首页 > 最新文献

Journal of speech and hearing research最新文献

英文 中文
Individual differences and the reliability of 2F1-F2 distortion-product otoacoustic emissions: effects of time-of-day, stimulus variables, and gender. 个体差异和2F1-F2失真产物耳声发射的可靠性:时间、刺激变量和性别的影响。
Pub Date : 1996-12-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1138
A T Cacace, W A McClelland, J Weiner, D J McFarland

Distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) measured from the ear canal can be a sensitive tool to detect changes in cochlear function over time. However, if multiple-measurement procedures are to be useful clinically, testing needs to be reliable and sources of variability within individuals should be known. Herein, the influence of time-of-day (TOD), stimulus frequency, stimulus sound pressure level (SPL), and gender were evaluated on 2f1-f2 DPOAE amplitude in 16 adult volunteers with normal hearing. The effects of oral temperature and resting-pulse rate were also assessed. This study demonstrated a TOD main effect, with a period approximating one cycle-per-day. The magnitude of this effect averaged less than one dB and was not dependent on stimulus (frequency or SPL) or participant variables (gender, oral temperature, or resting-pulse rate), nor was it synchronized to a particular point-in-time. Stimulus level and gender effects on DPOAEs across frequency were also observed. Using generalizability theory (GT), DP iso-level/frequency profiles (DPILFPs) were found to be reliable measures within-subjects over a contiguous 24-hour time period. Significant and reliable between-subject differences were also documented. This study demonstrates the influence of stimulus and participant variables, quantifies the within-subject reliability over a 24-hour time period, and confirms that significant and reliable between-subject differences exist on DPOAEs across frequency, SPL, and gender.

