首页 > 最新文献

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Clinical Assessment of Subjective Visual and Haptic Vertical Norms in Healthy Adults. 健康成年人主观视觉和触觉垂直规范的临床评估。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae049
Charlotte van der Waal, Wim Saeys, Steven Truijen, Elissa Embrechts

Background and objective: Accurate verticality perception is essential for daily life activities, such as correctly estimating object orientation in space. This study established normative data for the subjective visual vertical (SVV) and subjective haptic vertical (SHV) using the portable and self-constructable modified Bucket test and Rotating-Column test. Additionally, the contribution of age, sex, and starting position of the line/ column on SVV and SHV accuracy were evaluated.

Method: This study, part of the PRECISE project (ClinicalTrials.gov ID NCT05978596), was conducted following the STROBE guidelines. Healthy adults without visual/neurological/vestibular disorders were recruited. Subjective visual vertical and SHV accuracy were described in terms of constant errors (i.e., mean deviation from 0° [true vertical] respecting its direction), unsigned errors (i.e., mean deviation from 0° irrespective of direction), and variability (i.e., intra-individual standard deviation).

Results: Sixty participants were evaluated (mean age: 41.14 [SD = 16.74] years). Subjective visual vertical constant errors between -2.82° and 2.90°, unsigned errors up to 2.15°, and variability up to 1.61° are considered normal. Subjective haptic vertical constant errors ranged from -6.94° to 8.18°, unsigned errors up to 6.66° and variability up to 4.25°. Higher ages led to higher SVV unsigned errors and variability. SHV variability was higher in females compared to males. Certain starting positions led to higher SVV and SHV constants and SVV unsigned errors.

Discussion: Normative data are provided for affordable, self-constructable, and portable SVV and SHV tools. These norms are consistent with more sophisticated equipment and can be used to distinguish between normal and abnormal values.

背景和目的:准确的垂直感知对日常生活活动至关重要,例如正确估计物体在空间中的方位。本研究利用便携式自构造改良水桶测试和旋转柱测试,建立了主观视觉垂直度(SVV)和主观触觉垂直度(SHV)的标准数据。此外,还评估了年龄、性别和线/柱的起始位置对 SVV 和 SHV 准确性的影响:本研究是 PRECISE 项目(ClinicalTrials.gov ID NCT05978596)的一部分,按照 STROBE 指南进行。研究招募了无视觉/神经/前庭疾病的健康成年人。主观视觉垂直和SHV准确度用恒定误差(即与0°[真实垂直]方向的平均偏差)、无符号误差(即与0°方向的平均偏差,与方向无关)和变异性(即个体内部标准偏差)来描述:对 60 名参与者进行了评估(平均年龄:41.14 [SD = 16.74] 岁)。主观视觉垂直常数误差在 -2.82° 和 2.90° 之间,无符号误差不超过 2.15°,变异性不超过 1.61°,均视为正常。主观触觉垂直常数误差在-6.94°到8.18°之间,无符号误差最大为6.66°,变异性最大为4.25°。年龄越大,SVV 的无符号误差和变异性越高。与男性相比,女性的 SHV 变异性更高。某些起始位置会导致较高的 SVV 和 SHV 常数以及 SVV 无符号误差:为经济实惠、可自行构建和便携式 SVV 和 SHV 工具提供了规范数据。这些标准与更复杂的设备一致,可用于区分正常值和异常值。
{"title":"Clinical Assessment of Subjective Visual and Haptic Vertical Norms in Healthy Adults.","authors":"Charlotte van der Waal, Wim Saeys, Steven Truijen, Elissa Embrechts","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae049","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae049","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background and objective: </strong>Accurate verticality perception is essential for daily life activities, such as correctly estimating object orientation in space. This study established normative data for the subjective visual vertical (SVV) and subjective haptic vertical (SHV) using the portable and self-constructable modified Bucket test and Rotating-Column test. Additionally, the contribution of age, sex, and starting position of the line/ column on SVV and SHV accuracy were evaluated.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>This study, part of the PRECISE project (ClinicalTrials.gov ID NCT05978596), was conducted following the STROBE guidelines. Healthy adults without visual/neurological/vestibular disorders were recruited. Subjective visual vertical and SHV accuracy were described in terms of constant errors (i.e., mean deviation from 0° [true vertical] respecting its direction), unsigned errors (i.e., mean deviation from 0° irrespective of direction), and variability (i.e., intra-individual standard deviation).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Sixty participants were evaluated (mean age: 41.14 [SD = 16.74] years). Subjective visual vertical constant errors between -2.82° and 2.90°, unsigned errors up to 2.15°, and variability up to 1.61° are considered normal. Subjective haptic vertical constant errors ranged from -6.94° to 8.18°, unsigned errors up to 6.66° and variability up to 4.25°. Higher ages led to higher SVV unsigned errors and variability. SHV variability was higher in females compared to males. Certain starting positions led to higher SVV and SHV constants and SVV unsigned errors.</p><p><strong>Discussion: </strong>Normative data are provided for affordable, self-constructable, and portable SVV and SHV tools. These norms are consistent with more sophisticated equipment and can be used to distinguish between normal and abnormal values.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1408-1417"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141465755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anticholinergic Medication Burden and Cognitive Subtypes in Parkinson's Disease without Dementia. 无痴呆症帕金森病患者的抗胆碱能药物负担和认知亚型。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae041
Lauren G Santos, Lauren E Kenney, Alyssa Ray, Alfredo Paredes, Adrianna M Ratajska, Kara Eversole, Bhavana Patel, Ashley E Rawls, Michael S Okun, Dawn Bowers

Objective: Cognitive changes are heterogeneous in Parkinson's disease (PD). This study compared whether anticholinergic burden drives differences in cognitive domain performance and empirically-derived PD-cognitive phenotypes.

