainly shown for affective pain processing, while the contribution of somatosensory processes to empathy remains controversial. This controversy may be explained, however, by experimental paradigms that did not direct attention towards a specific body part, or that did not employ naturalistic depictions of others' pain. In this preregistered functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we aimed to test whether causal manipulation of first-hand pain affects empathy for naturalistic depictions of pain in a somatosensory-matched manner. Forty-five participants underwent a placebo analgesia induction in their right hand and observed pictures of other people's right and left hands in pain. We found neither behavioral nor neural evidence for somatosensory-specific modulation of pain empathy. However, exploratory analyses revealed a general effect of the placebo on empathy, and higher brain activity in bilateral anterior insula when viewing others' right hands in pain (i.e., corresponding to one's own placebo hand). These results refine our knowledge regarding the neural mechanisms of pain empathy, and imply that the sharing of somatosensory representations seems to play less of a causal role than the one of affective representations.

共享表征假设,共享他人的疼痛会招募在第一手疼痛期间也参与其中的潜在大脑功能。至关重要的是,这方面的直接因果证据主要表现为情感疼痛处理,而体感过程对移情的贡献仍然存在争议。然而,这种争议可以用实验范式来解释,这些范式没有将注意力集中在特定的身体部位,或者没有对他人的疼痛进行自然主义的描述。在这项预先注册的功能性磁共振成像研究中,我们旨在测试对第一手疼痛的因果操纵是否会以体感匹配的方式影响对疼痛的自然描述的同理心。45名参与者右手接受了安慰剂镇痛诱导,并观察了其他人左手和右手疼痛的照片。我们既没有发现躯体感觉特异性调节疼痛移情的行为证据,也没有发现神经证据。然而,探索性分析揭示了安慰剂对同理心的普遍影响,以及当看到他人疼痛的右手(即与自己的安慰剂手相对应)时,双侧前脑岛的大脑活动更高。这些结果完善了我们对疼痛移情神经机制的认识,并暗示体感表征的共享似乎比情感表征的共享起到的因果作用更小。
{"title":"Placebo Analgesia Does Not Reduce Empathy for Naturalistic Depictions of Others' Pain in a Somatosensory Specific Way.","authors":"Helena Hartmann, Federica Riva, Markus Rütgen, Claus Lamm","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab039","DOIUrl":"10.1093/texcom/tgab039","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The shared representations account postulates that sharing another's pain recruits underlying brain functions also engaged during first-hand pain. Critically, direct causal evidence for this was mainly shown for affective pain processing, while the contribution of somatosensory processes to empathy remains controversial. This controversy may be explained, however, by experimental paradigms that did not direct attention towards a specific body part, or that did not employ naturalistic depictions of others' pain. In this preregistered functional magnetic resonance imaging study, we aimed to test whether causal manipulation of first-hand pain affects empathy for naturalistic depictions of pain in a somatosensory-matched manner. Forty-five participants underwent a placebo analgesia induction in their right hand and observed pictures of other people's right and left hands in pain. We found neither behavioral nor neural evidence for somatosensory-specific modulation of pain empathy. However, exploratory analyses revealed a general effect of the placebo on empathy, and higher brain activity in bilateral anterior insula when viewing others' right hands in pain (i.e., corresponding to one's own placebo hand). These results refine our knowledge regarding the neural mechanisms of pain empathy, and imply that the sharing of somatosensory representations seems to play less of a causal role than the one of affective representations.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-06-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8276832/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cerebellar and Cortical Correlates of Internal and External Speech Error Monitoring. 内部和外部语音错误监测的小脑和皮层相关性
Pub Date : 2021-05-31 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab038
Elin Runnqvist, Valérie Chanoine, Kristof Strijkers, Chotiga Pattamadilok, Mireille Bonnard, Bruno Nazarian, Julien Sein, Jean-Luc Anton, Lydia Dorokhova, Pascal Belin, F-Xavier Alario

An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study examined how speakers inspect their own speech for errors. Concretely, we sought to assess 1) the role of the temporal cortex in monitoring speech errors, linked with comprehension-based monitoring; 2) the involvement of the cerebellum in internal and external monitoring, linked with forward modeling; and 3) the role of the medial frontal cortex for internal monitoring, linked with conflict-based monitoring. In a word production task priming speech errors, we observed enhanced involvement of the right posterior cerebellum for trials that were correct, but on which participants were more likely to make a word as compared with a nonword error (contrast of internal monitoring). Furthermore, comparing errors to correct utterances (contrast of external monitoring), we observed increased activation of the same cerebellar region, of the superior medial cerebellum, and of regions in temporal and medial frontal cortex. The presence of the cerebellum for both internal and external monitoring indicates the use of forward modeling across the planning and articulation of speech. Dissociations across internal and external monitoring in temporal and medial frontal cortex indicate that monitoring of overt errors is more reliant on vocal feedback control.

