首页 > 最新文献

Applied Animal Behaviour Science最新文献

英文 中文
Consistency of behavioral profiles in zebrafish: A machine learning approach to bold and shy individual differences 斑马鱼行为特征的一致性:大胆和害羞个体差异的机器学习方法
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106317
João Paulo Medeiros Mamede , Heloysa Araujo-Silva , Maria Clara Galvão-Pereira , Fúlvio Aurelio de Morais Freire , William J. Norton , Ana Carolina Luchiari

Behavioral variation among individuals is ascribed to the species' biology and the life history of each one. Many study areas consider individual differences, acknowledging their significant impact on fitness. Although boldness remains the most extensively studied behavioral dimension of individual differences in animals, ongoing debates persist regarding the evaluation of behavioral consistency over time and between contexts, as well as the determination of which features are crucial for delineating profiles. In this study, we investigated which behavioral traits explain the profiles of shyness and boldness and assessed their temporal and contextual consistency. For this, we divided zebrafish into bold and shy profiles by applying an emergence test (black-to-white entrance) three consecutive times with the same population of fish. The two groups formed (bold and shy) went through five different behavioural tests: novel tank, open field, black and white preference, aggressiveness, and sociability, which were employed twice, with an interval of 30 days. Bold animals showed less anxiety-like behaviour and higher aggressiveness compared to shy animals, and this pattern remained consistent over time for the two contexts. This suite of related behavious were considered the main factors to classify zebrafish into bold and shy profiles. In addition, the consistency appeared to be context dependent. The differences noted in the behavioural profiles allowed us to understand aspects of behavioural syndromes and how individuals behave when facing environmental challenges in different situations.

个体间的行为差异归因于物种的生物学特性和每个个体的生活史。许多研究领域都考虑到了个体差异,承认它们对适应性有重大影响。尽管胆量仍然是动物个体差异中研究最广泛的行为维度,但在评估不同时间和不同环境下的行为一致性,以及确定哪些特征对划分特征至关重要方面,一直存在争论。在这项研究中,我们调查了哪些行为特征可以解释害羞和大胆的特征,并评估了它们在时间和情境上的一致性。为此,我们在同一斑马鱼种群中连续进行了三次出现测试(从黑到白的入口),将斑马鱼分为大胆型和害羞型。这两组斑马鱼(胆大的和胆小的)分别进行了五种不同的行为测试:新水箱、开放场地、黑白偏好、攻击性和交际性,测试共进行两次,每次间隔 30 天。与害羞的动物相比,胆大的动物表现出较少的焦虑行为和较高的攻击性,而且这种模式在两种情况下长期保持一致。这一系列相关行为被认为是将斑马鱼分为大胆型和害羞型的主要因素。此外,这种一致性似乎与环境有关。行为特征的差异使我们能够了解行为综合症的各个方面,以及个体在不同情况下面对环境挑战时的行为方式。
{"title":"Consistency of behavioral profiles in zebrafish: A machine learning approach to bold and shy individual differences","authors":"João Paulo Medeiros Mamede ,&nbsp;Heloysa Araujo-Silva ,&nbsp;Maria Clara Galvão-Pereira ,&nbsp;Fúlvio Aurelio de Morais Freire ,&nbsp;William J. Norton ,&nbsp;Ana Carolina Luchiari","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106317","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106317","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Behavioral variation among individuals is ascribed to the species' biology and the life history of each one. Many study areas consider individual differences, acknowledging their significant impact on fitness. Although boldness remains the most extensively studied behavioral dimension of individual differences in animals, ongoing debates persist regarding the evaluation of behavioral consistency over time and between contexts, as well as the determination of which features are crucial for delineating profiles. In this study, we investigated which behavioral traits explain the profiles of shyness and boldness and assessed their temporal and contextual consistency. For this, we divided zebrafish into bold and shy profiles by applying an emergence test (black-to-white entrance) three consecutive times with the same population of fish. The two groups formed (bold and shy) went through five different behavioural tests: novel tank, open field, black and white preference, aggressiveness, and sociability, which were employed twice, with an interval of 30 days. Bold animals showed less anxiety-like behaviour and higher aggressiveness compared to shy animals, and this pattern remained consistent over time for the two contexts. This suite of related behavious were considered the main factors to classify zebrafish into bold and shy profiles. In addition, the consistency appeared to be context dependent. The differences noted in the behavioural profiles allowed us to understand aspects of behavioural syndromes and how individuals behave when facing environmental challenges in different situations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"276 ","pages":"Article 106317"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141412135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Altered reproductive behaviour in siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) after exposure to different level of turbidity derived from palm oil mill effluent 暹罗斗鱼(Betta splendens)暴露于棕榈油厂污水的不同浊度后生殖行为的改变
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106308
Ilham Zulfahmi , Agung Setia Batubara , Adli Waliul Perdana , Badratun Nafis , Tasya Alniza Putri , Furqan Maghfiriadi , Firman M. Nur , Kizar Ahmed Sumon , Mohammad Mahmudur Rahman

Increased turbidity due to Palm Oil Mill Effluent (POME) contamination has been reported in several water bodies. However, the impact of POME-derived turbidity on the reproductive behaviour of fish is still unexplored. Hence, this study was aimed to investigate the alteration in reproductive behavior of siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) after exposure to different levels of turbidity derived from POME. In total, 12 sexually mature pairs of siamese fighting fish were exposed to the different levels of turbidity, namely <1 NTU (control), 20 NTU (Treatment A), 40 NTU (Treatment B), and 60 NTU (Treatment C) with three replicates. Reproductive performance (total released egg, relative weight of released eggs, and nest area) and behaviour parameters (fin spreads, tail beats, chasing, leads, circling, and copulation) were measured. Results showed that an increase in turbidity (>40 NTU) due to POME contamination significantly decreased total released egg but did not have significant impact on relative weight of released eggs and nest area. Decreased total released eggs was occurred due to several alterations in the reproductive behaviour of siamese fighting fish during both the premating and mating stages. Duration of premating and mating stage was significantly decreased at turbidity level of 60 NTU. During premating stage, the number and average time of fin spreads behaviour at turbidity level of 60 NTU were significantly increased. In addition to, the number of chasing behaviour was significantly decreased with increasing turbidity levels. Furthermore, several alterations in reproductive behavior was also observed in mating stage, including increasing number and average time of lead, copulation interval, number of tail beat, average time of lead, and average time of circlings, followed by decreasing number of leads and number of circlings. In addition to, number of copulation and eggs per copulation showed an increasing and decreasing pattern at turbidity level of 40 and 60 NTU, respectively. The study suggests that reproductive behavior of fish might consider as alternative approach to support POME remediation management, mainly for turbidity parameters.