Method: A retrospective chart review contained participants (n = 493) who had idiopathic PD without dementia. Participants' medications were scored (0-3) and summed based on the anticholinergic cognitive burden scale (ACBS). We examined the ACBS' relationship to five cognitive domain composites (normative z-scores) and three (K-means clustering based) cognitive phenotypes: cognitively intact, low executive function (EF), and predominately impaired EF/memory. Analyses included Spearman correlations, analysis of covariance, and Pearson chi-squared test.

Results: Overall, phenotypes did not differ in anticholinergic burden, and (after false-discovery-rate corrections) no cognitive domains related. When comparing those above and below the clinically relevant ACBS cutoff (i.e., score ≥3), no significant phenotype or domain differences were found.

Conclusions: Anticholinergic medication usage did not drive cognitive performance in a large clinical sample of idiopathic PD without dementia.

目的:帕金森病(PD)患者的认知变化多种多样。本研究比较了抗胆碱能负荷是否会导致认知领域表现的差异以及根据经验得出的帕金森病认知表型:一项回顾性病历审查包含了患有特发性帕金森病且无痴呆症的参与者(n = 493)。根据抗胆碱能认知负担量表(ACBS)对参与者的用药情况进行评分(0-3分)和加总。我们研究了 ACBS 与五个认知领域复合体(常模 Z 值)和三种认知表型(基于 K-均值聚类)之间的关系:认知完好、执行功能(EF)低下和 EF/记忆力严重受损。分析包括斯皮尔曼相关性、协方差分析和皮尔逊卡方检验:结果:总体而言,表型在抗胆碱能负荷方面没有差异,而且(经过误诊率校正后)没有认知领域相关。在比较高于和低于 ACBS 临床相关临界值(即得分≥3)的患者时,没有发现明显的表型或领域差异:结论:抗胆碱能药物的使用并不会影响无痴呆的特发性帕金森病大样本患者的认知能力。
{"title":"Anticholinergic Medication Burden and Cognitive Subtypes in Parkinson's Disease without Dementia.","authors":"Lauren G Santos, Lauren E Kenney, Alyssa Ray, Alfredo Paredes, Adrianna M Ratajska, Kara Eversole, Bhavana Patel, Ashley E Rawls, Michael S Okun, Dawn Bowers","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae041","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae041","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Cognitive changes are heterogeneous in Parkinson's disease (PD). This study compared whether anticholinergic burden drives differences in cognitive domain performance and empirically-derived PD-cognitive phenotypes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A retrospective chart review contained participants (n = 493) who had idiopathic PD without dementia. Participants' medications were scored (0-3) and summed based on the anticholinergic cognitive burden scale (ACBS). We examined the ACBS' relationship to five cognitive domain composites (normative z-scores) and three (K-means clustering based) cognitive phenotypes: cognitively intact, low executive function (EF), and predominately impaired EF/memory. Analyses included Spearman correlations, analysis of covariance, and Pearson chi-squared test.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Overall, phenotypes did not differ in anticholinergic burden, and (after false-discovery-rate corrections) no cognitive domains related. When comparing those above and below the clinically relevant ACBS cutoff (i.e., score ≥3), no significant phenotype or domain differences were found.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Anticholinergic medication usage did not drive cognitive performance in a large clinical sample of idiopathic PD without dementia.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1443-1449"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11586454/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141154553","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to the Special Series on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Performances in Five Asian Countries Series. 韦氏成人智力量表-IV 在五个亚洲国家的表现特别系列丛书导言。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae081
Daryl E Fujii
{"title":"Introduction to the Special Series on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Performances in Five Asian Countries Series.","authors":"Daryl E Fujii","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae081","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae081","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1313-1320"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520831","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Similarities and Differences in the WAIS-IV Performances in Korean and U.S. Populations. 韩国人和美国人在 WAIS-IV 成绩上的异同。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae086
Dongwook D Lee, Kyoung Hi Ryu, So-Hyun Ahn

Objective: This exploratory study examines the cognitive profiles of South Koreans using the WAIS-IV. It compares scores from the original U.S. version (USW) with those from the Korean version (KW) to identify potential cultural influences on performance.

Method: Raw scores corresponding to a scaled score of 10 for each subtest of the KW were used to compare the performances of South Koreans and Americans across subtests and composite indices. The impact of education was assessed through correlations with educational attainment ratios from both countries.