一项事件相关功能磁共振成像研究考察了说话者如何检查自己的语音是否有误。具体来说,我们试图评估:1)颞叶皮层在监测语音错误中的作用,这与基于理解的监测有关;2)小脑在内部和外部监测中的参与,这与前向建模有关;3)内侧额叶皮层在内部监测中的作用,这与基于冲突的监测有关。在一项以言语错误为引子的单词生成任务中,我们观察到右侧小脑后部参与了正确的试验,但与非单词错误相比,参与者更有可能在这些试验中出现单词错误(内部监控对比)。此外,在将错误与正确的语句进行比较时(外部监控对比),我们观察到同一小脑区域、内侧小脑上部以及颞叶和内侧额叶皮层区域的激活增加。小脑在内部和外部监控中的存在表明,在语音的规划和发音过程中使用了前向建模。颞叶和额叶内侧皮层内部和外部监控的分离表明,对明显错误的监控更依赖于发声反馈控制。
{"title":"Cerebellar and Cortical Correlates of Internal and External Speech Error Monitoring.","authors":"Elin Runnqvist, Valérie Chanoine, Kristof Strijkers, Chotiga Pattamadilok, Mireille Bonnard, Bruno Nazarian, Julien Sein, Jean-Luc Anton, Lydia Dorokhova, Pascal Belin, F-Xavier Alario","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab038","DOIUrl":"10.1093/texcom/tgab038","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study examined how speakers inspect their own speech for errors. Concretely, we sought to assess 1) the role of the temporal cortex in monitoring speech errors, linked with comprehension-based monitoring; 2) the involvement of the cerebellum in internal and external monitoring, linked with forward modeling; and 3) the role of the medial frontal cortex for internal monitoring, linked with conflict-based monitoring. In a word production task priming speech errors, we observed enhanced involvement of the right posterior cerebellum for trials that were correct, but on which participants were more likely to make a word as compared with a nonword error (contrast of internal monitoring). Furthermore, comparing errors to correct utterances (contrast of external monitoring), we observed increased activation of the same cerebellar region, of the superior medial cerebellum, and of regions in temporal and medial frontal cortex. The presence of the cerebellum for both internal and external monitoring indicates the use of forward modeling across the planning and articulation of speech. Dissociations across internal and external monitoring in temporal and medial frontal cortex indicate that monitoring of overt errors is more reliant on vocal feedback control.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8237718/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211443","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brain Plasticity Reflects Specialized Cognitive Development Induced by Musical Training. 大脑可塑性反映了音乐训练引起的特殊认知发展。
Pub Date : 2021-05-31 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab037
Uk-Su Choi, Yul-Wan Sung, Seiji Ogawa

Learning a musical instrument requires a long period of training and might induce structural and functional changes in the brain. Previous studies have shown brain plasticity resulting from training with a musical instrument. However, these studies did not distinguish the effects on brain plasticity of specific musical instruments as they examined the brain of musicians who had learned a single musical instrument/genre and did not control for confounding factors, such as common or interactive effects involved in music training. To address this research gap, the present work investigated musicians who had experience with both a piano and a wind instrument, for example, flute, trumpet, clarinet etc. By examining the difference between the 2 musical instruments in the same subject, we avoided the effects common to all musical instruments and the confounding factors. Therefore, we identified several high-tier brain areas displaying a brain plasticity specific to each musical instrument. Our findings show that learning a musical instrument might result in the development of high cognitive functions reflecting the skills/abilities unique to the instrument played.

