首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology最新文献

英文 中文
The availability of male mates affects the social mating strategies of a predominantly socially monogamous passerine 雄性配偶的可得性影响着一种以社会一夫一妻制为主的传鸟的社会交配策略
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03467-2
Daniel P. Cáceres Apaza, Gustavo J. Fernández, Paula S. Garrido Coria, Ramiro S. Arrieta, Paulo E. Llambías

We evaluated whether the availability of male mates affected mating strategies in a predominantly socially monogamous passerine, the grass wren Cistothorus platensis. We used the natural variation in adult sex ratio (ASR) and a male removal manipulation to assess if social polygyny was more frequent when male mates were less abundant. We evaluated the potential costs that females paid when breeding with a polygynous male by assessing how males distributed parental care between nests and analysing four correlates of breeding success (clutch size, nestling body condition, number of fledglings, and nest fate). Social monogamy was predominant when males were abundant while social polygyny was more frequent when the ASR was female-biased. The removal of males was associated with a high rate of social polygyny. Social polygyny occurred when males annexed the territory of a neighbouring female or when an unpaired female settled within the territory of a mated male. We identified several potential costs of social polygyny to females. Secondary females (females that laid eggs after the primary female) produced nestlings of lower body condition during the first half of the breeding season, received less help in feeding the brood, and increased their parental contribution. We suggest that while intrasexual competition may constrain social polygyny when males are abundant, reduced female life expectancy combined with moderate costs of male desertion may facilitate social polygyny when potential male mates are not available.

我们评估了雄性配偶的可得性是否会影响以社会一夫一妻制为主的被动鸟类--草鹪(Cistothorus platensis)的交配策略。我们利用成体性别比(ASR)的自然变化和雄性移除操作来评估当雄性配偶较少时,社会多配偶是否更频繁。我们通过评估雄性如何在巢之间分配亲职照料以及分析繁殖成功率的四个相关因素(窝产仔数、雏鸟身体状况、雏鸟数量和巢的命运),评估了雌性在与多雄性繁殖时可能付出的代价。当雄鸟数量较多时,社会一夫一妻制占主导地位,而当ASR以雌鸟为主时,社会一夫多妻制更为常见。雄鸟被移走与社会一夫多妻制的高发生率有关。当雄性动物吞并邻近雌性动物的领地或未配对的雌性动物在已交配雄性动物的领地内定居时,社会多雄现象就会发生。我们发现了社会多性对雌性的几种潜在代价。次要雌性(在主要雌性之后产卵的雌性)在繁殖季节的前半期产下的雏鸟身体状况较差,在喂养雏鸟方面得到的帮助较少,并且增加了其对父母的贡献。我们认为,当雄性雌鸟数量较多时,性内竞争可能会限制社会多配偶制,而当没有潜在雄性配偶时,雌性雌鸟预期寿命的缩短加上适度的雄性遗弃成本可能会促进社会多配偶制。
{"title":"The availability of male mates affects the social mating strategies of a predominantly socially monogamous passerine","authors":"Daniel P. Cáceres Apaza, Gustavo J. Fernández, Paula S. Garrido Coria, Ramiro S. Arrieta, Paulo E. Llambías","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03467-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03467-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We evaluated whether the availability of male mates affected mating strategies in a predominantly socially monogamous passerine, the grass wren <i>Cistothorus platensis</i>. We used the natural variation in adult sex ratio (ASR) and a male removal manipulation to assess if social polygyny was more frequent when male mates were less abundant. We evaluated the potential costs that females paid when breeding with a polygynous male by assessing how males distributed parental care between nests and analysing four correlates of breeding success (clutch size, nestling body condition, number of fledglings, and nest fate). Social monogamy was predominant when males were abundant while social polygyny was more frequent when the ASR was female-biased. The removal of males was associated with a high rate of social polygyny. Social polygyny occurred when males annexed the territory of a neighbouring female or when an unpaired female settled within the territory of a mated male. We identified several potential costs of social polygyny to females. Secondary females (females that laid eggs after the primary female) produced nestlings of lower body condition during the first half of the breeding season, received less help in feeding the brood, and increased their parental contribution. We suggest that while intrasexual competition may constrain social polygyny when males are abundant, reduced female life expectancy combined with moderate costs of male desertion may facilitate social polygyny when potential male mates are not available.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140809215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When are females dominant over males in rats (Rattus norvegicus)? 大鼠(Rattus norvegicus)的雌性何时比雄性占优势?
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-27 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03465-4
Miguel A. Puentes-Escamilla, Manon K. Schweinfurth, Charlotte K. Hemelrijk