从耳道测量畸变产物耳声发射(dpoae)可以作为检测耳蜗功能随时间变化的敏感工具。然而,如果多种测量方法在临床上是有用的,测试必须是可靠的,并且应该知道个体内部变异的来源。本研究以16名听力正常的成年志愿者为研究对象,评估了刺激时间(TOD)、刺激频率、刺激声压级(SPL)和性别对2f1-f2 DPOAE振幅的影响。同时评估口腔温度和静息脉搏率的影响。这项研究证明了TOD的主要作用,其周期约为每天一个周期。这种效应的幅度平均小于1分贝,不依赖于刺激(频率或声压级)或参与者变量(性别、口腔温度或静息脉搏率),也不与特定的时间点同步。刺激水平和性别对不同频率dpoae的影响也有所不同。利用广义理论(GT), DP等水平/频率分布(DPILFPs)被发现是连续24小时内受试者内部的可靠测量。受试者之间显著且可靠的差异也被记录下来。本研究证明了刺激变量和被试变量的影响,量化了24小时内的被试内信度,并证实了不同频率、SPL和性别的被试间在dpoae上存在显著和可靠的差异。
{"title":"Individual differences and the reliability of 2F1-F2 distortion-product otoacoustic emissions: effects of time-of-day, stimulus variables, and gender.","authors":"A T Cacace,&nbsp;W A McClelland,&nbsp;J Weiner,&nbsp;D J McFarland","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3906.1138","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1138","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) measured from the ear canal can be a sensitive tool to detect changes in cochlear function over time. However, if multiple-measurement procedures are to be useful clinically, testing needs to be reliable and sources of variability within individuals should be known. Herein, the influence of time-of-day (TOD), stimulus frequency, stimulus sound pressure level (SPL), and gender were evaluated on 2f1-f2 DPOAE amplitude in 16 adult volunteers with normal hearing. The effects of oral temperature and resting-pulse rate were also assessed. This study demonstrated a TOD main effect, with a period approximating one cycle-per-day. The magnitude of this effect averaged less than one dB and was not dependent on stimulus (frequency or SPL) or participant variables (gender, oral temperature, or resting-pulse rate), nor was it synchronized to a particular point-in-time. Stimulus level and gender effects on DPOAEs across frequency were also observed. Using generalizability theory (GT), DP iso-level/frequency profiles (DPILFPs) were found to be reliable measures within-subjects over a contiguous 24-hour time period. Significant and reliable between-subject differences were also documented. This study demonstrates the influence of stimulus and participant variables, quantifies the within-subject reliability over a 24-hour time period, and confirms that significant and reliable between-subject differences exist on DPOAEs across frequency, SPL, and gender.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 6","pages":"1138-48"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3906.1138","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19922453","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 54
Word frequency and age effects in normally developing children's phonological processing. 词频和年龄对正常发育儿童语音加工的影响。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.1099
G A Troia, F P Roth, G H Yeni-Komshian
Eleven kindergarten-age students and 11 second-grade students were asked to perform each of four phonological processing tasks: (a) confrontation naming of object drawings, (b) rapid sequential naming of object drawings and letters, (c) segmentation of words into sounds, and (d) blending sounds to produce words. Response accuracy and, for the picture naming tasks, response latency were measured. In addition, single-word reading ability and silent reading comprehension were evaluated. Results indicated that high-frequency stimuli were named faster and, in one task, more accurately than low-frequency stimuli. Blending sounds to produce high-frequency words was less difficult than blending sounds to produce low-frequency words, but word frequency did not affect sound segmentation performance. Children in second grade generally were faster and more accurate than kindergarten children in naming pictures. They also were able to segment more sounds and correctly blend sounds to produce more target words than kindergarten students. Confrontation naming accuracy, rapid object- and letter-naming latency, and sound segmentation and blending accuracy were intercorrelated and were related to word recognition and to reading comprehension. Serial naming speed was highly related to phonological awareness in kindergarten, whereas confrontation naming accuracy was highly related to phonological awareness in second grade. A limited cognitive resources framework was adopted to interpret these findings.
11名幼儿园学生和11名二年级学生分别被要求完成四个语音处理任务:(a)对物体图形进行对位命名,(b)对物体图形和字母进行快速顺序命名,(c)将单词分割成声音,(d)将声音混合成单词。对图片命名任务的反应准确性和反应延迟进行了测量。此外,还对单字阅读能力和默读理解能力进行了评价。结果表明,高频刺激被命名的速度更快,而且在一个任务中,比低频刺激更准确。混合音产生高频词比混合音产生低频词更容易,但词频不影响分音性能。二年级儿童一般比幼儿园儿童更快、更准确地命名图片。与幼儿园的学生相比,他们也能够分割更多的声音,并正确地混合声音,从而产生更多的目标单词。对峙命名正确率、快速物体和字母命名延迟、声音分割和混合正确率相互关联,并与单词识别和阅读理解相关。在幼儿园阶段,连续命名速度与语音意识高度相关,而在二年级阶段,对抗命名准确性与语音意识高度相关。采用有限认知资源框架来解释这些发现。
{"title":"Word frequency and age effects in normally developing children's phonological processing.","authors":"G A Troia,&nbsp;F P Roth,&nbsp;G H Yeni-Komshian","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.1099","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.1099","url":null,"abstract":"Eleven kindergarten-age students and 11 second-grade students were asked to perform each of four phonological processing tasks: (a) confrontation naming of object drawings, (b) rapid sequential naming of object drawings and letters, (c) segmentation of words into sounds, and (d) blending sounds to produce words. Response accuracy and, for the picture naming tasks, response latency were measured. In addition, single-word reading ability and silent reading comprehension were evaluated. Results indicated that high-frequency stimuli were named faster and, in one task, more accurately than low-frequency stimuli. Blending sounds to produce high-frequency words was less difficult than blending sounds to produce low-frequency words, but word frequency did not affect sound segmentation performance. Children in second grade generally were faster and more accurate than kindergarten children in naming pictures. They also were able to segment more sounds and correctly blend sounds to produce more target words than kindergarten students. Confrontation naming accuracy, rapid object- and letter-naming latency, and sound segmentation and blending accuracy were intercorrelated and were related to word recognition and to reading comprehension. Serial naming speed was highly related to phonological awareness in kindergarten, whereas confrontation naming accuracy was highly related to phonological awareness in second grade. A limited cognitive resources framework was adopted to interpret these findings.","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"1099-108"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.1099","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862768","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
Treatment efficacy: stuttering. 治疗功效:口吃。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.s18
E G Conture

The purpose of this article is to review the state of the art regarding treatment efficacy for stuttering in children, teenagers, and adults. Available evidence makes it apparent that individuals who stutter benefit from the services of speech-language pathologists, but it is also apparent that determining the outcome of stuttering treatment is neither easy nor simple. Whereas considerable research has documented the positive influence of treatment on stuttering frequency and behavior, far less attention has been paid to the effects of treatment on the daily life activities of people who stutter and their families. Although it seems reasonable to assume that ameliorating the disability of stuttering lessens the handicap of stuttering, considerably more evidence is needed to confirm this assumption. Despite such concerns, it also seems reasonable to suggest that the outcomes of treatment for many people who stutter are positive and should become increasingly so with advances in applied as well as basic research.