Results: When combined across all age groups, the scores of the South Korean sample were significantly lower than those of U.S. sample on the Similarities, Comprehension, and Picture Completion subtests, and Verbal Comprehension Index. However, younger South Koreans scored significantly higher on Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, Visual Puzzles, Figure Weights, Digit Span, Arithmetic, Letter-Number Sequencing, Symbol Search, and Coding and all composite scores except Verbal Comprehension. Older South Koreans scored significantly lower on Similarities, Vocabulary, Comprehension, Matrix Reasoning, Visual Puzzles, Picture Completion, and the composite scores of Verbal Comprehension and Perceptual Reasoning, compared to their U.S. counterparts. All subtest and composite scores were highly correlated with education.

Conclusions: The South Korean and U.S. samples demonstrated distinct cognitive profiles on the WAIS-IV when comparing relative performances on subtests and composite measures. Cultural factors including socioeconomic changes, education quality, nuances in language characteristics, and implicit differences in cognitive processing were discussed as possible contributors to the unique cognitive profile of the South Korean sample.

研究目的本探索性研究使用 WAIS-IV 对韩国人的认知概况进行了调查。它比较了美国原版(USW)和韩国版(KW)的分数,以确定文化对成绩的潜在影响:方法:KW每个分测验的原始分数对应于10分的比例分数,用来比较韩国人和美国人在各分测验和综合指数上的表现。通过与两国教育程度比率的相关性来评估教育的影响:综合所有年龄组的情况,韩国样本在相似性、理解、图片完成子测验和言语理解指数上的得分明显低于美国样本。然而,年龄较小的韩国人在积木设计、矩阵推理、视觉拼图、图形权重、数字跨度、算术、字母-数字排序、符号搜索和编码以及除言语理解外的所有综合分数上的得分都明显高于美国人。与美国老年人相比,韩国老年人在相似性、词汇、理解、矩阵推理、视觉拼图、图片补全以及言语理解和感知推理的综合得分上明显较低。所有小测试和综合得分均与教育程度高度相关:结论:在比较分测验和综合测量的相对成绩时,韩国和美国样本在 WAIS-IV 中表现出了不同的认知特征。包括社会经济变化、教育质量、语言特点的细微差别以及认知处理过程中的内隐差异在内的文化因素可能是导致韩国样本出现独特认知特征的原因。
{"title":"Similarities and Differences in the WAIS-IV Performances in Korean and U.S. Populations.","authors":"Dongwook D Lee, Kyoung Hi Ryu, So-Hyun Ahn","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae086","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae086","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This exploratory study examines the cognitive profiles of South Koreans using the WAIS-IV. It compares scores from the original U.S. version (USW) with those from the Korean version (KW) to identify potential cultural influences on performance.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Raw scores corresponding to a scaled score of 10 for each subtest of the KW were used to compare the performances of South Koreans and Americans across subtests and composite indices. The impact of education was assessed through correlations with educational attainment ratios from both countries.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>When combined across all age groups, the scores of the South Korean sample were significantly lower than those of U.S. sample on the Similarities, Comprehension, and Picture Completion subtests, and Verbal Comprehension Index. However, younger South Koreans scored significantly higher on Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, Visual Puzzles, Figure Weights, Digit Span, Arithmetic, Letter-Number Sequencing, Symbol Search, and Coding and all composite scores except Verbal Comprehension. Older South Koreans scored significantly lower on Similarities, Vocabulary, Comprehension, Matrix Reasoning, Visual Puzzles, Picture Completion, and the composite scores of Verbal Comprehension and Perceptual Reasoning, compared to their U.S. counterparts. All subtest and composite scores were highly correlated with education.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The South Korean and U.S. samples demonstrated distinct cognitive profiles on the WAIS-IV when comparing relative performances on subtests and composite measures. Cultural factors including socioeconomic changes, education quality, nuances in language characteristics, and implicit differences in cognitive processing were discussed as possible contributors to the unique cognitive profile of the South Korean sample.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1332-1341"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520835","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revealing Cultural Dynamics in WAIS-IV Performance: a Comparative Analysis of Age Cohorts in Taiwanese and U.S. Populations. 揭示WAIS-IV成绩的文化动态:台湾与美国人口年龄组的比较分析
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae089
Yu-Ling Chang, Wen-Yu Cheng

Objective: This study examined the relationship between culture and cognitive abilities by comparing WAIS-IV subtests, index scores, and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) scores across various age groups in the Taiwanese and U.S. populations.

Method: The Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV are comprehensively compared, examining subtest items, psychometrics, and sample characteristics. Scaled scores are compared by extracting raw scores with a scaled score of 10 from each subtest scale in the Taiwanese version and applying U.S. age norms to acquire U.S. scaled scores.