Abstract

In group-living animals, males are assumed to be dominant over females when they are larger than females. Despite this, females have sometimes been proven to be dominant over some males via the winner-loser effect, which becomes stronger when the intensity of aggression in the group is higher. To test whether the winner-loser effect leads to (partial) female dominance in a species with a pronounced sexual dimorphism, we studied the hierarchy in 12 rat colonies (Rattus norvegicus) in which the rats could freely interact with their group members within a spacious area. To investigate the underlying mechanisms, we compared the empirical data to hypotheses generated by the agent-based model ‘DomWorld’. We show that females dominated on average 55% of the males, and occupied the alpha position in four colonies, in three of them they shared it with one or several males. Moreover, in line with the predictions of the computational model, females dominated a higher percentage of males when the intensity of aggression of the colony was higher. This shows that although females are only half as heavy as males, they dominate part of the males probably through the winner-loser effect. We suggest that this effect may be widespread in many other species and can be tested experimentally.

Significance statement

It is often assumed that males automatically dominate females because males are bigger and stronger than females in many species. However, the present study shows that females can dominate males due to the winner-loser effect. We used an agent-based computational model to generate specific hypotheses that we empirically tested in a large sample of rat colonies. Despite this species having a pronounced male-biased sex dimorphism, some females dominated males – with one female even occupying an unshared alpha position. Such partial female dominance was stronger in colonies with higher intensity of aggression. Here, defeated males may suffer a drastic decrease in their fighting capability and consequently give females more opportunities to surpass them in the hierarchy.