据报道,在一些水体中,棕榈油厂污水(POME)污染导致浑浊度增加。然而,POME 产生的浑浊度对鱼类繁殖行为的影响仍未得到探讨。因此,本研究旨在调查暹罗斗鱼(Betta splendens)在暴露于不同水平的 POME 浊度后的生殖行为变化。共有 12 对性成熟的暹罗斗鱼暴露于不同的浊度水平,即 1 NTU(对照)、20 NTU(处理 A)、40 NTU(处理 B)和 60 NTU(处理 C),共设三个重复。测量了繁殖性能(总产卵量、产卵相对重量和巢面积)和行为参数(展鳍、拍尾、追逐、引诱、盘旋和交配)。结果表明,POME 污染导致的浊度增加(40 NTU)显著降低了总释卵量,但对释卵相对重量和巢面积没有显著影响。暹罗斗鱼在交配前和交配阶段的生殖行为发生了一些变化,导致总产卵量减少。浊度为 60 NTU 时,预配和交配阶段的持续时间明显缩短。在交配前阶段,浊度为 60 NTU 时,鱼鳍张开的次数和平均时间明显增加。此外,随着浊度的增加,追逐行为的次数也明显减少。此外,在交配阶段还观察到生殖行为的一些变化,包括引尾次数和平均引尾时间、交配间隔、拍尾次数、平均引尾时间和平均绕圈时间的增加,随后引尾次数和绕圈次数的减少。此外,在浊度水平为 40 和 60 NTU 时,交配次数和每次交配产卵量分别呈上升和下降趋势。研究表明,鱼类的繁殖行为可作为支持 POME 修复管理的替代方法,主要是针对浊度参数。
{"title":"Altered reproductive behaviour in siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) after exposure to different level of turbidity derived from palm oil mill effluent","authors":"Ilham Zulfahmi ,&nbsp;Agung Setia Batubara ,&nbsp;Adli Waliul Perdana ,&nbsp;Badratun Nafis ,&nbsp;Tasya Alniza Putri ,&nbsp;Furqan Maghfiriadi ,&nbsp;Firman M. Nur ,&nbsp;Kizar Ahmed Sumon ,&nbsp;Mohammad Mahmudur Rahman","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106308","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106308","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Increased turbidity due to Palm Oil Mill Effluent (POME) contamination has been reported in several water bodies. However, the impact of POME-derived turbidity on the reproductive behaviour of fish is still unexplored. Hence, this study was aimed to investigate the alteration in reproductive behavior of siamese fighting fish (<em>Betta splendens</em>) after exposure to different levels of turbidity derived from POME. In total, 12 sexually mature pairs of siamese fighting fish were exposed to the different levels of turbidity, namely &lt;1 NTU (control), 20 NTU (Treatment A), 40 NTU (Treatment B), and 60 NTU (Treatment C) with three replicates. Reproductive performance (total released egg, relative weight of released eggs, and nest area) and behaviour parameters (fin spreads, tail beats, chasing, leads, circling, and copulation) were measured. Results showed that an increase in turbidity (&gt;40 NTU) due to POME contamination significantly decreased total released egg but did not have significant impact on relative weight of released eggs and nest area. Decreased total released eggs was occurred due to several alterations in the reproductive behaviour of siamese fighting fish during both the premating and mating stages. Duration of premating and mating stage was significantly decreased at turbidity level of 60 NTU. During premating stage, the number and average time of fin spreads behaviour at turbidity level of 60 NTU were significantly increased. In addition to, the number of chasing behaviour was significantly decreased with increasing turbidity levels. Furthermore, several alterations in reproductive behavior was also observed in mating stage, including increasing number and average time of lead, copulation interval, number of tail beat, average time of lead, and average time of circlings, followed by decreasing number of leads and number of circlings. In addition to, number of copulation and eggs per copulation showed an increasing and decreasing pattern at turbidity level of 40 and 60 NTU, respectively. The study suggests that reproductive behavior of fish might consider as alternative approach to support POME remediation management, mainly for turbidity parameters.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"276 ","pages":"Article 106308"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141303146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistent individual differences in behavior among beef cattle in handling contexts and social-feed preference testing 肉牛在处理环境和社会饲料偏好测试中的行为存在一致的个体差异
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106315
Maggie Creamer, Kristina Horback

Consistent individual differences (CIDs) in animal behavior, also known as personality or temperament, are measured in a variety of assays and are presumed to represent relatively stable traits of individual animals. Because these behaviors are presumed to represent stable traits, repetition of assays over short and long-term time frames and contexts is crucial; such repetitions are not commonly achieved in literature regarding the topic of CIDs, especially not in a singular study. In beef cattle, CIDs are often related to feeding and social preferences, which is relevant for extensively managed herds of ruminants. This study investigated the consistency of behavior among 50 breeding Angus x Hereford cows across repetitions within one year and over one year during three distinct contexts: handling and isolation in a chute corral system, choice in a social-feed tradeoff test, and response in a novel bucket approach test. We found that cows were consistent across short-term (assessed with repeatabilities [R]) and long-term (assessed with correlations across multivariate models [r]) timeframes in durations of behaviors exhibited in the management context (e.g. while handled [R = 0.60, r = 0.39], while traversing a cement chute [R = 0.69, r = 0.67] and an open squeeze stall [R = 0.76, r = 0.85]). Repeatable behaviors from the handling and isolation in chute system context were input into PCA with varimax rotation. Behaviors loaded onto three principal components that explained 66 % of the variance in the data and distinguished cows along axes of activity, fearfulness and excitability. Cows that were deemed less active (p = 0.010) and less excitable (p = 0.039) chose to approach the supplement bucket over gaining proximity to conspecifics in a social-feed tradeoff test. Even though the cows were not restrained in a squeeze stall, consistent individual differences in behavior across short (within year) and long-term (between years) timeframes were found. This suggests that future research can use unrestrained methods to evaluate cattle temperament, which could improve animal welfare and safety of handlers during experiments. Passive and less excitable beef cattle were more feed-centric, indicating that CIDs in behavior assays could potentially be used to predict feeding behavior because of an underlying stable trait in cows that ties response to handling and management with feed choice in a social-feed tradeoff task.