学习一种乐器需要长时间的训练,可能会引起大脑结构和功能的变化。先前的研究表明,使用乐器训练会导致大脑的可塑性。然而,这些研究并没有区分特定乐器对大脑可塑性的影响,因为他们检查的是学习过单一乐器/流派的音乐家的大脑,并且没有控制混杂因素,例如音乐训练中涉及的共同或互动影响。为了解决这一研究差距,目前的工作调查了有钢琴和管乐器经验的音乐家,例如,长笛,小号,单簧管等。通过检查同一科目中两种乐器之间的差异,我们避免了所有乐器共同的影响和混淆因素。因此,我们确定了几个高级大脑区域,显示出每种乐器特有的大脑可塑性。我们的研究结果表明,学习一种乐器可能会导致高级认知功能的发展,这反映了所演奏乐器所特有的技能/能力。
{"title":"Brain Plasticity Reflects Specialized Cognitive Development Induced by Musical Training.","authors":"Uk-Su Choi,&nbsp;Yul-Wan Sung,&nbsp;Seiji Ogawa","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab037","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab037","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning a musical instrument requires a long period of training and might induce structural and functional changes in the brain. Previous studies have shown brain plasticity resulting from training with a musical instrument. However, these studies did not distinguish the effects on brain plasticity of specific musical instruments as they examined the brain of musicians who had learned a single musical instrument/genre and did not control for confounding factors, such as common or interactive effects involved in music training. To address this research gap, the present work investigated musicians who had experience with both a piano and a wind instrument, for example, flute, trumpet, clarinet etc. By examining the difference between the 2 musical instruments in the same subject, we avoided the effects common to all musical instruments and the confounding factors. Therefore, we identified several high-tier brain areas displaying a brain plasticity specific to each musical instrument. Our findings show that learning a musical instrument might result in the development of high cognitive functions reflecting the skills/abilities unique to the instrument played.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8213952/pdf/tgab037.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Immunohistological Examination of AKT Isoforms in the Brain: Cell-Type Specificity That May Underlie AKT's Role in Complex Brain Disorders and Neurological Disease. 大脑中AKT亚型的免疫组织学检查:细胞类型特异性可能是AKT在复杂脑部疾病和神经系统疾病中作用的基础。
Pub Date : 2021-05-28 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab036
Josien Levenga, Helen Wong, Ryan Milstead, Lauren LaPlante, Charles A Hoeffer

Protein kinase B (PKB/AKT) is a central kinase involved in many neurobiological processes. AKT is expressed in the brain as three isoforms, AKT1, AKT2, and AKT3. Previous studies suggest isoform-specific roles in neural function, but very few studies have examined AKT isoform expression at the cellular level. In this study, we use a combination of histology, immunostaining, and genetics to characterize cell-type-specific expression of AKT isoforms in human and mouse brains. In mice, we find that AKT1 is the most broadly expressed isoform, with expression in excitatory neurons and the sole detectable AKT isoform in gamma-aminobutyric acid ergic interneurons and microglia. By contrast, we find that AKT2 is the sole isoform expressed in astroglia and is not detected in other neural cell types. We find that AKT3 is expressed in excitatory neurons with AKT1 but shows greater expression levels in dendritic compartments than AKT1. We extend our analysis to human brain tissues and find similar results. Using genetic deletion approaches, we also find that the cellular determinants restricting AKT isoform expression to specific cell types remain intact under Akt deficiency conditions. Because AKT signaling is linked to numerous neurological disorders, a greater understanding of cell-specific isoform expression could improve treatment strategies involving AKT.

蛋白激酶B (PKB/AKT)是参与许多神经生物学过程的中枢激酶。AKT在大脑中以AKT1、AKT2和AKT3三种亚型表达。以往的研究表明,同种异构体在神经功能中具有特异性作用,但很少有研究在细胞水平上检测AKT同种异构体的表达。在这项研究中,我们使用组织学、免疫染色和遗传学相结合的方法来表征人类和小鼠大脑中AKT亚型的细胞类型特异性表达。在小鼠中,我们发现AKT1是最广泛表达的亚型,在兴奋性神经元中表达,而在γ -氨基丁酸能间神经元和小胶质细胞中唯一可检测到的AKT亚型。相比之下,我们发现AKT2是唯一在星形胶质细胞中表达的亚型,在其他神经细胞类型中未检测到。我们发现AKT3与AKT1一起在兴奋性神经元中表达,但在树突状室中的表达水平高于AKT1。我们将我们的分析扩展到人脑组织,并发现了类似的结果。利用基因缺失方法,我们还发现,在AKT缺乏的条件下,限制AKT亚型表达的细胞决定因素保持完整。由于AKT信号与许多神经系统疾病有关,因此对细胞特异性异构体表达的更深入了解可以改善涉及AKT的治疗策略。
{"title":"Immunohistological Examination of AKT Isoforms in the Brain: Cell-Type Specificity That May Underlie AKT's Role in Complex Brain Disorders and Neurological Disease.","authors":"Josien Levenga,&nbsp;Helen Wong,&nbsp;Ryan Milstead,&nbsp;Lauren LaPlante,&nbsp;Charles A Hoeffer","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab036","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab036","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Protein kinase B (PKB/AKT) is a central kinase involved in many neurobiological processes. AKT is expressed in the brain as three isoforms, AKT1, AKT2, and AKT3. Previous studies suggest isoform-specific roles in neural function, but very few studies have examined AKT isoform expression at the cellular level. In this study, we use a combination of histology, immunostaining, and genetics to characterize cell-type-specific expression of AKT isoforms in human and mouse brains. In mice, we find that AKT1 is the most broadly expressed isoform, with expression in excitatory neurons and the sole detectable AKT isoform in gamma-aminobutyric acid ergic interneurons and microglia. By contrast, we find that AKT2 is the sole isoform expressed in astroglia and is not detected in other neural cell types. We find that AKT3 is expressed in excitatory neurons with AKT1 but shows greater expression levels in dendritic compartments than AKT1. We extend our analysis to human brain tissues and find similar results. Using genetic deletion approaches, we also find that the cellular determinants restricting AKT isoform expression to specific cell types remain intact under <i>Akt</i> deficiency conditions. Because AKT signaling is linked to numerous neurological disorders, a greater understanding of cell-specific isoform expression could improve treatment strategies involving AKT.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8223503/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
On the Complexity of Resting State Spiking Activity in Monkey Motor Cortex. 猴子运动皮层静息状态尖峰活动的复杂性研究。
Pub Date : 2021-05-18 eCollection Date: 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1093/texcom/tgab033
Paulina Anna Dąbrowska, Nicole Voges, Michael von Papen, Junji Ito, David Dahmen, Alexa Riehle, Thomas Brochier, Sonja Grün