Results: Despite the mean FSIQ score closely aligning with the U.S. sample, notable discrepancies are evident in the Taiwanese Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) score, potentially influenced by cultural fairness of the tests. Significant variations are observed among age cohorts in the Taiwanese sample, with younger individuals excelling in Processing Speed Index, Working Memory Index, Perceptual Reasoning Index, and FSIQ, while maintaining comparable VCI scores to their U.S. counterparts. Conversely, older cohorts demonstrate lower performance across various domains, except for visuospatial reasoning and organizational skills, compared to their U.S. counterparts. These subtest variations robustly correlate with educational disparities between the Taiwanese and U.S. samples.

Conclusions: Despite the similarity in factor structures between the Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV, this study reveals cultural bias in both verbal and non-verbal subtests. The study highlights the intricate interplay among cognitive processing styles, cultural influences, and educational factors contributing to performance disparities.

研究目的本研究通过比较台湾和美国不同年龄组人群的WAIS-IV分测验、指数得分和全量表智商(FSIQ)得分,研究文化与认知能力之间的关系:方法:对台湾版和美国版的WAIS-IV进行全面比较,考察分测验项目、心理测量学和样本特征。通过从台湾版的每个分测验量表中提取标度为10的原始分数,并应用美国年龄标准来获得美国的标度分数,从而对标度分数进行比较:结果:尽管 FSIQ 的平均得分与美国样本非常接近,但台湾的言语理解指数(VCI)得分却存在明显差异,这可能是受测试的文化公平性影响。在台湾样本中,各年龄组之间存在显著差异,年轻人在处理速度指数、工作记忆指数、感知推理指数和 FSIQ 方面表现突出,而 VCI 分数则与美国同龄人相当。相反,除视觉空间推理和组织能力外,老年人群在其他领域的表现均低于美国同龄人。这些分测验的差异与台湾和美国样本之间的教育差异密切相关:尽管台湾版和美国版WAIS-IV的因子结构相似,但本研究揭示了语言和非语言分测验中的文化偏差。本研究强调了认知加工风格、文化影响和教育因素之间错综复杂的相互作用,这些因素都是造成成绩差异的原因。
{"title":"Revealing Cultural Dynamics in WAIS-IV Performance: a Comparative Analysis of Age Cohorts in Taiwanese and U.S. Populations.","authors":"Yu-Ling Chang, Wen-Yu Cheng","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae089","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae089","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study examined the relationship between culture and cognitive abilities by comparing WAIS-IV subtests, index scores, and Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) scores across various age groups in the Taiwanese and U.S. populations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>The Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV are comprehensively compared, examining subtest items, psychometrics, and sample characteristics. Scaled scores are compared by extracting raw scores with a scaled score of 10 from each subtest scale in the Taiwanese version and applying U.S. age norms to acquire U.S. scaled scores.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Despite the mean FSIQ score closely aligning with the U.S. sample, notable discrepancies are evident in the Taiwanese Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) score, potentially influenced by cultural fairness of the tests. Significant variations are observed among age cohorts in the Taiwanese sample, with younger individuals excelling in Processing Speed Index, Working Memory Index, Perceptual Reasoning Index, and FSIQ, while maintaining comparable VCI scores to their U.S. counterparts. Conversely, older cohorts demonstrate lower performance across various domains, except for visuospatial reasoning and organizational skills, compared to their U.S. counterparts. These subtest variations robustly correlate with educational disparities between the Taiwanese and U.S. samples.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Despite the similarity in factor structures between the Taiwanese and U.S. versions of WAIS-IV, this study reveals cultural bias in both verbal and non-verbal subtests. The study highlights the intricate interplay among cognitive processing styles, cultural influences, and educational factors contributing to performance disparities.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1342-1353"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Special Series on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Performances in Five Asian Countries: Summary of Findings, Clinical Implications, and Future Directions. 韦氏成人智力量表-IV 在五个亚洲国家的表现特别系列:研究结果摘要、临床意义和未来方向。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae083
Daryl E Fujii

Introduction: This special series of exploratory studies compared WAIS-IV performances in five Asian countries: Japan, South Korea, Taiwan, India, and Indonesia with the U.S. The studies examined cognitive profiles across age groups to develop hypotheses on how culture can impact cognition. This summary article integrates the findings from each study to determine similarities and differences across Asian countries and proposes possible underlying cultural factors impacting cognition.

Methods: In each study, raw scores corresponding to a subscale score of 10 were scored with U.S. norms across all age groups. Index scores were calculated with the adjusted scale scores. The impact of education on test performances was determined by correlating WAIS-IV scores with a ratio of educational attainment for each country with the U.S. for each age group. Findings from each country were then integrated to develop hypotheses on cultural factors that impacted cognitive profiles.

Results: East Asian countries (Japan, South Korea, Taiwan) demonstrated weaker Verbal Comprehension Index scores and stronger Processing Speed Index scores than the U.S. sample, although the latter was moderated by age. East Asian countries demonstrated stronger performances on Perceptual Reasoning versus Verbal Comprehension tests. East Asians and the Java subset of the Indonesian sample demonstrated a pattern of stronger performances in younger versus older cohorts which was significantly correlated with educational attainment.

Discussion: Cultural factors impacting cognition were proposed. Clinical implications of the findings were discussed and directions for future studies to examine the relationship between culture and cognition were recommended.