动物行为中的一致个体差异(CIDs),也称为个性或气质,是通过各种测定方法测量的,被认为代表了动物个体相对稳定的特征。由于这些行为被假定为代表稳定的性状,因此在短期和长期的时间和环境中重复进行测定至关重要;在有关 CID 的文献中,这种重复并不常见,尤其是在单项研究中。在肉牛中,CID 通常与采食和社会偏好有关,这与广泛管理的反刍动物群相关。本研究调查了 50 头育种安格斯×赫里福德奶牛在一年内和一年以上在三种不同情况下重复出现的行为一致性:溜槽畜栏系统中的处理和隔离、社交-饲料权衡测试中的选择以及新型桶接近测试中的反应。我们发现,奶牛在短期(以重复率[R]评估)和长期(以多元模型的相关性[r]评估)时间范围内,在管理情境中表现出的行为持续时间是一致的(例如,被处理时[R = 0.60,r = 0.39],穿过水泥槽时[R = 0.69,r = 0.67],以及在开放式挤压栏时[R = 0.76,r = 0.85])。将滑道系统中搬运和隔离过程中的可重复行为输入 PCA,并进行方差旋转。这些行为被加载到三个主成分上,解释了数据中 66% 的方差,并沿着活动、恐惧和兴奋轴对奶牛进行了区分。在社交-饲料权衡测试中,被认为活动量较少(p = 0.010)和兴奋性较低(p = 0.039)的奶牛选择靠近补充桶,而不是靠近同类。尽管奶牛没有被限制在挤压式牛栏中,但在短期(年内)和长期(年与年之间)的时间范围内,仍发现了一致的个体行为差异。这表明,未来的研究可以使用不受约束的方法来评估牛的性情,从而改善动物福利和实验过程中操作人员的安全。被动和不太兴奋的肉牛更以饲料为中心,这表明行为测定中的 CID 有可能用于预测采食行为,因为奶牛的潜在稳定性状将对处理和管理的反应与社会-饲料权衡任务中的饲料选择联系在一起。
{"title":"Consistent individual differences in behavior among beef cattle in handling contexts and social-feed preference testing","authors":"Maggie Creamer,&nbsp;Kristina Horback","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106315","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Consistent individual differences (CIDs) in animal behavior, also known as personality or temperament, are measured in a variety of assays and are presumed to represent relatively stable traits of individual animals. Because these behaviors are presumed to represent stable traits, repetition of assays over short and long-term time frames and contexts is crucial; such repetitions are not commonly achieved in literature regarding the topic of CIDs, especially not in a singular study. In beef cattle, CIDs are often related to feeding and social preferences, which is relevant for extensively managed herds of ruminants. This study investigated the consistency of behavior among 50 breeding Angus x Hereford cows across repetitions within one year and over one year during three distinct contexts: handling and isolation in a chute corral system, choice in a social-feed tradeoff test, and response in a novel bucket approach test. We found that cows were consistent across short-term (assessed with repeatabilities [R]) and long-term (assessed with correlations across multivariate models [<em>r</em>]) timeframes in durations of behaviors exhibited in the management context (e.g. while handled [R = 0.60, <em>r</em> = 0.39], while traversing a cement chute [R = 0.69, <em>r</em> = 0.67] and an open squeeze stall [R = 0.76, <em>r</em> = 0.85]). Repeatable behaviors from the handling and isolation in chute system context were input into PCA with varimax rotation. Behaviors loaded onto three principal components that explained 66 % of the variance in the data and distinguished cows along axes of activity, fearfulness and excitability. Cows that were deemed less active (p = 0.010) and less excitable (p = 0.039) chose to approach the supplement bucket over gaining proximity to conspecifics in a social-feed tradeoff test. Even though the cows were not restrained in a squeeze stall, consistent individual differences in behavior across short (within year) and long-term (between years) timeframes were found. This suggests that future research can use unrestrained methods to evaluate cattle temperament, which could improve animal welfare and safety of handlers during experiments. Passive and less excitable beef cattle were more feed-centric, indicating that CIDs in behavior assays could potentially be used to predict feeding behavior because of an underlying stable trait in cows that ties response to handling and management with feed choice in a social-feed tradeoff task.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"276 ","pages":"Article 106315"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159124001631/pdfft?md5=de3d109504c303df54fe855f5cf48426&pid=1-s2.0-S0168159124001631-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141323980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of rehabilitation exercise on locomotion performance of captive Bengal slow loris 康复运动对圈养孟加拉慢猴运动能力的影响
IF 2.2 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106304
Xing Bolin , Liao Linhong , Han Dongchen , Liu Jiayong , Zhao Kairui , Fan Yumai , Teng Ping , Zhang Yan , Huang Anqi , Zeng Changjun , Xie Meng , Ni Qingyong

The Bengal slow loris (Nycticebus bengalensis) is one of the most confiscated and rescued primate species in China. These animals are often subject to physical issues due to poor husbandry practices in captivity. Many individuals rescued from the illegal wildlife trade are confined to captivity, leading to decreased locomotion ability and increased abnormal behavior. Recognizing the potential benefits of rehabilitation exercises, we conducted outdoor rehabilitation activities in forest environments to improve the locomotion performance of rescued Bengal slow lorises, and evaluated how individual characteristics and conditions in captivity influenced their locomotion recovery. The results of the rehabilitation exercises were promising, showing that the animals exhibited more elaborate route selection, improved action scores, longer distances traveled, and reduced periods of inactivity. Bengal slow lorises housed in larger cages and those in short-term captivity demonstrated better overall performance. Additionally, animals displaying stereotypical behavior achieved higher action scores, while male lorises had longer periods of motionlessness compared to females. These findings highlight the positive impact of rehabilitation exercises on the locomotion abilities of captive Bengal slow lorises, emphasizing the importance of enhancing animal welfare and implementing scientifically informed strategies for the successful reintroduction of pre-released animals.

孟加拉慢猴(Nycticebus bengalensis)是中国没收和救助最多的灵长类动物之一。由于人工饲养方式不当,这些动物经常会出现身体问题。许多从非法野生动物贸易中获救的个体被圈养,导致运动能力下降,异常行为增多。认识到康复训练的潜在益处,我们在森林环境中开展了户外康复活动,以改善获救孟加拉慢蜥的运动表现,并评估了个体特征和圈养条件对其运动恢复的影响。康复训练的结果令人欣慰,这些动物表现出更精细的路线选择、更好的动作评分、更长的行进距离以及更短的非活动时间。饲养在较大笼子里的孟加拉慢蜥和短期圈养的孟加拉慢蜥的整体表现更好。此外,表现出刻板行为的动物获得了更高的行动评分,而与雌性相比,雄性慢蜥的静止不动时间更长。这些研究结果凸显了康复训练对圈养孟加拉短尾猴运动能力的积极影响,强调了提高动物福利和实施科学合理的策略对成功放归前期动物的重要性。
{"title":"Effect of rehabilitation exercise on locomotion performance of captive Bengal slow loris","authors":"Xing Bolin ,&nbsp;Liao Linhong ,&nbsp;Han Dongchen ,&nbsp;Liu Jiayong ,&nbsp;Zhao Kairui ,&nbsp;Fan Yumai ,&nbsp;Teng Ping ,&nbsp;Zhang Yan ,&nbsp;Huang Anqi ,&nbsp;Zeng Changjun ,&nbsp;Xie Meng ,&nbsp;Ni Qingyong","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106304","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106304","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The Bengal slow loris (<em>Nycticebus bengalensis</em>) is one of the most confiscated and rescued primate species in China. These animals are often subject to physical issues due to poor husbandry practices in captivity. Many individuals rescued from the illegal wildlife trade are confined to captivity, leading to decreased locomotion ability and increased abnormal behavior. Recognizing the potential benefits of rehabilitation exercises, we conducted outdoor rehabilitation activities in forest environments to improve the locomotion performance of rescued Bengal slow lorises, and evaluated how individual characteristics and conditions in captivity influenced their locomotion recovery. The results of the rehabilitation exercises were promising, showing that the animals exhibited more elaborate route selection, improved action scores, longer distances traveled, and reduced periods of inactivity. Bengal slow lorises housed in larger cages and those in short-term captivity demonstrated better overall performance. Additionally, animals displaying stereotypical behavior achieved higher action scores, while male lorises had longer periods of motionlessness compared to females. These findings highlight the positive impact of rehabilitation exercises on the locomotion abilities of captive Bengal slow lorises, emphasizing the importance of enhancing animal welfare and implementing scientifically informed strategies for the successful reintroduction of pre-released animals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"276 ","pages":"Article 106304"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141281196","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Counterconditioning-based interventions for companion dog behavioural modification: A systematic review. 以反条件疗法为基础的伴侣犬行为矫正干预:系统综述。
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106305
Joanna Shnookal, Deanna Tepper, Tiffani Howell, Pauleen Bennett

