首页 > 最新文献

Animal Learning & Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Concurrent-chain performance in transition: effects of terminal-link duration and individual reinforcers. 过渡中的并发链绩效:终端链持续时间和个体强化因素的影响。
Pub Date : 2002-08-01 DOI: 10.3758/bf03192834
James E Mazur

Pigeons responded on concurrent-chain schedules with variable-interval initial links and equal delays as terminal links. The terminal-link delays were 1 sec in some conditions and 20 sec in other conditions. The percentages of reinforcers delivered for responses on the left key were 10%, 30%, 70%, or 90%, and this percentage was switched every five to nine sessions. The rate of change in the pigeons' response percentages after a switch was the same whether the terminal-link delays were 1 sec or 20 sec. Analysis of the effects of individual reinforcers showed that after a response on one key had been reinforced, response percentages on that key were higher for at least the next 100 responses. Small effects of individual reinforcers were evident after eight or nine additional reinforcers had been delivered. The effects of individual reinforcers were about equally large during times of transition and during periods in which overall response percentages were relatively stable.

鸽子以可变间隔的初始链路和相等延迟的终端链路响应并发链调度。终端链路延迟在某些情况下为1秒,在其他情况下为20秒。在左键上传递的强化物的百分比分别为10%,30%,70%或90%,并且这个百分比每5到9个会话切换一次。无论终端连接延迟是1秒还是20秒,转换后鸽子的反应百分比的变化率是相同的。对单个强化因素影响的分析表明,在对一个键的反应得到强化后,至少在接下来的100个反应中,对该键的反应百分比更高。个别强化物的小影响是明显的,在8或9个额外的强化物已交付。在过渡时期和总体反应百分比相对稳定的时期,个体强化因素的影响大致相同。
{"title":"Concurrent-chain performance in transition: effects of terminal-link duration and individual reinforcers.","authors":"James E Mazur","doi":"10.3758/bf03192834","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192834","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pigeons responded on concurrent-chain schedules with variable-interval initial links and equal delays as terminal links. The terminal-link delays were 1 sec in some conditions and 20 sec in other conditions. The percentages of reinforcers delivered for responses on the left key were 10%, 30%, 70%, or 90%, and this percentage was switched every five to nine sessions. The rate of change in the pigeons' response percentages after a switch was the same whether the terminal-link delays were 1 sec or 20 sec. Analysis of the effects of individual reinforcers showed that after a response on one key had been reinforced, response percentages on that key were higher for at least the next 100 responses. Small effects of individual reinforcers were evident after eight or nine additional reinforcers had been delivered. The effects of individual reinforcers were about equally large during times of transition and during periods in which overall response percentages were relatively stable.</p>","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"30 3","pages":"249-60"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3758/bf03192834","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22076230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Research productivity in animal learning from 1953 to 2000. 从1953年到2000年动物学习的研究生产力。
Pub Date : 2002-08-01 DOI: 10.3758/bf03192837
Michael Domjan, Mark A Krause

Searches conducted with Medline and PsycInfo showed that the number of publications dealing with learning in animals increased between 1975 and 2000 and that the increase was substantially greater in Medline than in PsycInfo. An examination of major journals dealing with behavioral studies of conditioning and learning for the years 1953, 1963, 1973, 1983, 1993, and 2000 revealed a different pattern of results. The number of papers published in these journals increased from 1953 to 1973 but has been declining steadily since then. However, this decline was partially offset by an increase in the number of experiments published in each paper. Substantially more experiments were published in 2000 than in 1963 or 1953. The number of core authors in the field also peaked in 1973 and has been declining since. However, there were only seven fewer core authors in 2000 than in 1983, and 1983 had as many core authors as 1963. These data suggest that pessimism about the status of behavioral studies of learning is not warranted if research activity is considered over a 40-year period. Furthermore, increased interest in the neural and biological mechanisms of learning should bode well for the status of behavioral research, because one cannot examine the physiological mechanisms of a behavioral process without first clearly understanding the phenomenon at the behavioral level.

