首页 > 最新文献

Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie最新文献

英文 中文
Understanding and valuing environmental issues: the effects of availability and anchoring on judgment. 理解和评价环境问题:可得性和锚定对判断的影响。
J van der Pligt, E C van Schie, R Hoevenagel

This paper addresses the effects of availability and anchoring-and-adjustment on people's beliefs and values concerning environmental issues. The first three studies focus on lay people's perceptions of the causes of large scale environmental risks, the second series of three studies deals with how people value environmental goods and how much they are prepared to pay to mitigate environmental risks. In studies 1-3 we investigate the effects of availability and anchoring-and-adjustment on estimating the contribution of various factors to large scale environmental risks. Highly complex risks such as acid rain and global warming tend to be associated with multiple causes, and our results show that estimating the role of these causes is clearly affected by availability and anchoring-and-adjustment. Both have sizeable effects and persist over time. Moreover, corrective procedures only seem to have a limited effect. Availability and anchoring-and-adjustment not only play a role in judging the possible causes of risks; they also play a role in research attempting to assess the public's willingness to pay (WTP) to protect our environment. The outcomes of WTP surveys are often used as a tool to help policy decision making. In the second part of this article we present three studies on this issue. Results provide further evidence of the impact of the two heuristics on the outcomes of WTP research. Implications for research and practice are briefly discussed.

本文探讨了可得性和锚定与调整对人们关于环境问题的信念和价值观的影响。前三个研究集中在外行人对大规模环境风险的原因的看法,第二个系列的三个研究涉及人们如何评估环境产品以及他们准备支付多少来减轻环境风险。在研究1-3中,我们研究了可用性和锚定调整对估计各种因素对大尺度环境风险的贡献的影响。高度复杂的风险,如酸雨和全球变暖往往与多个原因有关,我们的研究结果表明,这些原因的作用估计明显受到可用性和锚定和调整的影响。两者都有相当大的影响,并会持续一段时间。此外,矫正程序似乎只有有限的效果。可得性和锚定调整不仅在判断风险的可能原因方面起作用;它们也在试图评估公众为保护我们的环境而支付意愿的研究中发挥作用。WTP调查的结果经常被用作帮助政策制定的工具。在本文的第二部分,我们对这一问题进行了三个方面的研究。研究结果进一步证明了两种启发式方法对WTP研究结果的影响。简要讨论了对研究和实践的启示。
{"title":"Understanding and valuing environmental issues: the effects of availability and anchoring on judgment.","authors":"J van der Pligt,&nbsp;E C van Schie,&nbsp;R Hoevenagel","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This paper addresses the effects of availability and anchoring-and-adjustment on people's beliefs and values concerning environmental issues. The first three studies focus on lay people's perceptions of the causes of large scale environmental risks, the second series of three studies deals with how people value environmental goods and how much they are prepared to pay to mitigate environmental risks. In studies 1-3 we investigate the effects of availability and anchoring-and-adjustment on estimating the contribution of various factors to large scale environmental risks. Highly complex risks such as acid rain and global warming tend to be associated with multiple causes, and our results show that estimating the role of these causes is clearly affected by availability and anchoring-and-adjustment. Both have sizeable effects and persist over time. Moreover, corrective procedures only seem to have a limited effect. Availability and anchoring-and-adjustment not only play a role in judging the possible causes of risks; they also play a role in research attempting to assess the public's willingness to pay (WTP) to protect our environment. The outcomes of WTP surveys are often used as a tool to help policy decision making. In the second part of this article we present three studies on this issue. Results provide further evidence of the impact of the two heuristics on the outcomes of WTP research. Implications for research and practice are briefly discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"45 4","pages":"286-302"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1998-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20766292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[A fair test of the spreading activation hypothesis: a study of affective congruency effects in the Stroop test]. [扩散激活假说的公平检验:Stroop测验中情感一致性效应的研究]。
K Rothermund, D Wentura