Undesirable companion dog behaviours negatively impact the lives of both owners and dogs. Therefore, it is imperative that behaviour modification interventions are researched to establish effectiveness. Counterconditioning is commonly recommended for modification of a broad range of undesirable dog behaviours, however, to date, no comprehensive review has investigated the effectiveness of this technique. This study aimed to evaluate the existing literature on counterconditioning interventions for dog behaviour modification, adopting broad inclusion criteria. Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, 12 peer-reviewed papers and 2 dissertations were identified. A review of these 14 studies found that there were discrepancies in the definitions and applications of counterconditioning-based interventions. These discrepancies were based on the contingency with which dogs received reinforcement and could be categorised into classical and operant counterconditioning methodologies. The majority of the studies resulted in positive outcomes, particularly when addressing undesirable kennel behaviours and aggressive behaviours towards other dogs or household visitors. However, separation-related behaviours were found to be more resistant to change. Furthermore, heterogeneity in intervention techniques, reliance on expert implementation or instruction, and small sample sizes, limit the generalisability of the results. Our review confirms the effectiveness of counterconditioning in reducing undesirable dog behaviours, however it is essential that future studies clearly detail all aspects of the intervention, such that methodologies can be accurately understood, applied, and compared. Future research should also focus on increasing sample sizes and including physiological measures of stress of participating dogs.