本文的目的是回顾儿童、青少年和成人口吃治疗效果的最新进展。现有的证据清楚地表明,口吃者从语言病理学家的服务中受益,但同样明显的是,确定口吃治疗的结果既不容易也不简单。尽管大量的研究记录了治疗对口吃频率和行为的积极影响,但很少有人关注治疗对口吃者及其家庭日常生活活动的影响。虽然假设改善口吃的残疾会减少口吃的障碍似乎是合理的,但需要更多的证据来证实这一假设。尽管存在这些担忧,但似乎也有理由认为,对许多口吃患者的治疗结果是积极的,随着应用研究和基础研究的进步,这种效果应该会越来越明显。
{"title":"Treatment efficacy: stuttering.","authors":"E G Conture","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.s18","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.s18","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of this article is to review the state of the art regarding treatment efficacy for stuttering in children, teenagers, and adults. Available evidence makes it apparent that individuals who stutter benefit from the services of speech-language pathologists, but it is also apparent that determining the outcome of stuttering treatment is neither easy nor simple. Whereas considerable research has documented the positive influence of treatment on stuttering frequency and behavior, far less attention has been paid to the effects of treatment on the daily life activities of people who stutter and their families. Although it seems reasonable to assume that ameliorating the disability of stuttering lessens the handicap of stuttering, considerably more evidence is needed to confirm this assumption. Despite such concerns, it also seems reasonable to suggest that the outcomes of treatment for many people who stutter are positive and should become increasingly so with advances in applied as well as basic research.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"S18-26"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.s18","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 75
Treatment efficacy: dysarthria. 治疗效果:构音障碍。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.s46
K M Yorkston

The dysarthrias form a group of diverse, chronic motor speech disorders. The disorders of Parkinson's disease, stroke, traumatic brain injury, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and cerebral palsy are reviewed because they represent important clinical diagnoses in which dysarthria is a frequent and debilitating symptom. The roles played by speech-language pathologists include participation in differential diagnosis, provision of speech treatment, staging of treatment, and timely education so that clients and families can make informed decisions about communication alternatives. Both scientific and clinical evidence is presented that suggests that individuals with dysarthria benefit from the services of speech-language pathologists. Group-treatment studies, single-subject studies, and case reports illustrate the effectiveness of various types of speech treatment. Research into the effectiveness of augmentative and alternative communication systems for individuals with cerebral palsy is also presented.

构音障碍形成了一组多样的慢性运动语言障碍。本文对帕金森病、中风、创伤性脑损伤、肌萎缩侧索硬化症和脑瘫等疾病进行综述,因为它们代表了重要的临床诊断,构音障碍是一种常见的衰弱症状。语言病理学家所扮演的角色包括参与鉴别诊断、提供语言治疗、分期治疗和及时教育,以便患者和家庭能够做出明智的沟通选择。科学和临床证据都表明,构音障碍患者受益于语言病理学家的服务。群体治疗研究、单主题研究和病例报告说明了各种类型的语言治疗的有效性。对脑瘫患者的辅助和替代沟通系统的有效性的研究也被提出。
{"title":"Treatment efficacy: dysarthria.","authors":"K M Yorkston","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.s46","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.s46","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The dysarthrias form a group of diverse, chronic motor speech disorders. The disorders of Parkinson's disease, stroke, traumatic brain injury, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and cerebral palsy are reviewed because they represent important clinical diagnoses in which dysarthria is a frequent and debilitating symptom. The roles played by speech-language pathologists include participation in differential diagnosis, provision of speech treatment, staging of treatment, and timely education so that clients and families can make informed decisions about communication alternatives. Both scientific and clinical evidence is presented that suggests that individuals with dysarthria benefit from the services of speech-language pathologists. Group-treatment studies, single-subject studies, and case reports illustrate the effectiveness of various types of speech treatment. Research into the effectiveness of augmentative and alternative communication systems for individuals with cerebral palsy is also presented.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"S46-57"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.s46","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 126
Effects of priming on the naming accuracy of preschoolers with word-finding deficits. 启动对学龄前儿童单词发现缺陷命名准确性的影响。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.1048
K K McGregor, J Windsor