在Medline和PsycInfo上进行的搜索显示,1975年至2000年间,有关动物学习的出版物数量有所增加,而且Medline上的增幅明显大于PsycInfo。对1953年、1963年、1973年、1983年、1993年和2000年有关条件反射和学习行为研究的主要期刊的研究揭示了一种不同的结果模式。从1953年到1973年,在这些期刊上发表的论文数量有所增加,但此后一直在稳步下降。然而,每篇论文中发表的实验数量的增加部分抵消了这一下降。2000年发表的实验比1963年或1953年多得多。该领域核心作者的数量也在1973年达到顶峰,此后一直在下降。然而,2000年的核心作者只比1983年少了7名,1983年的核心作者数量与1963年一样多。这些数据表明,如果研究活动考虑超过40年的时间,对学习行为研究现状的悲观态度是没有根据的。此外,对学习的神经和生物学机制的兴趣增加,对行为研究的现状来说应该是一个好兆头,因为如果不首先清楚地了解行为层面的现象,就无法检查行为过程的生理机制。
{"title":"Research productivity in animal learning from 1953 to 2000.","authors":"Michael Domjan,&nbsp;Mark A Krause","doi":"10.3758/bf03192837","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192837","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Searches conducted with Medline and PsycInfo showed that the number of publications dealing with learning in animals increased between 1975 and 2000 and that the increase was substantially greater in Medline than in PsycInfo. An examination of major journals dealing with behavioral studies of conditioning and learning for the years 1953, 1963, 1973, 1983, 1993, and 2000 revealed a different pattern of results. The number of papers published in these journals increased from 1953 to 1973 but has been declining steadily since then. However, this decline was partially offset by an increase in the number of experiments published in each paper. Substantially more experiments were published in 2000 than in 1963 or 1953. The number of core authors in the field also peaked in 1973 and has been declining since. However, there were only seven fewer core authors in 2000 than in 1983, and 1983 had as many core authors as 1963. These data suggest that pessimism about the status of behavioral studies of learning is not warranted if research activity is considered over a 40-year period. Furthermore, increased interest in the neural and biological mechanisms of learning should bode well for the status of behavioral research, because one cannot examine the physiological mechanisms of a behavioral process without first clearly understanding the phenomenon at the behavioral level.</p>","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"30 3","pages":"282-5"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3758/bf03192837","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22076702","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Imitative learning in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the bidirectional control procedure. 用双向控制程序研究日本鹌鹑的模仿学习。
Pub Date : 2002-08-01 DOI: 10.3758/bf03192836
Chana K Akins, Emily D Klein, Thomas R Zentall

In the bidirectional control procedure, observers are exposed to a conspecific demonstrator responding to a manipulandum in one of two directions (e.g., left vs. right). This procedure controls for socially mediated effects (the mere presence of a conspecific) and stimulus enhancement (attention drawn to a manipulandum by its movement), and it has the added advantage of being symmetrical (the two different responses are similar in topography). Imitative learning is demonstrated when the observers make the response in the direction that they observed it being made. Recently, however, it has been suggested that when such evidence is found with a predominantly olfactory animal, such as the rat, it may result artifactually from odor cues left on one side of the manipulandum by the demonstrator. In the present experiment, we found that Japanese quail, for which odor cues are not likely to play a role, also showed significant correspondence between the direction in which the demonstrator and the observer push a screen to gain access to reward. Furthermore, control quail that observed the screen move, when the movement of the screen was not produced by the demonstrator, did not show similar correspondence between the direction of screen movement observed and that performed by the observer. Thus, with the appropriate control, the bidirectional procedure appears to be useful for studying imitation in avian species.