伴侣犬的不良行为会对主人和狗狗的生活造成负面影响。因此,必须对行为矫正干预措施进行研究,以确定其有效性。反条件反射通常被推荐用于改变狗狗的各种不良行为,但迄今为止,还没有对这种技术的有效性进行过全面研究。本研究采用广泛的纳入标准,旨在评估有关反条件疗法干预犬行为矫正的现有文献。根据《系统综述和元分析首选报告项目》(PRISMA)指南,共确定了 12 篇经同行评审的论文和 2 篇学位论文。对这 14 项研究的综述发现,基于反调节的干预措施在定义和应用上存在差异。这些差异是根据狗接受强化的条件而定的,可分为经典反条件法和操作性反条件法。大多数研究都取得了积极的成果,尤其是在处理不良犬舍行为和对其他狗狗或家中访客的攻击行为时。然而,研究发现与分离有关的行为更难改变。此外,干预技术的异质性、对专家实施或指导的依赖以及样本量较小等因素也限制了研究结果的普遍性。我们的综述证实了反条件疗法在减少狗的不良行为方面的有效性,但今后的研究必须明确详细地说明干预的各个方面,以便准确地理解、应用和比较干预方法。未来的研究还应注重增加样本量,并对参与研究的狗进行压力生理测量。
{"title":"Counterconditioning-based interventions for companion dog behavioural modification: A systematic review.","authors":"Joanna Shnookal,&nbsp;Deanna Tepper,&nbsp;Tiffani Howell,&nbsp;Pauleen Bennett","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106305","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106305","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Undesirable companion dog behaviours negatively impact the lives of both owners and dogs. Therefore, it is imperative that behaviour modification interventions are researched to establish effectiveness. Counterconditioning is commonly recommended for modification of a broad range of undesirable dog behaviours, however, to date, no comprehensive review has investigated the effectiveness of this technique. This study aimed to evaluate the existing literature on counterconditioning interventions for dog behaviour modification, adopting broad inclusion criteria. Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, 12 peer-reviewed papers and 2 dissertations were identified. A review of these 14 studies found that there were discrepancies in the definitions and applications of counterconditioning-based interventions. These discrepancies were based on the contingency with which dogs received reinforcement and could be categorised into classical and operant counterconditioning methodologies. The majority of the studies resulted in positive outcomes, particularly when addressing undesirable kennel behaviours and aggressive behaviours towards other dogs or household visitors. However, separation-related behaviours were found to be more resistant to change. Furthermore, heterogeneity in intervention techniques, reliance on expert implementation or instruction, and small sample sizes, limit the generalisability of the results. Our review confirms the effectiveness of counterconditioning in reducing undesirable dog behaviours, however it is essential that future studies clearly detail all aspects of the intervention, such that methodologies can be accurately understood, applied, and compared. Future research should also focus on increasing sample sizes and including physiological measures of stress of participating dogs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"276 ","pages":"Article 106305"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159124001539/pdfft?md5=9eef926759b6ed794a669a4b207dc834&pid=1-s2.0-S0168159124001539-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141279621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring affect-related attention bias to emotionally valenced visual stimuli in horses 测量马匹对有情绪价值的视觉刺激的情感相关注意偏差
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106303
Sarah Kappel , Marco A.Ramirez Montes De Oca , Sarah Collins , Katherine Herborn , Michael Mendl , Carole Fureix
<div><p>Negative affect appears to mediate animals’ attention to competing emotional stimuli (e.g., threatening vs. non-threatening conspecific face images), similarly to anxiety-related enhanced attention to social threat reported in humans. To investigate this ‘attention bias’ (AB, i.e., the differential attention allocation to certain types of information over others) in horses, we developed a visual AB test assessing horses’ attention towards image pairs showing unfamiliar conspecifics’ facial expressions indicating, a) negative (social threat), b) more neutral (at rest), and c) positive (food anticipation) situations. We predicted that horses exhibit greater attention to negative compared to neutral or positive face images (as a normal adaptive response), and that horses in negative affective states (inferred from validated welfare indices comprising direct (health, behaviour) and indirect (housing, management) measures summarised as individual welfare scores and subscores reflecting health, social and environmental aspects) show greater AB to negative face stimuli and all images overall. Comparing AB to positive <em>versus</em> neutral social stimuli is rarely considered in AB studies, we therefore explored horses’ AB responses without <em>a priori</em> predictions. Over six trials, 44 horses from three facilities were shown stimulus pairs (negative/neutral, negative/positive, positive/neutral) presented simultaneously on two projector screens. Attention was assessed as absolute attention duration to each image, the proportion of time the negative/positive stimulus was attended to relative to the other stimulus, and overall attention (i.e., duration of head turns towards both stimuli combined). AB to stimulus type, side, effects of facility and individual characteristics (welfare and subscores, age) was analysed using linear and generalised mixed-effect models. Against our predictions, horses attended to the images within the three stimulus pairs for similar lengths of time (negative-neutral: W=1870.5, p=0.2572; negative-positive: W=2542.5, p=0.9296; positive-neutral: W=1762.5, p=0.1019). Due to Covid-19 interruptions, our sample size was lower than our estimated required number (N=113). Still, lower welfare (X<sup>2<sub>1</sub></sup>=4.71, p=0.03) and health scores (X<sup>2<sub>1</sub></sup>=4.13, p=0.04) significantly predicted shorter attention to the negative face stimuli, possibly reflecting threat avoidance previously reported in other animals. We found significant facility effects on overall attention to the stimuli (X<sup>2<sub>2</sub></sup>=77.42, p<0.001), likely due to varying yard-specific conditions (e.g., lighting, noise). This highlights that external influences on visual attention require consideration when conducting cognitive tests at different testing sites. Further methodological investigation (e.g., test cue suitability, perceptual processing of computer-generated images; test stimuli familiarity; individual differen
消极情绪似乎会介导动物对竞争性情绪刺激(例如,具有威胁性与不具有威胁性的同类面部图像)的注意,这与人类报告的焦虑相关的对社会威胁的强化注意类似。为了研究马匹的这种 "注意偏差"(AB,即对某些类型信息的注意分配不同于其他类型信息),我们开发了一种视觉 AB 测试,评估马匹对显示陌生同种动物面部表情的图像对的注意,这些表情分别表示:a)负面(社交威胁);b)较中性(休息);c)正面(食物期待)。我们预测,与中性或积极的面部图像相比,马匹会表现出更多的注意力(作为一种正常的适应性反应),而处于消极情绪状态的马匹(根据经过验证的福利指数推断,这些指数包括直接(健康、行为)和间接(饲养、管理)措施,概括为个人福利分数和反映健康、社会和环境方面的子分数)会对消极面部刺激和所有图像表现出更多的 AB。在AB研究中,很少考虑对正面与中性社会刺激的AB反应进行比较,因此我们在没有先验预测的情况下探讨了马匹的AB反应。在六次试验中,来自三个设施的 44 匹马在两个投影仪屏幕上同时看到了刺激对(负面/中性、负面/正面、正面/中性)。注意力的评估包括对每个图像的绝对注意持续时间、相对于另一个刺激物的消极/积极刺激物被注意的时间比例以及总体注意(即对两个刺激物的头部转动持续时间的总和)。使用线性和广义混合效应模型分析了AB对刺激类型、侧面、设施影响和个体特征(福利和分值、年龄)的影响。与我们的预测相反,马匹对三对刺激中的图像的注意时间长度相似(消极-中性,W=1870.5,p=0.05):W=1870.5,p=0.2572;负-正:W=2542.5,p=0.9296;正-中性:W=1762.5,p=0.9296:W=1762.5,p=0.1019)。由于 Covid-19 的中断,我们的样本量低于预计的所需数量(N=113)。尽管如此,较低的福利评分(X21=4.71,p=0.03)和健康评分(X21=4.13,p=0.04)显著预测了对负面面部刺激的较短注意力,这可能反映了之前在其他动物身上报道的威胁回避。我们发现,设施对刺激物的总体注意力有明显影响(X22=77.42,p<0.001),这可能是由于不同的庭院条件(如照明、噪音)造成的。这突出表明,在不同测试场地进行认知测试时,需要考虑视觉注意力的外部影响因素。要评估 AB 作为马匹情绪价值指标的潜力,还需要进一步的方法学研究(如测试线索的适宜性、计算机生成图像的感知处理;测试刺激物的熟悉程度;个体差异)。
{"title":"Measuring affect-related attention bias to emotionally valenced visual stimuli in horses","authors":"Sarah Kappel ,&nbsp;Marco A.Ramirez Montes De Oca ,&nbsp;Sarah Collins ,&nbsp;Katherine Herborn ,&nbsp;Michael Mendl ,&nbsp;Carole Fureix","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106303","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106303","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;div&gt;&lt;p&gt;Negative affect appears to mediate animals’ attention to competing emotional stimuli (e.g., threatening vs. non-threatening conspecific face images), similarly to anxiety-related enhanced attention to social threat reported in humans. To investigate this ‘attention bias’ (AB, i.e., the differential attention allocation to certain types of information over others) in horses, we developed a visual AB test assessing horses’ attention towards image pairs showing unfamiliar conspecifics’ facial expressions indicating, a) negative (social threat), b) more neutral (at rest), and c) positive (food anticipation) situations. We predicted that horses exhibit greater attention to negative compared to neutral or positive face images (as a normal adaptive response), and that horses in negative affective states (inferred from validated welfare indices comprising direct (health, behaviour) and indirect (housing, management) measures summarised as individual welfare scores and subscores reflecting health, social and environmental aspects) show greater AB to negative face stimuli and all images overall. Comparing AB to positive &lt;em&gt;versus&lt;/em&gt; neutral social stimuli is rarely considered in AB studies, we therefore explored horses’ AB responses without &lt;em&gt;a priori&lt;/em&gt; predictions. Over six trials, 44 horses from three facilities were shown stimulus pairs (negative/neutral, negative/positive, positive/neutral) presented simultaneously on two projector screens. Attention was assessed as absolute attention duration to each image, the proportion of time the negative/positive stimulus was attended to relative to the other stimulus, and overall attention (i.e., duration of head turns towards both stimuli combined). AB to stimulus type, side, effects of facility and individual characteristics (welfare and subscores, age) was analysed using linear and generalised mixed-effect models. Against our predictions, horses attended to the images within the three stimulus pairs for similar lengths of time (negative-neutral: W=1870.5, p=0.2572; negative-positive: W=2542.5, p=0.9296; positive-neutral: W=1762.5, p=0.1019). Due to Covid-19 interruptions, our sample size was lower than our estimated required number (N=113). Still, lower welfare (X&lt;sup&gt;2&lt;sub&gt;1&lt;/sub&gt;&lt;/sup&gt;=4.71, p=0.03) and health scores (X&lt;sup&gt;2&lt;sub&gt;1&lt;/sub&gt;&lt;/sup&gt;=4.13, p=0.04) significantly predicted shorter attention to the negative face stimuli, possibly reflecting threat avoidance previously reported in other animals. We found significant facility effects on overall attention to the stimuli (X&lt;sup&gt;2&lt;sub&gt;2&lt;/sub&gt;&lt;/sup&gt;=77.42, p&lt;0.001), likely due to varying yard-specific conditions (e.g., lighting, noise). This highlights that external influences on visual attention require consideration when conducting cognitive tests at different testing sites. Further methodological investigation (e.g., test cue suitability, perceptual processing of computer-generated images; test stimuli familiarity; individual differen","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"275 ","pages":"Article 106303"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159124001515/pdfft?md5=1c1ab2e2e5069e9fc3b01ba477f8497b&pid=1-s2.0-S0168159124001515-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141244546","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strain and age but not maternal feeding of n-3 fatty acids affect the performance of laying hen offspring in a series of associative learning tasks 品系和年龄(而非母体饲喂的 n-3 脂肪酸)会影响蛋鸡后代在一系列联想学习任务中的表现
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106297
Rosemary H. Whittle , Elijah G. Kiarie , Alexandra Harlander , Tina M. Widowski