Eight preschoolers with word-finding (WF) deficits and 16 controls with normal word-finding abilities (8 preschoolers and 8 adults) named 40 pictured objects under primed and unprimed conditions. Each picture could be correctly labeled with a simple noun or a compound (e.g., cane or walking stick). The primed condition involved semantic primes for both the simple and compound targets as well as a partial lexical prime for the compound targets. All participant groups decreased naming errors when given the primes. Two results indicated that the participants made use of the lexical primes. The first was a shift in form of correct responses from simple nouns in the unprimed condition to compound nouns in the primed condition. The second was an increase in errors that incorporated the lexical prime in the primed condition. There were limits to the benefit that the WF group derived from the primes. First, the primes did not enable the WF group to compensate fully for their naming problems. The gap between the error rates of the WF group and the control groups was not reduced in the primed condition. Second, the quality of errors made by the WF group did not improve in response to primes. Compared to the controls, the WF group made proportionately more errors that indicated no access to the target neighborhood (particularly "I don't know" responses) in the unprimed condition. With primes, the controls further reduced their use of these errors, but the WF group did not. When members of the control groups did make errors, they were more likely than the WF children to produce a word substitution that bore a close semantic, visual, or phonological relation to the target in both unprimed and primed conditions. These limitations on the benefit of priming for participants in the WF group suggest deficiencies in size, elaboration, or organization of their lexicons.

在启动和非启动条件下,8名有单词发现缺陷的学龄前儿童和16名具有正常单词发现能力的对照组(8名学龄前儿童和8名成人)分别说出了40个图片物体。每张图片都可以用一个简单的名词或复合词正确地标记(例如,手杖或手杖)。启动条件包括简单目标和复合目标的语义启动条件和复合目标的部分词汇启动条件。所有的参与者组在给出质数时都减少了命名错误。两个结果表明,参与者使用了词汇启动词。首先是正确回答的形式从未启动条件下的简单名词到启动条件下的复合名词的转变。第二是在启动条件下包含词汇启动的错误增加。WF组从启动项中获得的好处是有限的。首先,质数不能使WF小组完全弥补它们的命名问题。在启动条件下,WF组与对照组的误差率差距没有缩小。其次,WF组犯错误的质量并没有随着启动数的增加而提高。与对照组相比,在未启动条件下,WF组在表示无法进入目标社区(特别是“我不知道”的回答)方面的错误比例更高。对于素数,对照组进一步减少了这些错误的使用,但WF组没有。当控制组的成员犯错误时,他们比WF组的孩子更有可能在没有启动和启动的条件下产生一个与目标具有密切语义、视觉或语音关系的单词替换。对WF组的参与者来说,启动的好处的这些限制表明他们在词汇的大小、阐述或组织方面存在缺陷。
{"title":"Effects of priming on the naming accuracy of preschoolers with word-finding deficits.","authors":"K K McGregor,&nbsp;J Windsor","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.1048","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.1048","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Eight preschoolers with word-finding (WF) deficits and 16 controls with normal word-finding abilities (8 preschoolers and 8 adults) named 40 pictured objects under primed and unprimed conditions. Each picture could be correctly labeled with a simple noun or a compound (e.g., cane or walking stick). The primed condition involved semantic primes for both the simple and compound targets as well as a partial lexical prime for the compound targets. All participant groups decreased naming errors when given the primes. Two results indicated that the participants made use of the lexical primes. The first was a shift in form of correct responses from simple nouns in the unprimed condition to compound nouns in the primed condition. The second was an increase in errors that incorporated the lexical prime in the primed condition. There were limits to the benefit that the WF group derived from the primes. First, the primes did not enable the WF group to compensate fully for their naming problems. The gap between the error rates of the WF group and the control groups was not reduced in the primed condition. Second, the quality of errors made by the WF group did not improve in response to primes. Compared to the controls, the WF group made proportionately more errors that indicated no access to the target neighborhood (particularly \"I don't know\" responses) in the unprimed condition. With primes, the controls further reduced their use of these errors, but the WF group did not. When members of the control groups did make errors, they were more likely than the WF children to produce a word substitution that bore a close semantic, visual, or phonological relation to the target in both unprimed and primed conditions. These limitations on the benefit of priming for participants in the WF group suggest deficiencies in size, elaboration, or organization of their lexicons.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"1048-58"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.1048","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 37
Reliability and stability of various hearing-aid outcome measures in a group of elderly hearing-aid wearers. 在一组老年助听器佩戴者中各种助听器结果测量的可靠性和稳定性。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.923
L E Humes, D Halling, M Coughlin