导言:这一系列探索性研究比较了五个亚洲国家的 WAIS-IV 成绩,包括日本、韩国、台湾、印度和印度尼西亚:这些研究考察了不同年龄组的认知概况,从而提出了文化如何影响认知的假设。本总结文章综合了每项研究的结果,以确定亚洲各国之间的异同,并提出影响认知的潜在文化因素:在每项研究中,所有年龄组的原始分数都与美国标准的 10 分分量表相对应。根据调整后的量表得分计算指数得分。通过将 WAIS-IV 分数与每个年龄组中每个国家与美国的教育程度比率相关联,确定教育对测试成绩的影响。然后综合各国的研究结果,就影响认知能力的文化因素提出假设:与美国样本相比,东亚国家(日本、韩国、台湾)的言语理解指数得分较低,处理速度指数得分较高,但后者受年龄影响较小。东亚国家在感知推理测试和言语理解测试中的表现更强。东亚人和印尼样本中的爪哇子集表现出年轻组群相对于年长组群更强的模式,这与受教育程度有显著相关:讨论:提出了影响认知的文化因素。讨论:提出了影响认知的文化因素,讨论了研究结果的临床意义,并为今后研究文化与认知之间关系的方向提出了建议。
{"title":"Special Series on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Performances in Five Asian Countries: Summary of Findings, Clinical Implications, and Future Directions.","authors":"Daryl E Fujii","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae083","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae083","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>This special series of exploratory studies compared WAIS-IV performances in five Asian countries: Japan, South Korea, Taiwan, India, and Indonesia with the U.S. The studies examined cognitive profiles across age groups to develop hypotheses on how culture can impact cognition. This summary article integrates the findings from each study to determine similarities and differences across Asian countries and proposes possible underlying cultural factors impacting cognition.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>In each study, raw scores corresponding to a subscale score of 10 were scored with U.S. norms across all age groups. Index scores were calculated with the adjusted scale scores. The impact of education on test performances was determined by correlating WAIS-IV scores with a ratio of educational attainment for each country with the U.S. for each age group. Findings from each country were then integrated to develop hypotheses on cultural factors that impacted cognitive profiles.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>East Asian countries (Japan, South Korea, Taiwan) demonstrated weaker Verbal Comprehension Index scores and stronger Processing Speed Index scores than the U.S. sample, although the latter was moderated by age. East Asian countries demonstrated stronger performances on Perceptual Reasoning versus Verbal Comprehension tests. East Asians and the Java subset of the Indonesian sample demonstrated a pattern of stronger performances in younger versus older cohorts which was significantly correlated with educational attainment.</p><p><strong>Discussion: </strong>Cultural factors impacting cognition were proposed. Clinical implications of the findings were discussed and directions for future studies to examine the relationship between culture and cognition were recommended.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1373-1380"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520836","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural Considerations in Intelligence Test Adaptations: a Critical Review of the WAIS-IV India and Its U.K. and U.S. Counterparts. 智力测验改编中的文化考量:WAIS-IV 印度版及其英国和美国版的批判性评论。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae090
Porrselvi A P, Farzin Irani, Urvashi Shah, Preeti Sunderaraman

Objective: This study critically examined the adaptation and normative processes of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV)India.

Method: WAIS-IV U.K. Edition Administration and Scoring Manual, WAIS-IVIndia Administration and Scoring Manual, and WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual and Technical and Interpretive Manuals were used to extract test development and adaptation process data. The analyses were descriptive and qualitative.

Results: A review of the manuals showed that the normative sample were fluent English speakers with high education levels, which is not representative of India's multilingual and educational diversity. The study identified minimal cultural and linguistic adaptations in subtests, which raised concern about potential biases in the test adaptation process across cultures. The lack of clarity in item selection, demographic data, sample selection, and psychometric properties raised questions about the test's reliability and validity. Issues related to education, age, gender, and regional distribution are discussed, emphasizing the test's limitations in capturing the diverse characteristics of the heterogeneous Indian population.

Conclusions: WAIS-IV India may not adequately represent India's population. More careful consideration of socioecological and psychometric factors when adapting intelligence (IQ) tests and use of advanced data harmonization methods can provide robust cross-national statistical harmonization in the future along with more ecologically valid local test development efforts. This can allow consideration of social determinants of health and other factors that can inform a more nuanced approach to IQ test development in non-western contexts.