Commercial poultry are typically fed diets with fatty acid compositions that differ considerably from wild-type diets in the omega-6 to omega-3 FA ratio (n-6: n-3). Maternal dietary n-3 FA have been shown to affect offspring brain development and cognition in various avian and mammalian species. This study explored the effects of feeding egg-type layer breeders with flaxseed on the performance of their female offspring in a three-stage discrimination task. Shaver White and ISA Brown breeders were fed either a control diet (n-6: n-3 = 14.7:1) or a flaxseed-enriched diet (n-6: n-3 = 5.3:1) throughout the rearing and laying phases. At 30 and 36 weeks of age, eggs were collected, incubated and hatched. Offspring were housed in enriched floor pens and received a control diet. At 12 (pullets) and 33 (hens) weeks of age, three learning tasks were conducted: behavioural shaping (N=437), discrimination test (N=350), and reversal learning (N=213). Food rewards (sweetcorn and raisins) were placed in the “reward” cup. Behavioural shaping consisted of five phases (P): P1 clear cup, P2 coloured cup, P3 coloured cup half-covered with a lid, P4 coloured cup three quarter covered with a lid, and P5 coloured cup covered with a lid. One cup, alternating between blue and green, was presented. Chickens had to eat the food reward from the cup in 4/5 trials to pass each phase. Two cups (green and blue) were presented during discrimination testing and reversal learning with food only in the predetermined “reward” cup. Learning criterion was choosing the correct cup in 4/5 trials for three consecutive days in discrimination testing and for two consecutive days in reversal learning. Data were analysed using survival class analyses. The hazard ratio (HR), 95% confidence intervals (CI) [LL, UL] and p-values to determine the probability of reaching learning criteria are reported. In P1 of behavioural shaping, Brown hens were likelier to reach criteria (HR=2.28, 95% CI [1.48, 3.55], p<0.001). For overall success in behavioural shaping (P1–5), hens were likelier to reach criteria than pullets (HR=2.42, 95% CI [1.50, 3.91], p<0.001). White chickens were likelier to reach criteria in the discrimination test than Brown chickens (HR=1.03, 95% CI [1.01, 1.75], p=0.04). Finally, pullets were likelier to reach criteria in reversal learning than hens (HR=0.44, 95% CI [0.23, 0.86], p=0.016). These results show that age, strain, and their interaction, but not maternal n-3 FA diet, affected the success of chickens in learning tasks.

商业家禽通常饲喂脂肪酸组成与野生型家禽有很大差异的日粮,即ω-6 和 ω-3 脂肪酸的比例(n-6:n-3)。在各种鸟类和哺乳动物中,母体膳食中的 n-3 脂肪酸已被证明会影响后代的大脑发育和认知能力。本研究探讨了用亚麻籽喂养蛋鸡种鸡对其雌性后代在三阶段辨别任务中表现的影响。在整个育雏和产蛋阶段,分别饲喂对照日粮(n-6: n-3 = 14.7:1)或富含亚麻籽的日粮(n-6: n-3 = 5.3:1)。在 30 周龄和 36 周龄时,收集、孵化和孵化鸡蛋。后代饲养在强化地板围栏中,并摄入对照组日粮。12周龄(小母鸡)和33周龄(母鸡)时,进行三项学习任务:行为塑造(437只)、辨别力测试(350只)和逆向学习(213只)。在 "奖励 "杯中放置食物奖励(甜角和葡萄干)。行为塑造包括五个阶段(P):P1 为透明杯,P2 为彩色杯,P3 为盖子盖住一半的彩色杯,P4 为盖子盖住四分之三的彩色杯,P5 为盖子盖住的彩色杯。其中,蓝色和绿色交替出现。小鸡必须在 4/5 次试验中吃掉杯中的食物奖励,才能通过每个阶段。在辨别测试和逆向学习期间,会出现两个杯子(绿色和蓝色),食物只放在预先确定的 "奖励 "杯中。学习标准是在辨别测试中连续三天在 4/5 次试验中选择正确的杯子,在逆转学习中连续两天选择正确的杯子。数据采用生存分类分析法进行分析。报告了达到学习标准的概率的危险比(HR)、95%置信区间(CI)[LL, UL]和P值。在行为塑造的第一阶段,褐壳雌鸡更容易达到标准(HR=2.28,95% CI [1.48,3.55],p<0.001)。在行为塑造的总体成功率方面(P1-5),母鸡比小鸡更容易达到标准(HR=2.42,95% CI [1.50,3.91],p<0.001)。白羽鸡比褐羽鸡更容易在辨别力测试中达到标准(HR=1.03,95% CI [1.01,1.75],p=0.04)。最后,小母鸡比母鸡更容易在逆转学习中达到标准(HR=0.44,95% CI [0.23,0.86],p=0.016)。这些结果表明,年龄、品系以及它们之间的相互作用,而不是母鸡的 n-3 脂肪酸饮食,会影响鸡在学习任务中的成功率。
{"title":"Strain and age but not maternal feeding of n-3 fatty acids affect the performance of laying hen offspring in a series of associative learning tasks","authors":"Rosemary H. Whittle ,&nbsp;Elijah G. Kiarie ,&nbsp;Alexandra Harlander ,&nbsp;Tina M. Widowski","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106297","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Commercial poultry are typically fed diets with fatty acid compositions that differ considerably from wild-type diets in the omega-6 to omega-3 FA ratio (n-6: n-3). Maternal dietary n-3 FA have been shown to affect offspring brain development and cognition in various avian and mammalian species. This study explored the effects of feeding egg-type layer breeders with flaxseed on the performance of their female offspring in a three-stage discrimination task. Shaver White and ISA Brown breeders were fed either a control diet (n-6: n-3 = 14.7:1) or a flaxseed-enriched diet (n-6: n-3 = 5.3:1) throughout the rearing and laying phases. At 30 and 36 weeks of age, eggs were collected, incubated and hatched. Offspring were housed in enriched floor pens and received a control diet. At 12 (pullets) and 33 (hens) weeks of age, three learning tasks were conducted: behavioural shaping (N=437), discrimination test (N=350), and reversal learning (N=213). Food rewards (sweetcorn and raisins) were placed in the “reward” cup. Behavioural shaping consisted of five phases (P): P1 clear cup, P2 coloured cup, P3 coloured cup half-covered with a lid, P4 coloured cup three quarter covered with a lid, and P5 coloured cup covered with a lid. One cup, alternating between blue and green, was presented. Chickens had to eat the food reward from the cup in 4/5 trials to pass each phase. Two cups (green and blue) were presented during discrimination testing and reversal learning with food only in the predetermined “reward” cup. Learning criterion was choosing the correct cup in 4/5 trials for three consecutive days in discrimination testing and for two consecutive days in reversal learning. Data were analysed using survival class analyses. The hazard ratio (HR), 95% confidence intervals (CI) [LL, UL] and p-values to determine the probability of reaching learning criteria are reported. In P1 of behavioural shaping, Brown hens were likelier to reach criteria (HR=2.28, 95% CI [1.48, 3.55], p&lt;0.001). For overall success in behavioural shaping (P1–5), hens were likelier to reach criteria than pullets (HR=2.42, 95% CI [1.50, 3.91], p&lt;0.001). White chickens were likelier to reach criteria in the discrimination test than Brown chickens (HR=1.03, 95% CI [1.01, 1.75], p=0.04). Finally, pullets were likelier to reach criteria in reversal learning than hens (HR=0.44, 95% CI [0.23, 0.86], p=0.016). These results show that age, strain, and their interaction, but not maternal n-3 FA diet, affected the success of chickens in learning tasks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"275 ","pages":"Article 106297"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S016815912400145X/pdfft?md5=06368eac62f64805d02c4903e825ea76&pid=1-s2.0-S016815912400145X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141244547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sound feeding signal for gestating sows: Evaluation of an individual learning strategy and its beneficial effects in groups. 妊娠母猪的声音饲喂信号:评估个体学习策略及其在群体中的有益效果。
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106302
Anna Blanc, Clémentine Deroiné, Caroline Clouard, Charlotte Gaillard