Twenty elderly persons with hearing impairment were fit with binaural in-the-ear hearing aids and followed for a 6-month period post-fit. Several hearing-aid outcome measures were obtained at 0, 7, 15, 30, 60, 90, and 180 days post-fit. Outcome measures included (a) objective measures of benefit obtained with nonsense-syllable materials in quiet (CUNY Nonsense Syllable Test, NST) and sentences in multitalker babble (Hearing in Noise Test, HINT); (b) two subjective measures of benefit, one derived from pre-fit/post-fit comparisons on a general scale of hearing handicap (Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly, HHIE) and the other based on a subjective scale of post-fit hearing-aid benefit (Hearing Aid Performance Inventory, HAPI); (c) a questionnaire on hearing-aid satisfaction; (d) an objective measure of hearing-aid use; and (e) a subjective measure of hearing-aid use. Reliability and stability of each measure were examined through repeated-measures analyses of variance, a series of test-retest correlations, and, where possible, scatterplots of the scores against their corresponding 95% critical differences. Many of the measures were found to be both reliable and stable indicators of hearing-aid outcome.

对20例老年听力障碍患者配戴双耳耳内助听器,并随访6个月。在配戴后0、7、15、30、60、90和180天获得了几种助听器结果测量。结果测量包括(a)在安静环境下使用无音节材料(CUNY无音节测试,NST)和在多语者咿呀学语中使用句子(噪音听力测试,HINT)获得的效益的客观测量;(b)两种主观的效益衡量标准,一种是根据听力障碍的一般量表(老年人听力障碍量表,HHIE)进行验配前/验配后的比较,另一种是基于验配后助听器效益的主观量表(助听器性能量表,HAPI);(c)助听器满意度问卷;(d)助听器使用情况的客观衡量标准;(e)助听器使用的主观测量。每个测量的可靠性和稳定性通过重复测量的方差分析,一系列测试-再测试相关性,以及在可能的情况下,分数与相应的95%临界差异的散点图来检验。许多测量方法被发现是可靠和稳定的助听器效果指标。
{"title":"Reliability and stability of various hearing-aid outcome measures in a group of elderly hearing-aid wearers.","authors":"L E Humes,&nbsp;D Halling,&nbsp;M Coughlin","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.923","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.923","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Twenty elderly persons with hearing impairment were fit with binaural in-the-ear hearing aids and followed for a 6-month period post-fit. Several hearing-aid outcome measures were obtained at 0, 7, 15, 30, 60, 90, and 180 days post-fit. Outcome measures included (a) objective measures of benefit obtained with nonsense-syllable materials in quiet (CUNY Nonsense Syllable Test, NST) and sentences in multitalker babble (Hearing in Noise Test, HINT); (b) two subjective measures of benefit, one derived from pre-fit/post-fit comparisons on a general scale of hearing handicap (Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly, HHIE) and the other based on a subjective scale of post-fit hearing-aid benefit (Hearing Aid Performance Inventory, HAPI); (c) a questionnaire on hearing-aid satisfaction; (d) an objective measure of hearing-aid use; and (e) a subjective measure of hearing-aid use. Reliability and stability of each measure were examined through repeated-measures analyses of variance, a series of test-retest correlations, and, where possible, scatterplots of the scores against their corresponding 95% critical differences. Many of the measures were found to be both reliable and stable indicators of hearing-aid outcome.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"923-35"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.923","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 116
Electrophysiological indices of lexical processing: the effects of verb complexity and age. 词汇加工的电生理指标:动词复杂程度和年龄的影响。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.1071
S S Rubin, M Newhoff, R K Peach, L P Shapiro

To further investigate the effects that argument structure can have on language processing, reaction-time (RT) and event-related potential (ERP) data were collected for 14 younger subjects (M = 23 years) and 13 older subjects (M = 66 years). A cross-modal lexical decision (CMLD) task, involving online processing of high- and low-complexity verbs embedded in sentences, was used. Results of a baseline nonlinguistic visual ERP task indicated that the older group of subjects demonstrated significantly longer P300 latencies and significantly lower P300 amplitudes than the younger subjects. In the sentence task, younger subjects exhibited significantly higher P300 amplitudes when processing high- versus low-complexity verbs, with a similar pattern noted for the older subjects. P300 latencies were significantly shorter for the older group than the younger group. Neither P300 latencies nor RTs were significantly related to verb complexity, although medium to large effect sizes were present. Overall, these findings support earlier notions of argument structure effects.