It is argued that the Stroop color-naming task is especially suited to investigate affective priming effects in the sense of an automatic spreading of activation to other concepts of the same valence, because (a) the Stroop task is not prone to an explanation of affective congruency effects on the basis of reaction priming or reaction interference, and (b) it is possible to detect specific (fast and efficient stimulus processing due to heightened accessibility) as well as nonspecific (cognitive interference, triggering of global action tendencies) effects of an activation of valenced concepts in the Stroop task. Two experiments were conducted to investigate associative and affective priming effects with the Stroop task. In a first experiment (N = 36, SOA = 300 ms) a standard priming procedure was chosen; the primes were presented without any processing instructions. In a second experiment (N = 48, SOA = 500ms) the primes had to be reproduced after naming the color of the target. In both experiments significant association effects were found for the associative material. For the valenced material no affective congruency effects were found in either experiment. The present results are not compatible with the hypothesis of an automatic affective spreading of activation that was given as an explanation of affective congruency effects in previous studies using different tasks.

有人认为,Stroop颜色命名任务特别适合于在激活自动扩散到其他相同效价概念的意义上研究情感启动效应,因为(a) Stroop任务不倾向于在反应启动或反应干扰的基础上解释情感一致性效应;(b)在Stroop任务中,我们可以检测到特定的(由于可及性的提高而产生的快速和有效的刺激处理)以及非特定的(认知干扰,触发整体行动倾向)对激活价值概念的影响。通过两个实验研究了Stroop任务的联想启动效应和情感启动效应。在第一个实验中(N = 36, SOA = 300 ms),选择一个标准启动程序;这些质数没有任何处理说明。在第二个实验中(N = 48, SOA = 500毫秒),必须在指定目标的颜色后重现质数。在两个实验中都发现了显著的联想效应。在两种实验中均未发现有情感一致性的材料。本研究结果与以往研究中使用不同任务来解释情感一致性效应的自动情感传播假说不一致。
{"title":"[A fair test of the spreading activation hypothesis: a study of affective congruency effects in the Stroop test].","authors":"K Rothermund,&nbsp;D Wentura","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is argued that the Stroop color-naming task is especially suited to investigate affective priming effects in the sense of an automatic spreading of activation to other concepts of the same valence, because (a) the Stroop task is not prone to an explanation of affective congruency effects on the basis of reaction priming or reaction interference, and (b) it is possible to detect specific (fast and efficient stimulus processing due to heightened accessibility) as well as nonspecific (cognitive interference, triggering of global action tendencies) effects of an activation of valenced concepts in the Stroop task. Two experiments were conducted to investigate associative and affective priming effects with the Stroop task. In a first experiment (N = 36, SOA = 300 ms) a standard priming procedure was chosen; the primes were presented without any processing instructions. In a second experiment (N = 48, SOA = 500ms) the primes had to be reproduced after naming the color of the target. In both experiments significant association effects were found for the associative material. For the valenced material no affective congruency effects were found in either experiment. The present results are not compatible with the hypothesis of an automatic affective spreading of activation that was given as an explanation of affective congruency effects in previous studies using different tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"45 2","pages":"120-35"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1998-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20594111","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Psychological aspects of environmental risks]. [环境风险的心理方面]。
G Böhm, J Rost, H Spada
{"title":"[Psychological aspects of environmental risks].","authors":"G Böhm,&nbsp;J Rost,&nbsp;H Spada","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"45 4","pages":"243-50"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1998-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20766289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[The pattern of variables predicting self-reported environmental behavior]. [预测自我报告的环境行为的变量模式]。
O Kannapin, K Pawlik, F Zinn