Competition for feed in a group of gestating sows leads to aggression around feeding stations, which has a negative impact on their welfare. This study investigates the potential of teaching gestating sows an individual sound signal to reduce aggression resulting from competition for feed access, and thus improve their welfare in a group. A total of 32 sows were used. In a test room, “learning” sows (n = 16) went through 4 individual learning phases (27 days in total) to associate the individual sound signal with an invitation to feed from a one-way feeding station and to discriminate this individual sound signal from other unknown sound signals. After the learning phases, sows were subjected to a 3-day evaluation phase in groups of 4 sows. The "naive" sows (n = 16) were also introduced to the test room individually for 18 days, and in groups of 4 for 3 days without following the learning procedure. Learning sows correctly responded to 100% of their individual sound signal after only 8 days of individual learning, suggesting that they successfully associated the sound signal with feed access. Distinguishing between different sounds was harder as shown by only 18.8% of success after an unknown sound emission at the end of the individual learning phases. Naïve sows reduced the time spent in the feeder compared to learning sows (P < 0.001). On the second day of the group phase, learning sows were less aggressive than naïve sows (P < 0.05). Compared to high-ranking sows, low-ranking sows displayed a reduced number of spontaneous approaches to the feeder during the last individual learning phase (P < 0.001), and higher success rates in the group phase (P < 0.05). The study suggests that, for group-housed sows fed by an individual feeder, teaching sows an individual sound signal can modify their feeding and social behaviors, enhancing their overall well-being during feeding time. Furthermore, the results suggest that this individual learning may be particularly beneficial for low-ranking sows.

一群后备母猪争抢饲料会导致在饲喂站周围发生攻击行为,从而对母猪的福利产生负面影响。本研究探讨了向后备母猪传授个体声音信号的可能性,以减少因争抢饲料而产生的攻击行为,从而改善它们在群体中的福利。共使用了 32 头母猪。在试验室中,"学习 "母猪(n = 16)经过 4 个学习阶段(共 27 天),将个体声音信号与单向饲喂站的喂食邀请联系起来,并将个体声音信号与其他未知声音信号区分开来。学习阶段结束后,母猪将接受为期 3 天的评估阶段,每 4 头母猪为一组。天真 "母猪(n = 16)也单独进入试验室学习 18 天,每 4 头母猪为一组,学习 3 天,但不按照学习程序进行。学习母猪仅在单独学习 8 天后就能对各自的声音信号做出 100% 的正确反应,这表明它们已成功地将声音信号与获取饲料联系起来。在个体学习阶段结束时,母猪对未知声音信号的成功率仅为 18.8%,这表明区分不同的声音信号较为困难。与学习母猪相比,新手母猪减少了在饲喂器中停留的时间(P < 0.001)。在小组学习阶段的第二天,学习型母猪的攻击性低于初学型母猪(P <0.05)。与高等级母猪相比,低等级母猪在最后一个个体学习阶段自发接近饲喂器的次数减少(P <0.001),而在群体阶段成功率更高(P <0.05)。该研究表明,对于由个体饲喂器饲喂的群居母猪来说,教母猪学习个体声音信号可以改变它们的饲喂和社交行为,提高它们在饲喂期间的整体健康水平。此外,研究结果表明,这种个体学习可能对低等级母猪特别有益。
{"title":"Sound feeding signal for gestating sows: Evaluation of an individual learning strategy and its beneficial effects in groups.","authors":"Anna Blanc,&nbsp;Clémentine Deroiné,&nbsp;Caroline Clouard,&nbsp;Charlotte Gaillard","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106302","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106302","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Competition for feed in a group of gestating sows leads to aggression around feeding stations, which has a negative impact on their welfare. This study investigates the potential of teaching gestating sows an individual sound signal to reduce aggression resulting from competition for feed access, and thus improve their welfare in a group. A total of 32 sows were used. In a test room, “learning” sows (n = 16) went through 4 individual learning phases (27 days in total) to associate the individual sound signal with an invitation to feed from a one-way feeding station and to discriminate this individual sound signal from other unknown sound signals. After the learning phases, sows were subjected to a 3-day evaluation phase in groups of 4 sows. The \"naive\" sows (n = 16) were also introduced to the test room individually for 18 days, and in groups of 4 for 3 days without following the learning procedure. Learning sows correctly responded to 100% of their individual sound signal after only 8 days of individual learning, suggesting that they successfully associated the sound signal with feed access. Distinguishing between different sounds was harder as shown by only 18.8% of success after an unknown sound emission at the end of the individual learning phases. Naïve sows reduced the time spent in the feeder compared to learning sows (P &lt; 0.001). On the second day of the group phase, learning sows were less aggressive than naïve sows (P &lt; 0.05). Compared to high-ranking sows, low-ranking sows displayed a reduced number of spontaneous approaches to the feeder during the last individual learning phase (P &lt; 0.001), and higher success rates in the group phase (P &lt; 0.05). The study suggests that, for group-housed sows fed by an individual feeder, teaching sows an individual sound signal can modify their feeding and social behaviors, enhancing their overall well-being during feeding time. Furthermore, the results suggest that this individual learning may be particularly beneficial for low-ranking sows.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"275 ","pages":"Article 106302"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159124001503/pdfft?md5=3bd90631a34e765982c81c42b2ac1564&pid=1-s2.0-S0168159124001503-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141244545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Publishing ethics 出版伦理
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106301
Irene Camerlink , Péter Pongrácz
{"title":"Publishing ethics","authors":"Irene Camerlink ,&nbsp;Péter Pongrácz","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106301","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106301","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"275 ","pages":"Article 106301"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141307997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feel-good songs: application of a novel playback paradigm to induce a positive affective state in juvenile male Wistar rats 感觉良好的歌曲:应用新颖的播放范式诱导幼年雄性 Wistar 大鼠产生积极的情绪状态
IF 2.3 2区 农林科学 Q1 AGRICULTURE, DAIRY & ANIMAL SCIENCE Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106296
Tayla J. Hammond , Sarah M. Brown , Simone L. Meddle , Birte L. Nielsen , Alistair B. Lawrence , Vincent Bombail