为了进一步研究论点结构对语言处理的影响,我们收集了14名年轻被试(M = 23岁)和13名年长被试(M = 66岁)的反应时间(RT)和事件相关电位(ERP)数据。使用跨模态词汇决策(CMLD)任务,包括在线处理嵌入在句子中的高复杂性和低复杂性动词。一项基线非语言视觉ERP任务的结果表明,老年组受试者的P300潜伏期明显长于年轻组受试者,P300振幅明显低于年轻组受试者。在句子任务中,年轻的被试在处理复杂程度高的动词时比处理复杂程度低的动词时表现出明显更高的P300振幅,年龄较大的被试也有类似的模式。老年组的P300潜伏期明显短于年轻组。尽管存在中等到较大的效应量,但P300潜伏期和RTs都与动词复杂性无关。总的来说,这些发现支持了先前关于论点结构效应的观点。
{"title":"Electrophysiological indices of lexical processing: the effects of verb complexity and age.","authors":"S S Rubin,&nbsp;M Newhoff,&nbsp;R K Peach,&nbsp;L P Shapiro","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.1071","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.1071","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To further investigate the effects that argument structure can have on language processing, reaction-time (RT) and event-related potential (ERP) data were collected for 14 younger subjects (M = 23 years) and 13 older subjects (M = 66 years). A cross-modal lexical decision (CMLD) task, involving online processing of high- and low-complexity verbs embedded in sentences, was used. Results of a baseline nonlinguistic visual ERP task indicated that the older group of subjects demonstrated significantly longer P300 latencies and significantly lower P300 amplitudes than the younger subjects. In the sentence task, younger subjects exhibited significantly higher P300 amplitudes when processing high- versus low-complexity verbs, with a similar pattern noted for the older subjects. P300 latencies were significantly shorter for the older group than the younger group. Neither P300 latencies nor RTs were significantly related to verb complexity, although medium to large effect sizes were present. Overall, these findings support earlier notions of argument structure effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"1071-80"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.1071","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862879","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Effects of single-band syllabic amplitude compression on temporal speech information in nonsense syllables and in sentences. 单波段音节振幅压缩对无意义音节和句子时间语音信息的影响。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.912
D J Van Tasell, T D Trine

The effects of single-band amplitude compression on the use by subjects with normal hearing of temporal speech information were assessed using speech stimuli that had been processed to remove most spectral information before being compressed. The resulting signal-related-noise (SRN) stimuli isolated the effects of compression on the temporal information in speech by making it impossible for subjects to identify stimulus items on the basis of spectral speech information. Subjects with normal hearing listened to /aCa/ SRN disyllables that had been subjected to single-band compression at various combinations of compression ratio (CR) and time constants (TC). Performance was reduced only in the most severe compression condition (CR = 8; TC = 50), and then only slightly. Additional testing showed that subjects could use both periodicity and compression-overshoot artifactual information--in addition to envelope information--to identify the compressed /aCa/ stimuli. When a list of 10 context-controlled sentences was converted to SRN and compressed at CR = 8 and TC = 50, the ability of subjects with normal hearing to identify the sentences was significantly affected. Results established that (a) subjects with normal hearing differ widely in their abilities to use temporal information for speech identification, even after training; (b) subjects can learn to use both temporal envelope and periodicity information for identification if disyllables, even though; (c) subjects with normal hearing need envelope but not periodicity information to identify SRN sentences in a closed set. These results suggest that single-band compression at CR = 8 and TC = 50 would be undesirable for persons with limited ability to resolve speech spectral information. It is currently not known how less severe compression conditions would affect envelope information in sentences.