Resting state has been established as a classical paradigm of brain activity studies, mostly based on large-scale measurements such as functional magnetic resonance imaging or magneto- and electroencephalography. This term typically refers to a behavioral state characterized by the absence of any task or stimuli. The corresponding neuronal activity is often called idle or ongoing. Numerous modeling studies on spiking neural networks claim to mimic such idle states, but compare their results with task- or stimulus-driven experiments, or to results from experiments with anesthetized subjects. Both approaches might lead to misleading conclusions. To provide a proper basis for comparing physiological and simulated network dynamics, we characterize simultaneously recorded single neurons' spiking activity in monkey motor cortex at rest and show the differences from spontaneous and task- or stimulus-induced movement conditions. We also distinguish between rest with open eyes and sleepy rest with eyes closed. The resting state with open eyes shows a significantly higher dimensionality, reduced firing rates, and less balance between population level excitation and inhibition than behavior-related states.

静息状态已被确立为脑活动研究的经典范式,主要基于大规模测量,如功能性磁共振成像或脑磁图和脑电图。这个术语通常指的是一种行为状态,其特征是没有任何任务或刺激。相应的神经元活动通常被称为空闲或进行中。许多关于尖峰神经网络的建模研究声称可以模拟这种空闲状态,但将其结果与任务或刺激驱动的实验或麻醉受试者的实验结果进行比较。这两种方法都可能导致误导性的结论。为了提供适当的基础来比较生理和模拟的网络动力学,我们描述了同时记录的猴子运动皮层中单个神经元在休息时的峰值活动,并显示了自发和任务或刺激诱导的运动条件的差异。我们也区分睁着眼睛休息和闭着眼睛睡觉休息。与行为相关状态相比,睁开眼睛的静息状态表现出明显更高的维数、更低的放电率和更低的群体水平兴奋和抑制之间的平衡。
{"title":"On the Complexity of Resting State Spiking Activity in Monkey Motor Cortex.","authors":"Paulina Anna Dąbrowska,&nbsp;Nicole Voges,&nbsp;Michael von Papen,&nbsp;Junji Ito,&nbsp;David Dahmen,&nbsp;Alexa Riehle,&nbsp;Thomas Brochier,&nbsp;Sonja Grün","doi":"10.1093/texcom/tgab033","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab033","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Resting state has been established as a classical paradigm of brain activity studies, mostly based on large-scale measurements such as functional magnetic resonance imaging or magneto- and electroencephalography. This term typically refers to a behavioral state characterized by the absence of any task or stimuli. The corresponding neuronal activity is often called idle or ongoing. Numerous modeling studies on spiking neural networks claim to mimic such idle states, but compare their results with task- or stimulus-driven experiments, or to results from experiments with anesthetized subjects. Both approaches might lead to misleading conclusions. To provide a proper basis for comparing physiological and simulated network dynamics, we characterize simultaneously recorded single neurons' spiking activity in monkey motor cortex at rest and show the differences from spontaneous and task- or stimulus-induced movement conditions. We also distinguish between rest with open eyes and sleepy rest with eyes closed. The resting state with open eyes shows a significantly higher dimensionality, reduced firing rates, and less balance between population level excitation and inhibition than behavior-related states.</p>","PeriodicalId":72551,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex communications","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1093/texcom/tgab033","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39211444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cerebral cortex communications
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1