Many of the changes observed in our environment today may be traced back to human action. In addition to a descriptive elaboration of possible predictor variables, the psychological analysis of environmental behavior is directed towards the identification of key variables and the structural relationships among these and behavior. Variables that are suitable to predict environmental behavior need to be identified. In this study, 215 subjects each were drawn from an urban and a rural sample; 85 further subjects were considered to be highly environmentally engaged. Thus the total sample population for this study included 515 subjects. Scales on acquisition of information, values, locus of control, attribution of responsibility, and environmental threat were administered to all subjects. A modified version of the protection-motivation theory formulated by Gardner and Stern (1996) served as a reference model. Simultaneous regression analysis revealed that scales specifically directed to the domain of environmental behavior are well suited to explain environmental actions, especially in the subset of highly engaged persons (R2 = .58). In contrast to both other groups, the acquisition of environmentally specific information was a strong predictor in this group. On the basis of these regression analyses, it is argued that additional predictors--along with the ones used in this study--must be taken into account in groups that do not display extraordinary engagement in environmental matters.

今天在我们的环境中观察到的许多变化可以追溯到人类的活动。除了对可能的预测变量进行描述性阐述外,环境行为的心理分析还旨在确定关键变量以及这些变量与行为之间的结构关系。需要确定适合预测环境行为的变量。在这项研究中,分别从城市和农村抽取了215名受试者;另外85个科目被认为与环境密切相关。因此,本研究的总样本人口包括515名受试者。对所有被试进行信息获取、价值观、控制点、责任归因和环境威胁的量表。加德纳和斯特恩(Gardner and Stern, 1996)提出的一种修正版的保护动机理论作为参考模型。同时回归分析显示,专门针对环境行为领域的尺度非常适合解释环境行动,特别是在高度投入的人群中(R2 = .58)。与其他两组相比,在这一组中,环境特定信息的获取是一个强有力的预测因素。在这些回归分析的基础上,有人认为,在对环境问题没有特别参与的群体中,必须考虑到其他预测因素——以及本研究中使用的预测因素。
{"title":"[The pattern of variables predicting self-reported environmental behavior].","authors":"O Kannapin,&nbsp;K Pawlik,&nbsp;F Zinn","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many of the changes observed in our environment today may be traced back to human action. In addition to a descriptive elaboration of possible predictor variables, the psychological analysis of environmental behavior is directed towards the identification of key variables and the structural relationships among these and behavior. Variables that are suitable to predict environmental behavior need to be identified. In this study, 215 subjects each were drawn from an urban and a rural sample; 85 further subjects were considered to be highly environmentally engaged. Thus the total sample population for this study included 515 subjects. Scales on acquisition of information, values, locus of control, attribution of responsibility, and environmental threat were administered to all subjects. A modified version of the protection-motivation theory formulated by Gardner and Stern (1996) served as a reference model. Simultaneous regression analysis revealed that scales specifically directed to the domain of environmental behavior are well suited to explain environmental actions, especially in the subset of highly engaged persons (R2 = .58). In contrast to both other groups, the acquisition of environmentally specific information was a strong predictor in this group. On the basis of these regression analyses, it is argued that additional predictors--along with the ones used in this study--must be taken into account in groups that do not display extraordinary engagement in environmental matters.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"45 4","pages":"365-77"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1998-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20767397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Negative priming designs: comparison and improvement of measurements for detection of cognitive inhibition]. [负启动设计:认知抑制检测测量的比较和改进]。
P Schmuck, R Bloem

Ignoring a distractor on a prime trial generally impairs responses to that stimulus on a subsequent probe trial. This so called negative-priming (NP) effect supports the view that the representations of distracting stimuli underly a cognitive inhibition during target selection (Tipper, 1985). Aiming at the optimization of NP designs for analyzing cognitive inhibition the present paper investigates two open questions within this context: Which classical designs using words, letters, or pictures as stimuli are most accepted by subjects and at the same time result in possibly strong NP effects? What is the time course of NP, and at which interstimulus interval (ISI) the maximum is reached? In experiment 1, three "classical" NP designs were compared within one sample of subjects and a picture design was selected as the most suitable one. In experiment 2 the ISI was varied within subjects; the maximal NP effect was found at an ISI of around 1000 ms.