在双向控制过程中,观察者接触到一个在两个方向(例如,左或右)中的一个方向上响应操纵器的同一演示者。这个过程控制了社会中介效应(同物的存在)和刺激增强(被操纵物的运动引起的注意),并且它具有对称的额外优势(两种不同的反应在地形上相似)。当观察者按照他们观察到的方向做出反应时,模仿学习就被证明了。然而,最近有人提出,当这种证据是在嗅觉占优势的动物(如老鼠)身上发现时,它可能是由演示者在操纵器一侧留下的气味线索人为地产生的。在本实验中,我们发现日本鹌鹑在气味线索不太可能发挥作用的情况下,也表现出演示者和观察者推动屏幕以获得奖励的方向之间的显著对应关系。此外,观察屏幕移动的对照组鹌鹑,当屏幕的移动不是由演示者产生的,在观察到的屏幕移动方向和观察者执行的方向之间没有类似的对应关系。因此,在适当的控制下,双向程序似乎对研究鸟类的模仿是有用的。
{"title":"Imitative learning in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the bidirectional control procedure.","authors":"Chana K Akins,&nbsp;Emily D Klein,&nbsp;Thomas R Zentall","doi":"10.3758/bf03192836","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192836","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the bidirectional control procedure, observers are exposed to a conspecific demonstrator responding to a manipulandum in one of two directions (e.g., left vs. right). This procedure controls for socially mediated effects (the mere presence of a conspecific) and stimulus enhancement (attention drawn to a manipulandum by its movement), and it has the added advantage of being symmetrical (the two different responses are similar in topography). Imitative learning is demonstrated when the observers make the response in the direction that they observed it being made. Recently, however, it has been suggested that when such evidence is found with a predominantly olfactory animal, such as the rat, it may result artifactually from odor cues left on one side of the manipulandum by the demonstrator. In the present experiment, we found that Japanese quail, for which odor cues are not likely to play a role, also showed significant correspondence between the direction in which the demonstrator and the observer push a screen to gain access to reward. Furthermore, control quail that observed the screen move, when the movement of the screen was not produced by the demonstrator, did not show similar correspondence between the direction of screen movement observed and that performed by the observer. Thus, with the appropriate control, the bidirectional procedure appears to be useful for studying imitation in avian species.</p>","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"30 3","pages":"275-81"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.3758/bf03192836","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"22076232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 56
Extinction revisited: Similarities between extinction and reductions in US intensity in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response 灭绝重访:在兔的闪膜反应的经典条件反射中,灭绝和US强度减少之间的相似性
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192912
E. Kehoe, Natasha E. White
{"title":"Extinction revisited: Similarities between extinction and reductions in US intensity in classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response","authors":"E. Kehoe, Natasha E. White","doi":"10.3758/BF03192912","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192912","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"39 1","pages":"96-111"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73427320","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 46
Target-defining features in a “people-present/people-absent” discrimination task by pigeons 鸽子“人在/人不在”判别任务中的目标定义特征
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192918
Ulrike Aust, L. Huber
{"title":"Target-defining features in a “people-present/people-absent” discrimination task by pigeons","authors":"Ulrike Aust, L. Huber","doi":"10.3758/BF03192918","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192918","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"29 1","pages":"165-176"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81407036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 42
Enumeration of briefly presented items by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens) 黑猩猩(类人猿)和人类(智人)简要列举的物品
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192916
M. Tomonaga, T. Matsuzawa
{"title":"Enumeration of briefly presented items by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens)","authors":"M. Tomonaga, T. Matsuzawa","doi":"10.3758/BF03192916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192916","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"1 1","pages":"143-157"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79821334","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 76
Evaluation and development of a connectionist theory of configural learning 构形学习的联结主义理论的评价与发展
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192911
J. Pearce
{"title":"Evaluation and development of a connectionist theory of configural learning","authors":"J. Pearce","doi":"10.3758/BF03192911","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192911","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"45 1","pages":"73-95"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84905784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 221
An empirical analysis of the super-latent inhibition effect 超潜伏抑制效应的实证分析
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192913
L. G. Casa, R. Lubow
{"title":"An empirical analysis of the super-latent inhibition effect","authors":"L. G. Casa, R. Lubow","doi":"10.3758/BF03192913","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192913","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"210 1","pages":"112-120"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89040637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 51
Integration of cardiac responses to serial stimuli after Pavlovian conditioning in rats 巴甫洛夫条件反射后大鼠心脏对一系列刺激反应的整合
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192915
A. Marchand
{"title":"Integration of cardiac responses to serial stimuli after Pavlovian conditioning in rats","authors":"A. Marchand","doi":"10.3758/BF03192915","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192915","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"164 1","pages":"132-142"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79816131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Monkey auditory list memory: tests with mixed and blocked retention delays 猴子听觉列表记忆:混合和阻塞保留延迟的测试
Pub Date : 2002-05-01 DOI: 10.3758/BF03192917
A. Wright
{"title":"Monkey auditory list memory: tests with mixed and blocked retention delays","authors":"A. Wright","doi":"10.3758/BF03192917","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192917","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7824,"journal":{"name":"Animal Learning & Behavior","volume":"34 1","pages":"158-164"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2002-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79045694","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
期刊
Animal Learning & Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1