研究目的本研究对韦氏成人智力量表--第四版(WAIS-IV)印度版的改编和常模过程进行了批判性研究:方法:使用《WAIS-IV 英国版管理与计分手册》、《WAIS-IV 印度版管理与计分手册》、《WAIS-IV 管理与计分手册》和《技术与解释手册》来提取测验开发和适应过程的数据。分析采用描述性和定性分析:对手册的审查表明,常模样本都是讲流利英语且受教育程度较高的人,这并不能代表印度的多语言和教育多样性。研究发现,分测验在文化和语言方面的调整微乎其微,这引起了人们对跨文化测验调整过程中可能出现的偏差的担忧。在项目选择、人口统计数据、样本选择和心理测量特性方面缺乏明确性,使人对测验的可靠性和有效性产生疑问。我们讨论了与教育、年龄、性别和地区分布有关的问题,强调了该测试在捕捉印度异质人口的不同特征方面的局限性:结论:WAIS-IV 印度版可能无法充分代表印度人口。在改编智力(IQ)测验时,更仔细地考虑社会生态学和心理测量学因素,并使用先进的数据协调方法,可以在未来提供稳健的跨国统计协调,以及更生态有效的本地测验开发工作。这样就可以考虑健康的社会决定因素和其他因素,为在非西方背景下开发智商测验提供更细致的方法。
{"title":"Cultural Considerations in Intelligence Test Adaptations: a Critical Review of the WAIS-IV India and Its U.K. and U.S. Counterparts.","authors":"Porrselvi A P, Farzin Irani, Urvashi Shah, Preeti Sunderaraman","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae090","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae090","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study critically examined the adaptation and normative processes of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV)India.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>WAIS-IV U.K. Edition Administration and Scoring Manual, WAIS-IVIndia Administration and Scoring Manual, and WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual and Technical and Interpretive Manuals were used to extract test development and adaptation process data. The analyses were descriptive and qualitative.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>A review of the manuals showed that the normative sample were fluent English speakers with high education levels, which is not representative of India's multilingual and educational diversity. The study identified minimal cultural and linguistic adaptations in subtests, which raised concern about potential biases in the test adaptation process across cultures. The lack of clarity in item selection, demographic data, sample selection, and psychometric properties raised questions about the test's reliability and validity. Issues related to education, age, gender, and regional distribution are discussed, emphasizing the test's limitations in capturing the diverse characteristics of the heterogeneous Indian population.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>WAIS-IV India may not adequately represent India's population. More careful consideration of socioecological and psychometric factors when adapting intelligence (IQ) tests and use of advanced data harmonization methods can provide robust cross-national statistical harmonization in the future along with more ecologically valid local test development efforts. This can allow consideration of social determinants of health and other factors that can inform a more nuanced approach to IQ test development in non-western contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1354-1362"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Japanese Performance Profile on the WAIS-IV and Purported Cultural Influences. 日本人在 WAIS-IV 考试中的成绩概况及所谓的文化影响。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae080
Daryl E Fujii, Erin T Kaseda, Maiko Sakamoto-Pomeroy

Objective: The present exploratory study aimed to examine the potential impact of culture on cognitive skills and intelligence by comparing the Japanese Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) (JW) subtests, IQs, and index scores to those of the U.S. WAIS-IV (USW).

Method: JW raw scores corresponding to a scaled score of 10 for each subtest were scored with USW norms. Subtest, index, and IQ scores were then calculated for each age range. The impact of education on scores was evaluated through ratio of educational attainment for each cohort of the Japanese and US samples. Japanese and US data were compared using one-sample t-tests. Correlations between subtest, index, and IQ scores and education were calculated.

Results: The USW sample performed higher than the JW sample on verbal comprehension subtests, while the JW sample demonstrated stronger performances in processing speed and perceptual reasoning subtests. However, all scores with the exception of Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, and Figure Weights were significantly associated with differential educational attainment between the two countries. Educational differences were linearly associated with age with the higher educational attainment for younger Japanese age groups and higher attainment for the older US cohorts.

Conclusions: The present study demonstrates cognitive differences between Japan and the USA. Performance on the WAIS-IV subtest and composite measures are highly correlated with education. Cultural factors impacting the cognitive profile of the Japanese sample such as differences in worldview, customs, educational systems, and writing system, were proposed. Clinical neuropsychologists should take these aspects into account when administering and interpreting test results.