在启动试验中忽略一个干扰因素,通常会削弱后续探测试验中对该刺激的反应。这种所谓的负启动效应(NP)支持了这样一种观点,即在目标选择过程中,分散注意力的刺激表征是认知抑制的基础(Tipper, 1985)。为了优化NP设计以分析认知抑制,本文探讨了在此背景下的两个开放性问题:哪些使用单词、字母或图片作为刺激的经典设计最容易被受试者接受,同时可能产生强烈的NP效应?NP的时间过程是怎样的?在哪个间刺激间隔(ISI)达到最大值?在实验1中,在一个被试样本中比较了三种“经典”NP设计,并选择了一种图片设计作为最合适的设计。在实验2中,被试的ISI是不同的;在ISI约1000 ms时发现最大的NP效应。
{"title":"[Negative priming designs: comparison and improvement of measurements for detection of cognitive inhibition].","authors":"P Schmuck,&nbsp;R Bloem","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ignoring a distractor on a prime trial generally impairs responses to that stimulus on a subsequent probe trial. This so called negative-priming (NP) effect supports the view that the representations of distracting stimuli underly a cognitive inhibition during target selection (Tipper, 1985). Aiming at the optimization of NP designs for analyzing cognitive inhibition the present paper investigates two open questions within this context: Which classical designs using words, letters, or pictures as stimuli are most accepted by subjects and at the same time result in possibly strong NP effects? What is the time course of NP, and at which interstimulus interval (ISI) the maximum is reached? In experiment 1, three \"classical\" NP designs were compared within one sample of subjects and a picture design was selected as the most suitable one. In experiment 2 the ISI was varied within subjects; the maximal NP effect was found at an ISI of around 1000 ms.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"45 1","pages":"60-71"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1998-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20577495","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Possibilities of computer-assisted administration of the revised Freiburg Personality Inventory. 2 experimental studies]. [计算机辅助管理修订弗莱堡人格量表的可能性。2项实验研究]。
G H Franke

Empirical results of two studies on the practice of computer administration of psychodiagnostic inventories are presented. Based on the "Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar" (FPI-R, a German multidimensional personality inventory, Fahrenberg, Hampel & Selg, 1994) two studies (total N = 600 university students) were conducted in which (1) the standard paper-pencil questionnaire was compared with a computer administrated version, and (2) the standard item arrangement was contrasted with the presentation of the items of each subscale as a block (homogeneous item blocking), which corresponds to the extraction of subscales from more extensive inventories. The results confirm low significant effects of computer administration and of item blocking as to the means, as well as no interactions with the factor "gender." The parameters of reliability increased partially under computer administration. Computer administration itself led to a higher degree of self-consciousness. The adequacy of the FPI-R, which was standardized by paper-pencil inquest, and a computer administrated version is thus demonstrated proven.

本文介绍了两项关于心理诊断量表计算机管理实践的实证结果。基于“Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar”(FPI-R,德国多维人格量表,Fahrenberg, Hampel & Selg, 1994)进行了两项研究(共N = 600名大学生),其中(1)将标准纸笔问卷与计算机管理版本进行比较,(2)将标准项目安排与每个子量表的项目作为一个块(同质项目块)进行比较。这对应于从更广泛的清单中提取子量表。结果证实了计算机管理和项目封锁对手段的低显著影响,以及与因素“性别”没有相互作用。在计算机管理下,可靠性参数部分增加。计算机管理本身导致了更高程度的自我意识。通过纸笔调查标准化的FPI-R和计算机管理的版本的充分性因此得到证明。
{"title":"[Possibilities of computer-assisted administration of the revised Freiburg Personality Inventory. 2 experimental studies].","authors":"G H Franke","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Empirical results of two studies on the practice of computer administration of psychodiagnostic inventories are presented. Based on the \"Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar\" (FPI-R, a German multidimensional personality inventory, Fahrenberg, Hampel & Selg, 1994) two studies (total N = 600 university students) were conducted in which (1) the standard paper-pencil questionnaire was compared with a computer administrated version, and (2) the standard item arrangement was contrasted with the presentation of the items of each subscale as a block (homogeneous item blocking), which corresponds to the extraction of subscales from more extensive inventories. The results confirm low significant effects of computer administration and of item blocking as to the means, as well as no interactions with the factor \"gender.\" The parameters of reliability increased partially under computer administration. Computer administration itself led to a higher degree of self-consciousness. The adequacy of the FPI-R, which was standardized by paper-pencil inquest, and a computer administrated version is thus demonstrated proven.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"44 2","pages":"332-56"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1997-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20412164","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[llocalization of grouping effect in overlapping tasks]. [重叠任务中分组效应的局部化]。
W Sommer, H Leuthold, R Abdel-Rahman, E M Pfütze