单波段振幅压缩对听力正常受试者使用时间语音信息的影响,使用在压缩前处理去除了大部分频谱信息的语音刺激进行评估。由此产生的信号相关噪声(SRN)刺激使受试者无法根据频谱语音信息识别刺激项目,从而隔离了压缩对语音时间信息的影响。听力正常的被试以不同的压缩比(CR)和时间常数(TC)组合收听经过单波段压缩的/aCa/ SRN双音节。仅在最严重的压缩条件下(CR = 8;TC = 50),然后只有一点点。额外的测试表明,除了包络信息外,受试者还可以使用周期性和压缩超调人工信息来识别压缩/aCa/刺激。在CR = 8和TC = 50时,将10个语境控制句子转换为SRN并压缩后,听力正常的被试识别句子的能力受到显著影响。结果表明:(a)即使经过训练,听力正常的受试者在使用时间信息进行语音识别的能力上也存在很大差异;(b)受试者可以学习使用时间包络和周期性信息来识别双音节,即使;(c)听力正常的受试者需要包络信息而不需要周期性信息来识别封闭集合中的SRN句子。这些结果表明,CR = 8和TC = 50的单波段压缩对于解析语音频谱信息能力有限的人来说是不可取的。目前尚不清楚不那么严重的压缩条件会如何影响句子中的包络信息。
{"title":"Effects of single-band syllabic amplitude compression on temporal speech information in nonsense syllables and in sentences.","authors":"D J Van Tasell,&nbsp;T D Trine","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.912","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.912","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The effects of single-band amplitude compression on the use by subjects with normal hearing of temporal speech information were assessed using speech stimuli that had been processed to remove most spectral information before being compressed. The resulting signal-related-noise (SRN) stimuli isolated the effects of compression on the temporal information in speech by making it impossible for subjects to identify stimulus items on the basis of spectral speech information. Subjects with normal hearing listened to /aCa/ SRN disyllables that had been subjected to single-band compression at various combinations of compression ratio (CR) and time constants (TC). Performance was reduced only in the most severe compression condition (CR = 8; TC = 50), and then only slightly. Additional testing showed that subjects could use both periodicity and compression-overshoot artifactual information--in addition to envelope information--to identify the compressed /aCa/ stimuli. When a list of 10 context-controlled sentences was converted to SRN and compressed at CR = 8 and TC = 50, the ability of subjects with normal hearing to identify the sentences was significantly affected. Results established that (a) subjects with normal hearing differ widely in their abilities to use temporal information for speech identification, even after training; (b) subjects can learn to use both temporal envelope and periodicity information for identification if disyllables, even though; (c) subjects with normal hearing need envelope but not periodicity information to identify SRN sentences in a closed set. These results suggest that single-band compression at CR = 8 and TC = 50 would be undesirable for persons with limited ability to resolve speech spectral information. It is currently not known how less severe compression conditions would affect envelope information in sentences.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"912-22"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 37
Phonological characteristics of children whose stuttering persisted or recovered. 口吃持续或恢复儿童的语音特征。
Pub Date : 1996-10-01 DOI: 10.1044/jshr.3905.981
E P Paden, E Yairi

Among children who stutter, more will be identified with articulation/phonological deficiencies than among normally fluent children of the same ages. Most current literature has focused on phonological differences between those children who stutter and those who do not. The present study examines early phonological differences between young children whose stuttering persisted and those who recovered from early stuttering. Thirty-six children, 12 whose stuttering persisted, 12 who recovered early, and 12 who recovered later, had been assessed by means of the Assessment of Phonological Processes--Revised (Hodson, 1986) soon after they were identified as exhibiting stuttering. After many months of longitudinal evaluation of their stuttering that led to their classification into the three groups, the early phonological assessments of these children were re-examined to identify differences. Overall mean percentage of error scores as well as error scores on specific phonological patterns showed that the persistent group differed significantly from normally fluent control subjects matched by age and sex. Scores of the two groups who recovered and their matched controls, however, did not differ significantly. Although poor phonological ability in the early stage of stuttering appears to be a contributing factor to the differentiation of persistence and recovery, the wide individual variations in scores within groups suggest that additional factors are necessary for reliable prediction.