研究目的本探索性研究旨在通过比较日本韦氏成人智力量表-IV(WAIS-IV)(JW)与美国韦氏成人智力量表-IV(USW)的分测验、智商和指数得分,研究文化对认知技能和智力的潜在影响:方法:根据 USW 标准,对 JW 的原始分数进行评分,每个分测验的比例分数为 10。然后计算每个年龄段的分测验、指数和智商分数。教育程度对分数的影响是通过日本和美国样本中每个组群的教育程度比率来评估的。日本和美国的数据使用单样本 t 检验进行比较。计算了分测验、指数和智商分数与教育程度之间的相关性:结果:在言语理解分测验中,美西样本的成绩高于日西样本,而日西样本在处理速度和感知推理分测验中的成绩更强。然而,除了积木设计、矩阵推理和图形权重之外,其他所有得分都与两国教育程度的差异有显著关联。教育差异与年龄呈线性相关,日本年轻群体的教育程度较高,而美国年长群体的教育程度较高:本研究显示了日本和美国之间的认知差异。WAIS-IV分测验和综合测量的成绩与教育程度高度相关。研究提出了影响日本样本认知概况的文化因素,如世界观、习俗、教育体系和书写系统的差异。临床神经心理学家在实施和解释测试结果时应将这些因素考虑在内。
{"title":"Japanese Performance Profile on the WAIS-IV and Purported Cultural Influences.","authors":"Daryl E Fujii, Erin T Kaseda, Maiko Sakamoto-Pomeroy","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae080","DOIUrl":"10.1093/arclin/acae080","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The present exploratory study aimed to examine the potential impact of culture on cognitive skills and intelligence by comparing the Japanese Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) (JW) subtests, IQs, and index scores to those of the U.S. WAIS-IV (USW).</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>JW raw scores corresponding to a scaled score of 10 for each subtest were scored with USW norms. Subtest, index, and IQ scores were then calculated for each age range. The impact of education on scores was evaluated through ratio of educational attainment for each cohort of the Japanese and US samples. Japanese and US data were compared using one-sample t-tests. Correlations between subtest, index, and IQ scores and education were calculated.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The USW sample performed higher than the JW sample on verbal comprehension subtests, while the JW sample demonstrated stronger performances in processing speed and perceptual reasoning subtests. However, all scores with the exception of Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, and Figure Weights were significantly associated with differential educational attainment between the two countries. Educational differences were linearly associated with age with the higher educational attainment for younger Japanese age groups and higher attainment for the older US cohorts.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The present study demonstrates cognitive differences between Japan and the USA. Performance on the WAIS-IV subtest and composite measures are highly correlated with education. Cultural factors impacting the cognitive profile of the Japanese sample such as differences in worldview, customs, educational systems, and writing system, were proposed. Clinical neuropsychologists should take these aspects into account when administering and interpreting test results.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"1321-1331"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142520832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Diagnosed and Nondisclosed Concussions Among Young Athletes With ADHD. 患有多动症的年轻运动员中确诊和未披露的脑震荡。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-20 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae108
Ara J Schmitt, Michael Matta, Abigail C Bretzin, Maria Tina Benno, Kirstin Franklin, Erica Beidler

Objective: Within a sample of young athletes with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), to explore the frequency of disclosed and nondisclosed concussions, identify reasons the youth did not report a suspected concussion, and learn the frequency that the youth still practiced or played in a game after a suspected concussion.

Method: Cross-sectional surveys were completed by 448 youth athletes (ages 8-14 years) and a corresponding parent (or caregiver). As part of larger respective surveys, questions regarding ADHD status and concussion history were asked of the youth and parents. Data regarding 40 youth with ADHD were available for analyses. Due to the low frequency of concussive injuries within the total sample, descriptive statistics and qualitative techniques were used to contextualize the data.

Results: The youth with ADHD were not more likely to experience at least one diagnosed concussion or nondisclosed concussion as compared to non-ADHD controls. Three of 40 youth athletes with ADHD (8%) had been diagnosed with a concussion, whereas 5 (13%) self-reported a nondisclosed concussion. Not wanting to lose playing time and not wanting to miss a game were the two most endorsed reasons for concussion nondisclosure.

Conclusions: More research is needed to understand the present results considering other research that puts similarly aged athletes with ADHD at greater risk for concussion. Education for youth with ADHD might help reduce the number of nondisclosed concussions that have a more complex recovery trajectory in this population.

目的在注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)青少年运动员样本中,调查已披露和未披露脑震荡的频率,确定青少年未报告疑似脑震荡的原因,并了解青少年在疑似脑震荡后仍进行训练或参加比赛的频率:448 名青少年运动员(8-14 岁)和相应的家长(或看护人)完成了横向调查。作为各自调查的一部分,还向青少年和家长询问了有关多动症状况和脑震荡病史的问题。40 名患有多动症的青少年的数据可供分析。由于所有样本中脑震荡受伤的频率较低,因此采用了描述性统计和定性技术来分析数据的来龙去脉:与非多动症对照组相比,患有多动症的青少年并不更有可能经历至少一次经诊断的脑震荡或未披露的脑震荡。在 40 名患有多动症的青少年运动员中,有 3 人(8%)被诊断出患有脑震荡,而有 5 人(13%)自述患有未披露的脑震荡。不想失去上场时间和不想缺席比赛是不披露脑震荡的两个最被认可的原因:考虑到其他研究表明,患有多动症的同龄运动员患脑震荡的风险更大,因此需要开展更多的研究来了解目前的研究结果。对患有多动症的青少年进行教育可能有助于减少未披露脑震荡的数量,因为这类人群的康复轨迹更为复杂。
{"title":"Diagnosed and Nondisclosed Concussions Among Young Athletes With ADHD.","authors":"Ara J Schmitt, Michael Matta, Abigail C Bretzin, Maria Tina Benno, Kirstin Franklin, Erica Beidler","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae108","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acae108","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Within a sample of young athletes with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), to explore the frequency of disclosed and nondisclosed concussions, identify reasons the youth did not report a suspected concussion, and learn the frequency that the youth still practiced or played in a game after a suspected concussion.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Cross-sectional surveys were completed by 448 youth athletes (ages 8-14 years) and a corresponding parent (or caregiver). As part of larger respective surveys, questions regarding ADHD status and concussion history were asked of the youth and parents. Data regarding 40 youth with ADHD were available for analyses. Due to the low frequency of concussive injuries within the total sample, descriptive statistics and qualitative techniques were used to contextualize the data.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The youth with ADHD were not more likely to experience at least one diagnosed concussion or nondisclosed concussion as compared to non-ADHD controls. Three of 40 youth athletes with ADHD (8%) had been diagnosed with a concussion, whereas 5 (13%) self-reported a nondisclosed concussion. Not wanting to lose playing time and not wanting to miss a game were the two most endorsed reasons for concussion nondisclosure.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>More research is needed to understand the present results considering other research that puts similarly aged athletes with ADHD at greater risk for concussion. Education for youth with ADHD might help reduce the number of nondisclosed concussions that have a more complex recovery trajectory in this population.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142675003","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Retainer Bias: Ethical and Practical Considerations for the Forensic Neuropsychologist. 预聘偏差:法医神经心理学家的伦理与实践考量》(Retainer Bias: Ethical and Practical Considerations for the Forensic Neuropsychologist)。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acae104
Diana S Goldstein, Joel E Morgan