In certain kinds of overlapping tasks subjects have to respond to two stimuli (S1 and S2), presented in close succession with variable onset asynchrony (SOA), carrying out two different responses (R1 and R2). When responding to S1 is given priority over S2 processing, reaction time (RT) to the first stimulus is unaffected by SOA. However, sometimes subjects adopt, either spontaneously or because of instruction, a different processing strategy called grouping. In this case R1 is postponed until R2 has been determined too. The present study addresses the localization of the postponed processing stage by means of the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). One group of subjects was instructed to group responses and a second group was to give priority to S1 processing. The adherence to instructions was verified by subjects' reaction times. In case of grouping the interval between LRP onset and overt reaction increased proportionally to the postponement caused by SOA. In contrast, the interval between stimulus and LRP onset was unaffected by SOA. These findings speak against assumptions that grouping causes response activation postponement; rather, the corresponding reaction is already selected and activated prior to postponement.

在某些类型的重叠任务中,被试必须对两个刺激(S1和S2)作出反应,它们以可变启动异步(SOA)紧密连续呈现,并执行两种不同的反应(R1和R2)。当对S1的响应优先于S2处理时,对第一个刺激的反应时间(RT)不受SOA的影响。然而,有时受试者自发地或由于指示而采用一种不同的处理策略,称为分组。在这种情况下,R1被推迟到R2也被确定。本研究利用侧化准备电位(LRP)研究延迟加工阶段的定位。一组受试者被要求对反应进行分组,另一组受试者被要求优先处理S1。受试者的反应时间证实了他们对指示的遵守程度。在分组的情况下,LRP开始和公开反应之间的间隔与SOA引起的延迟成比例地增加。相比之下,刺激和LRP开始之间的间隔不受SOA的影响。这些发现反驳了分组导致反应激活延迟的假设;相反,相应的反应在推迟之前就已经被选择和激活了。
{"title":"[llocalization of grouping effect in overlapping tasks].","authors":"W Sommer,&nbsp;H Leuthold,&nbsp;R Abdel-Rahman,&nbsp;E M Pfütze","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In certain kinds of overlapping tasks subjects have to respond to two stimuli (S1 and S2), presented in close succession with variable onset asynchrony (SOA), carrying out two different responses (R1 and R2). When responding to S1 is given priority over S2 processing, reaction time (RT) to the first stimulus is unaffected by SOA. However, sometimes subjects adopt, either spontaneously or because of instruction, a different processing strategy called grouping. In this case R1 is postponed until R2 has been determined too. The present study addresses the localization of the postponed processing stage by means of the lateralized readiness potential (LRP). One group of subjects was instructed to group responses and a second group was to give priority to S1 processing. The adherence to instructions was verified by subjects' reaction times. In case of grouping the interval between LRP onset and overt reaction increased proportionally to the postponement caused by SOA. In contrast, the interval between stimulus and LRP onset was unaffected by SOA. These findings speak against assumptions that grouping causes response activation postponement; rather, the corresponding reaction is already selected and activated prior to postponement.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"44 1","pages":"103-16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1997-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20422396","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Effect of preload of working memory on components of evoked potentials during encoding of sequentially presented letter sequences]. [工作记忆预负荷对顺序呈现字母序列编码诱发电位成分的影响]。
G Kusak, K Grune, H Hagendorf, A M Metz