在口吃的儿童中,与正常流利的同龄儿童相比,更多的儿童被认为有发音/语音缺陷。目前大多数文献都集中在口吃儿童和非口吃儿童的语音差异上。本研究调查了早期口吃持续的幼儿和从早期口吃中恢复的幼儿之间的语音差异。36名儿童,其中12名口吃持续,12名早期恢复,12名较晚恢复,在他们被确定为口吃后不久,通过语音过程评估-修订(Hodson, 1986)对他们进行了评估。在对他们的口吃进行了几个月的纵向评估后,他们被分为三组,这些孩子的早期语音评估被重新检查,以确定差异。总体平均错误分数百分比以及特定语音模式的错误分数表明,坚持组与按年龄和性别匹配的正常流利对照组有显著差异。然而,两组康复者和对照组的得分并没有显著差异。虽然早期口吃的语音能力差似乎是区分持续性和恢复的一个因素,但组内分数的广泛个体差异表明,可靠的预测需要其他因素。
{"title":"Phonological characteristics of children whose stuttering persisted or recovered.","authors":"E P Paden,&nbsp;E Yairi","doi":"10.1044/jshr.3905.981","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.981","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Among children who stutter, more will be identified with articulation/phonological deficiencies than among normally fluent children of the same ages. Most current literature has focused on phonological differences between those children who stutter and those who do not. The present study examines early phonological differences between young children whose stuttering persisted and those who recovered from early stuttering. Thirty-six children, 12 whose stuttering persisted, 12 who recovered early, and 12 who recovered later, had been assessed by means of the Assessment of Phonological Processes--Revised (Hodson, 1986) soon after they were identified as exhibiting stuttering. After many months of longitudinal evaluation of their stuttering that led to their classification into the three groups, the early phonological assessments of these children were re-examined to identify differences. Overall mean percentage of error scores as well as error scores on specific phonological patterns showed that the persistent group differed significantly from normally fluent control subjects matched by age and sex. Scores of the two groups who recovered and their matched controls, however, did not differ significantly. Although poor phonological ability in the early stage of stuttering appears to be a contributing factor to the differentiation of persistence and recovery, the wide individual variations in scores within groups suggest that additional factors are necessary for reliable prediction.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"981-90"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1044/jshr.3905.981","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862872","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 57
Treatment efficacy: cognitive-communicative disorders resulting from traumatic brain injury in adults. 治疗效果:成人外伤性脑损伤所致认知交际障碍。
C A Coelho, F DeRuyter, M Stein

Traumatic brain injuries (TBI) may result in a broad array of cognitive-communicative impairments. Cognitive-communicative impairments are the result of deficits in linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive functions. The speech-language pathologist functions as a member of the multidisciplinary team of professionals that collaboratively assess and treat individuals with TBI. The role of the speech-language pathologist includes assessment of all aspects of communication, as well as the communicative implications of cognitive deficits, and swallowing; treatment planning and programming, as determined by the individual's stage of recovery; client and family training/counseling; and interdisciplinary consultation. The effectiveness of speech and language intervention for specific cognitive deficits (e.g., attention, memory, executive functions) as well as general issues of social-skills training and early intervention are illustrated by scientific and clinical evidence from group-treatment and single-subject studies as well as case studies.

创伤性脑损伤(TBI)可导致广泛的认知交际障碍。认知交际障碍是语言和非语言认知功能缺失的结果。语言病理学家作为多学科专业团队的一员,共同评估和治疗TBI患者。语言病理学家的角色包括评估沟通的各个方面,以及认知缺陷和吞咽的沟通影响;治疗计划和规划,取决于个人的康复阶段;客户和家庭培训/咨询;以及跨学科的咨询。言语和语言干预对特定认知缺陷(例如,注意力、记忆、执行功能)的有效性,以及社交技能训练和早期干预的一般问题,都是通过群体治疗、单受试者研究以及案例研究的科学和临床证据来说明的。
{"title":"Treatment efficacy: cognitive-communicative disorders resulting from traumatic brain injury in adults.","authors":"C A Coelho,&nbsp;F DeRuyter,&nbsp;M Stein","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Traumatic brain injuries (TBI) may result in a broad array of cognitive-communicative impairments. Cognitive-communicative impairments are the result of deficits in linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive functions. The speech-language pathologist functions as a member of the multidisciplinary team of professionals that collaboratively assess and treat individuals with TBI. The role of the speech-language pathologist includes assessment of all aspects of communication, as well as the communicative implications of cognitive deficits, and swallowing; treatment planning and programming, as determined by the individual's stage of recovery; client and family training/counseling; and interdisciplinary consultation. The effectiveness of speech and language intervention for specific cognitive deficits (e.g., attention, memory, executive functions) as well as general issues of social-skills training and early intervention are illustrated by scientific and clinical evidence from group-treatment and single-subject studies as well as case studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":76022,"journal":{"name":"Journal of speech and hearing research","volume":"39 5","pages":"S5-17"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1996-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"19862769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of speech and hearing research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1