How is it that practicing forensic neuropsychologists occasionally see substandard work from other colleagues, or more fundamentally, have such disparate opinions on the same case? One answer might be that in every profession, competence varies. Another possibility has little to do with competence, but professional conduct. In this paper we discuss the process by which retainer bias may occur. Retainer bias is a form of confirmatory bias, i.e., in assessment, the tendency to seek, favor, and interpret data and make judgments and decisions that support a predetermined expectation or hypothesis, ignoring or dismissing data that challenge that hypothesis ( Nickerson, 1998). The tendency to interpret data in support of the retaining attorney's position of advocacy may be intentional - that is, within conscious awareness and explicit, or it may be unintentional, outside of one's awareness, representing implicit bias. While some practitioners accept referrals from both sides in litigation, numerous uncontrollable factors converge in such a manner that one's practice may nevertheless become associated with one side. Such imbalance is not a reliable index of bias. With brief hypothetical scenarios, in this paper we discuss contextual factors that increase risk for retainer bias and problematic practice approaches that may be used to support one side in litigation, violating ethical principles, codes of conduct and guidelines for engaging in forensic work. We also discuss debiasing techniques recommended within the empirical literature and call on the subspecialty field of forensic neuropsychology to conduct research into retainer bias and other sources of opinion variability.

为什么执业的法医神经心理学家偶尔会看到其他同事的工作不达标,或者更重要的是,他们对同一案件会有如此不同的意见?答案之一可能是,每个行业的能力都有差异。另一种可能与能力无关,而是与职业操守有关。在本文中,我们将讨论聘用偏见可能发生的过程。预聘偏差是一种确认性偏差,即在评估中,倾向于寻找、偏爱和解释数据,并做出支持预先确定的预期或假设的判断和决定,而忽略或驳回质疑该假设的数据(Nickerson,1998 年)。为支持聘用律师的辩护立场而解释数据的倾向可能是有意为之的,即在有意识的范围内,并且是明确的;也可能是无意的,在个人意识之外,代表着隐性偏见。虽然有些执业律师在诉讼中接受来自双方的转介,但众多不可控因素汇聚在一起,可能会使其执业与一方产生关联。这种不平衡并不是偏见的可靠指标。本文通过简短的假设情景,讨论了增加聘用偏倚风险的背景因素,以及可能被用于支持诉讼一方的有问题的执业方法,这些都违反了从事法证工作的道德原则、行为准则和指导方针。我们还讨论了实证文献中推荐的去伪存真技术,并呼吁法医神经心理学的亚专业领域对预聘人员偏见和其他意见变异来源进行研究。
{"title":"Retainer Bias: Ethical and Practical Considerations for the Forensic Neuropsychologist.","authors":"Diana S Goldstein, Joel E Morgan","doi":"10.1093/arclin/acae104","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acae104","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How is it that practicing forensic neuropsychologists occasionally see substandard work from other colleagues, or more fundamentally, have such disparate opinions on the same case? One answer might be that in every profession, competence varies. Another possibility has little to do with competence, but professional conduct. In this paper we discuss the process by which retainer bias may occur. Retainer bias is a form of confirmatory bias, i.e., in assessment, the tendency to seek, favor, and interpret data and make judgments and decisions that support a predetermined expectation or hypothesis, ignoring or dismissing data that challenge that hypothesis ( Nickerson, 1998). The tendency to interpret data in support of the retaining attorney's position of advocacy may be intentional - that is, within conscious awareness and explicit, or it may be unintentional, outside of one's awareness, representing implicit bias. While some practitioners accept referrals from both sides in litigation, numerous uncontrollable factors converge in such a manner that one's practice may nevertheless become associated with one side. Such imbalance is not a reliable index of bias. With brief hypothetical scenarios, in this paper we discuss contextual factors that increase risk for retainer bias and problematic practice approaches that may be used to support one side in litigation, violating ethical principles, codes of conduct and guidelines for engaging in forensic work. We also discuss debiasing techniques recommended within the empirical literature and call on the subspecialty field of forensic neuropsychology to conduct research into retainer bias and other sources of opinion variability.</p>","PeriodicalId":8176,"journal":{"name":"Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142675123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1