In recent experiments with a pseudo-random sequence of 7 consonants to recall after trial (stimulus onset asynchrony 1000 ms) a reverse relationship between P300 amplitude of the event-related potential (ERP) and letter presentation position was shown (Grune et al., 1996). It was assumed that this relationship reflects competition between encoding and retention processes in this time regime. In order to find evidence for the resource competition hypothesis in working memory a 4 x 4 grid containing 4 digits preceded a sequence of 6 consonants. In the first block subjects were instructed to ignore the grid, in two remaining blocks they were asked to remember the digits or their position in the grid and to recognize them after the letter recall. In the ignore condition the expected effect of P300 decrease with letter position was found, especially at posterior electrode sites. When subjects had to process the preceding digit grid there was no position effect of P300 caused by small P300 amplitudes in the ERPs that were elicited by the first letters of the sequence. This effect was not specific to the modality of the interfering with the letter task information from the digit grid. We conclude that the grid information occupies working memory resources that are not available for the event-related processing of consonants to recall.

在最近的实验中,7个辅音的伪随机序列在试验后(刺激开始异步1000毫秒)被试回忆,结果显示事件相关电位(ERP)的P300振幅与字母呈现位置之间存在反向关系(Grune et al., 1996)。假设这种关系反映了编码和保留过程在这个时间范围内的竞争。为了寻找工作记忆中资源竞争假说的证据,一个4 x 4的网格包含4个数字,前面是6个辅音序列。在第一个模块中,受试者被要求忽略网格,在剩下的两个模块中,他们被要求记住数字或它们在网格中的位置,并在回忆字母后认出它们。在忽略条件下,P300随字母位置的降低有预期效果,尤其是在电极后部。当受试者必须处理前面的数字网格时,序列的第一个字母引发的erp中P300的小振幅没有引起P300的位置效应。这种影响并不特定于干扰来自数字网格的字母任务信息的方式。我们得出结论,网格信息占用了工作记忆资源,而这些工作记忆资源是不能用于辅音事件相关加工的。
{"title":"[Effect of preload of working memory on components of evoked potentials during encoding of sequentially presented letter sequences].","authors":"G Kusak,&nbsp;K Grune,&nbsp;H Hagendorf,&nbsp;A M Metz","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In recent experiments with a pseudo-random sequence of 7 consonants to recall after trial (stimulus onset asynchrony 1000 ms) a reverse relationship between P300 amplitude of the event-related potential (ERP) and letter presentation position was shown (Grune et al., 1996). It was assumed that this relationship reflects competition between encoding and retention processes in this time regime. In order to find evidence for the resource competition hypothesis in working memory a 4 x 4 grid containing 4 digits preceded a sequence of 6 consonants. In the first block subjects were instructed to ignore the grid, in two remaining blocks they were asked to remember the digits or their position in the grid and to recognize them after the letter recall. In the ignore condition the expected effect of P300 decrease with letter position was found, especially at posterior electrode sites. When subjects had to process the preceding digit grid there was no position effect of P300 caused by small P300 amplitudes in the ERPs that were elicited by the first letters of the sequence. This effect was not specific to the modality of the interfering with the letter task information from the digit grid. We conclude that the grid information occupies working memory resources that are not available for the event-related processing of consonants to recall.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"44 1","pages":"38-61"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1997-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20422929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Event related potentials and emotional pictures:effect of stimulus presentation time]. 事件相关电位与情绪图像:刺激呈现时间的影响。
E Naumann, G Becker, S Maier, O Diedrich, D Bartussek