Across animal welfare science there is a lack of validated models of positive affective states. Previous work has shown that presentation of contrastingly valenced ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs) to rats alters their behaviour. However, the potential of using playback of USVs to induce a positive affective state and promote positive animal welfare has yet to be explored. We used three cohorts of juvenile male Wistar rats (37 days old) in three independent experiments to develop a novel home cage playback paradigm to induce a positive affective state in rats. The intention behind this paradigm was to create a low-stress environment, given the heightened susceptibility of positive affective states to stress. Rats were presented in pairs with a playback track consisting of positively valenced 50-kHz USVs, White Noise (within the 30 – 100 kHz range), or Background Noise in their home cage. In Experiments 1 (N = 7 cages) and 2 (N = 14 cages), rats received a single presentation of each playback track in a Latin square experimental design. In Experiment 3 (N = 20 cages), rats received repeated presentations of the same playback track over five consecutive days. Changes in affective state were measured through USV production, approach to the stimulus, and play behaviour. Across all three experiments, the presentation of 50-kHz stimuli USVs increased subject-produced positively valenced 50 kHz USVs compared to presentation of Background Noise (e.g. Experiment 2; F2,239 = 6.05, p < 0.05). Similarly, rats also expressed an increase in approach behaviour towards the speaker in response to 50 kHz stimuli USVs compared to White Noise and Background Noise (Experiment 3 duration of approach behaviour; F2,479 = 10.55, p < 0.001). Whilst there was complexity in the relationship between the presentation of different acoustic stimuli and play behaviour, rats presented with the 50 kHz stimuli showed increased social play in the ten minutes during presentation under some of our test conditions. The impact of acoustic stimuli on measures of affective state across cohorts provides evidence that the home cage playback paradigm holds promise as a method for inducing a positive affective state in rats.

在整个动物福利科学领域,都缺乏经过验证的积极情绪状态模型。先前的研究表明,向大鼠播放具有对比价值的超声波发声(USVs)会改变它们的行为。然而,利用重放 USVs 来诱导积极情绪状态和促进积极动物福利的潜力还有待探索。我们在三项独立实验中使用了三组幼年雄性 Wistar 大鼠(37 天大),开发了一种新颖的家笼重放范例,以诱导大鼠的积极情绪状态。鉴于积极情绪状态对压力的易感性较高,该范例的目的是创造一个低压力环境。大鼠成对出现在笼子里的回放音轨上,该音轨由正面情绪的 50 千赫 USV、白噪声(30 - 100 千赫范围内)或背景噪声组成。在实验 1(N = 7 个笼子)和实验 2(N = 14 个笼子)中,大鼠在拉丁方形实验设计中接受每种播放轨道的单次播放。在实验 3(N = 20 个笼子)中,大鼠在连续五天内重复接受同一播放曲目。情感状态的变化是通过 USV 的产生、接近刺激和游戏行为来测量的。在所有三项实验中,与播放背景噪音相比,播放 50 千赫刺激的 USVs 会增加受试者产生的 50 千赫 USVs(例如实验 2;F2,239 = 6.05,p <0.05)。同样,与白噪声和背景噪声相比,大鼠在 50 kHz USVs 刺激下对说话者的接近行为也有所增加(实验 3 接近行为持续时间;F2,479 = 10.55,p < 0.001)。虽然不同声音刺激的呈现与玩耍行为之间的关系很复杂,但在我们的一些测试条件下,呈现 50 kHz 刺激的大鼠在呈现刺激的十分钟内表现出更多的社会性玩耍。声音刺激对不同群组大鼠情绪状态测量的影响证明,家笼播放范式有望成为诱导大鼠积极情绪状态的一种方法。
{"title":"Feel-good songs: application of a novel playback paradigm to induce a positive affective state in juvenile male Wistar rats","authors":"Tayla J. Hammond ,&nbsp;Sarah M. Brown ,&nbsp;Simone L. Meddle ,&nbsp;Birte L. Nielsen ,&nbsp;Alistair B. Lawrence ,&nbsp;Vincent Bombail","doi":"10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106296","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Across animal welfare science there is a lack of validated models of positive affective states. Previous work has shown that presentation of contrastingly valenced ultrasonic vocalisations (USVs) to rats alters their behaviour. However, the potential of using playback of USVs to induce a positive affective state and promote positive animal welfare has yet to be explored. We used three cohorts of juvenile male Wistar rats (37 days old) in three independent experiments to develop a novel home cage playback paradigm to induce a positive affective state in rats. The intention behind this paradigm was to create a low-stress environment, given the heightened susceptibility of positive affective states to stress. Rats were presented in pairs with a playback track consisting of positively valenced 50-kHz USVs, White Noise (within the 30 – 100 kHz range), or Background Noise in their home cage. In Experiments 1 (N = 7 cages) and 2 (N = 14 cages), rats received a single presentation of each playback track in a Latin square experimental design. In Experiment 3 (N = 20 cages), rats received repeated presentations of the same playback track over five consecutive days. Changes in affective state were measured through USV production, approach to the stimulus, and play behaviour. Across all three experiments, the presentation of 50-kHz stimuli USVs increased subject-produced positively valenced 50 kHz USVs compared to presentation of Background Noise (e.g. Experiment 2; F<sub>2,239</sub> = 6.05, <em>p &lt; 0.05</em>). Similarly, rats also expressed an increase in approach behaviour towards the speaker in response to 50 kHz stimuli USVs compared to White Noise and Background Noise (Experiment 3 duration of approach behaviour; F2,479 = 10.55, p &lt; 0.001). Whilst there was complexity in the relationship between the presentation of different acoustic stimuli and play behaviour, rats presented with the 50 kHz stimuli showed increased social play in the ten minutes during presentation under some of our test conditions. The impact of acoustic stimuli on measures of affective state across cohorts provides evidence that the home cage playback paradigm holds promise as a method for inducing a positive affective state in rats.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8222,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Behaviour Science","volume":"275 ","pages":"Article 106296"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159124001448/pdfft?md5=94227399dcbd5f63b63e061d4bcf10ae&pid=1-s2.0-S0168159124001448-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141244548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"农林科学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Animal Behaviour Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1