The perception of the emotional content of a stimulus is a preattentive automatic process which causes an emotional reaction. As the ongoing stream of behavior might be disturbed by the emotional reaction, a controlled process is initialited at the same time, which normally leads to an inhibition of the emotional response. By means of event related potentials it should be possible to observe these controlled processes. In a first study using photographs from the International Affective Picture System, Diedrich et al. (1997) reported enhanded P300 amplitudes for emotional stimuli, even when the task distracted from the emotional content of the stimuli. This was interpreted as an index of the additional, controlled information processing elicited by the emotional content of the stimuli. Additionally, Diedrich et al. observed a frontel slow positivity, which might indicate the inhibition of the emotional response. However, this frontal slow wave might also be explained by the stimulus presentation time, which lasted 500 ms. This study is a conceptual replication of the experiment of Diedrich et al. Stimulus presentation time of neutral and emotional slides was varied in three steps (250 ms, 500 ms and 2000 ms). Subjects either performed a structural or an emotion-focused task on the stimuli. The results for the P300 component were exactly replicated. However, the variation of slow frontal positivity differed from that in the first study. Differences in the intensity of the emotional stimuli are discussed as a reason für this result.

对刺激的情绪内容的感知是一个引起情绪反应的预先注意的自动过程。由于正在进行的行为流可能会受到情绪反应的干扰,因此同时启动了一个受控过程,这通常会导致情绪反应的抑制。通过事件相关电位,应该可以观察到这些受控过程。在第一个使用国际情感图片系统照片的研究中,Diedrich等人(1997)报告了情绪刺激的enhand P300振幅,即使任务从刺激的情感内容中分散了注意力。这被解释为由刺激的情感内容引起的额外的、受控制的信息处理的指数。此外,Diedrich等人观察到正面缓慢的积极情绪,这可能表明情绪反应受到抑制。然而,这种额叶慢波也可能与刺激呈现时间有关,刺激呈现时间持续了500 ms。本研究是对Diedrich等人实验的概念复制。中性和情绪性载玻片的刺激呈现时间分250 ms、500 ms和2000 ms三个阶段变化。受试者对刺激物进行结构任务或情感任务。P300组件的结果完全相同。然而,慢额叶正性的变化与第一项研究有所不同。讨论了情绪刺激强度的差异作为导致这一结果的原因。
{"title":"[Event related potentials and emotional pictures:effect of stimulus presentation time].","authors":"E Naumann,&nbsp;G Becker,&nbsp;S Maier,&nbsp;O Diedrich,&nbsp;D Bartussek","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of the emotional content of a stimulus is a preattentive automatic process which causes an emotional reaction. As the ongoing stream of behavior might be disturbed by the emotional reaction, a controlled process is initialited at the same time, which normally leads to an inhibition of the emotional response. By means of event related potentials it should be possible to observe these controlled processes. In a first study using photographs from the International Affective Picture System, Diedrich et al. (1997) reported enhanded P300 amplitudes for emotional stimuli, even when the task distracted from the emotional content of the stimuli. This was interpreted as an index of the additional, controlled information processing elicited by the emotional content of the stimuli. Additionally, Diedrich et al. observed a frontel slow positivity, which might indicate the inhibition of the emotional response. However, this frontal slow wave might also be explained by the stimulus presentation time, which lasted 500 ms. This study is a conceptual replication of the experiment of Diedrich et al. Stimulus presentation time of neutral and emotional slides was varied in three steps (250 ms, 500 ms and 2000 ms). Subjects either performed a structural or an emotion-focused task on the stimuli. The results for the P300 component were exactly replicated. However, the variation of slow frontal positivity differed from that in the first study. Differences in the intensity of the emotional stimuli are discussed as a reason für this result.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"44 1","pages":"163-85"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1997-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20422927","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Electrophysiologic dissociations in memory encoding of abstract and concrete words]. [抽象和具体词汇记忆编码中的电生理分离]。
A Mecklinger, A D Friederici

This study examined memory encoding of auditorily presented abstract and concrete nouns. 22 subjects performed various blocks of a free recall memory task in which lists of 22 either abstract or concrete words had to be memorized. Consistent with a large variety of memory studies, recall performance was better for concrete than for abstract words. When the event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded during study were selectively averaged for those words that were subsequently recalled and those subsequently not recalled, the ERPs were more positive going for words that were subsequently recalled. These Dm effects (Difference due to memory) started around 500 ms post-stimulus and differed in timing and scalp topography for both types of words: For abstract words, they were present in an early (i.e., 600 to 1100 ms) time interval at parieto-occipital electrodes only. In contrast, for concrete words, Dm effects were obtained with a broad topographic distribution in the 600 to 1000 ms time range and were also present in a late time interval (1100 to 1600 ms) at fronto-central recording sites. The topographical dissociations of the Dm effects in the early time interval are taken to reflect the larger distinctiveness of concrete words during encoding, whereas the late effects presumably play a functional role in elaborative processing of concrete words. The results do not agree with models of word concreteness that propose separate processing systems for the two types of words, and rather support those models that propose quantitative differences in the processing of abstract and concrete words.

本研究考察了听觉呈现的抽象名词和具体名词的记忆编码。22名受试者完成了一项自由回忆记忆任务的不同部分,该任务要求他们记住22个抽象或具体的单词。与大量的记忆研究一致,具体的记忆比抽象的记忆表现得更好。将实验中记录的事件相关电位(ERPs)选择性地平均之后被回忆的单词和之后没有被回忆的单词,发现之后被回忆的单词的ERPs更积极。这些Dm效应(由记忆引起的差异)在刺激后500毫秒左右开始,两种类型的单词在时间和头皮地形上都有所不同:对于抽象单词,它们仅在顶枕电极的早期(即600至1100毫秒)时间间隔出现。相比之下,对于具体单词,Dm效应在600至1000 ms时间范围内具有广泛的地形分布,并且在前中央记录点也存在较晚的时间间隔(1100至1600 ms)。早期Dm效应的地形解离反映了编码过程中具体词的显著性,而后期Dm效应可能在具体词的精细加工中起功能作用。研究结果不支持两类词的具体加工系统分离的模型,而支持抽象词和具体词加工数量差异的模型。
{"title":"[Electrophysiologic dissociations in memory encoding of abstract and concrete words].","authors":"A Mecklinger,&nbsp;A D Friederici","doi":"","DOIUrl":"","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined memory encoding of auditorily presented abstract and concrete nouns. 22 subjects performed various blocks of a free recall memory task in which lists of 22 either abstract or concrete words had to be memorized. Consistent with a large variety of memory studies, recall performance was better for concrete than for abstract words. When the event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded during study were selectively averaged for those words that were subsequently recalled and those subsequently not recalled, the ERPs were more positive going for words that were subsequently recalled. These Dm effects (Difference due to memory) started around 500 ms post-stimulus and differed in timing and scalp topography for both types of words: For abstract words, they were present in an early (i.e., 600 to 1100 ms) time interval at parieto-occipital electrodes only. In contrast, for concrete words, Dm effects were obtained with a broad topographic distribution in the 600 to 1000 ms time range and were also present in a late time interval (1100 to 1600 ms) at fronto-central recording sites. The topographical dissociations of the Dm effects in the early time interval are taken to reflect the larger distinctiveness of concrete words during encoding, whereas the late effects presumably play a functional role in elaborative processing of concrete words. The results do not agree with models of word concreteness that propose separate processing systems for the two types of words, and rather support those models that propose quantitative differences in the processing of abstract and concrete words.</p>","PeriodicalId":79386,"journal":{"name":"Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie","volume":"44 1","pages":"62-81"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1997-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"20422931","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1