摘要 在群居动物中,当雄性体型大于雌性时,人们认为雄性对雌性占优势。尽管如此,雌性有时也被证明通过输赢效应对某些雄性占优势,当群体中的攻击强度较高时,输赢效应会变得更强。为了检验输赢效应是否会导致雌性在一个性别二形性明显的物种中占据(部分)优势地位,我们研究了 12 个大鼠群落(Rattus norvegicus)中的等级制度,在这些群落中,大鼠可以在一个宽敞的区域内与它们的群体成员自由互动。为了研究其潜在机制,我们将经验数据与基于代理的模型 "DomWorld "所产生的假设进行了比较。结果表明,雌鼠平均统治了 55% 的雄鼠,并在四个群落中占据了首领位置,在其中三个群落中,雌鼠与一只或几只雄鼠共同占据首领位置。此外,与计算模型的预测结果一致,当蚁群的攻击强度较高时,雌蚁支配雄蚁的比例也较高。这表明,虽然雌性的体重只有雄性的一半,但它们可能通过输赢效应支配了部分雄性。我们认为,这种效应可能在许多其他物种中普遍存在,可以通过实验进行检验。意义声明在许多物种中,人们通常认为雄性会自动支配雌性,因为雄性比雌性大而强壮。然而,本研究表明,由于赢家输家效应,雌性可以支配雄性。我们使用了一个基于代理的计算模型来生成特定的假设,并在一个大样本鼠群中进行了实证测试。尽管该物种具有明显的雄性偏向的性别二形性,但一些雌性却支配着雄性--其中一只雌鼠甚至占据了未共享的首领位置。在攻击强度较高的鼠群中,这种部分雌性优势更为明显。在这种情况下,战败的雄性的战斗力可能会急剧下降,从而使雌性有更多的机会在等级制度中超越它们。
{"title":"When are females dominant over males in rats (Rattus norvegicus)?","authors":"Miguel A. Puentes-Escamilla, Manon K. Schweinfurth, Charlotte K. Hemelrijk","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03465-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03465-4","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>In group-living animals, males are assumed to be dominant over females when they are larger than females. Despite this, females have sometimes been proven to be dominant over some males via the winner-loser effect, which becomes stronger when the intensity of aggression in the group is higher. To test whether the winner-loser effect leads to (partial) female dominance in a species with a pronounced sexual dimorphism, we studied the hierarchy in 12 rat colonies (<i>Rattus norvegicus</i>) in which the rats could freely interact with their group members within a spacious area. To investigate the underlying mechanisms, we compared the empirical data to hypotheses generated by the agent-based model ‘DomWorld’. We show that females dominated on average 55% of the males, and occupied the alpha position in four colonies, in three of them they shared it with one or several males. Moreover, in line with the predictions of the computational model, females dominated a higher percentage of males when the intensity of aggression of the colony was higher. This shows that although females are only half as heavy as males, they dominate part of the males probably through the winner-loser effect. We suggest that this effect may be widespread in many other species and can be tested experimentally.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>It is often assumed that males automatically dominate females because males are bigger and stronger than females in many species. However, the present study shows that females can dominate males due to the winner-loser effect. We used an agent-based computational model to generate specific hypotheses that we empirically tested in a large sample of rat colonies. Despite this species having a pronounced male-biased sex dimorphism, some females dominated males – with one female even occupying an unshared alpha position. Such partial female dominance was stronger in colonies with higher intensity of aggression. Here, defeated males may suffer a drastic decrease in their fighting capability and consequently give females more opportunities to surpass them in the hierarchy.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"291 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140804380","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of honey bee (Apis mellifera) group size on hygienic behavior performance 蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)群体大小对卫生行为表现的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03471-6
Phoebe Snyder, Jaymie Martin, Jacob J. Herman, Shlomo Franklin, Kaira M. Wagoner, Victoria Soroker, Olav Rueppell
<h3 data-test="abstract-sub-heading">Abstract</h3><p>The size of animal groups has profound effects on individual and collective behavior, particularly in social insect colonies. Larger colonies are predicted to be more complex with more specialization among members. However, the empirical support of this theoretical expectation is limited. Hygienic behavior of honey bees is a complex cooperative behavior of workers detecting, uncapping, and removing unhealthy brood. It is an important defense against brood diseases, including the ectoparasitic mite <i>Varroa destructor</i>. We support the prediction that hygienic behavior increases with group size using a simulation model. To also test this prediction empirically, we performed five experiments, to compare the hygienic performance of small and large honey bee groups at four different scales, roughly representing four orders of magnitude. Hygienic performance qualitatively increased across the different scales, but different methodologies limit quantitative comparisons across experiments. Within experiments, group size was also positively related to hygienic behavior. The strongest effects of group size were measured in groups that were smaller than what honey bees adopt under natural conditions. The group-size effect on hygienic performance decreased with increasing scale and at the full colony scale, group size was unrelated to hygienic assay scores. Therefore, colony size is unlikely to confound the hygienic evaluation of colonies in apicultural practice although we demonstrate clear effects of group size on hygienic behavior. Direct observations of individual behavior that were performed in two small scale experiments did not support our prediction of increased individual specialization in larger groups. Thus, our study supports the notion of performance benefits of larger groups in the context of social immunity, although the mechanisms of how group size enhances hygienic behavior remain to be investigated further.</p><h3 data-test="abstract-sub-heading">Significance Statement</h3><p>Social insects owe their ecological success partly to their efficient division of labor and behavioral specialization of colony members. Empirical support for the theoretical argument that group performance increases with group size is insufficient. Hygienic behavior is an important defense against brood pests and diseases that threaten honey bee health. Yet, it has not been investigated with respect to group size. Here, we analyze a simulation model, demonstrating theoretically that group size is predicted to increase hygienic behavior. We then provide experimental support for this prediction across a range of group sizes, though results of some experiments are equivocal and sample sizes are limited, constraining our empirical conclusions. In small groups, we find support for the theoretical idea that hygienic performance increases with group size, but our study also indicates that this effect is not very import
摘要 动物群体的大小对个体和集体行为有着深远的影响,特别是在社会性昆虫群落中。据预测,较大的群体会更加复杂,成员之间的专业化程度更高。然而,对这一理论预期的经验支持却很有限。蜜蜂的卫生行为是工蜂发现、揭开盖子和清除不健康雏蜂的复杂合作行为。这是蜜蜂抵御包括外寄生螨破坏者 Varroa 在内的育雏疾病的重要手段。我们通过模拟模型证实了卫生行为随群体大小而增加的预测。为了从经验上验证这一预测,我们进行了五次实验,比较了四种不同规模(大致代表四个数量级)的小型蜜蜂群体和大型蜜蜂群体的卫生表现。不同规模的蜂群的卫生表现都有质的提高,但不同的方法限制了不同实验之间的定量比较。在实验中,群体大小也与卫生行为呈正相关。与蜜蜂在自然条件下的行为相比,群体规模越小,对卫生行为的影响越大。蜂群规模对卫生表现的影响随着规模的扩大而减小,在整个蜂群规模中,蜂群规模与卫生检测得分无关。因此,在养蜂实践中,虽然我们证明了群体大小对卫生行为的明显影响,但群体大小不太可能干扰对蜂群的卫生评价。在两个小规模实验中对个体行为的直接观察并不支持我们的预测,即在较大的群体中个体的专业化程度会提高。因此,我们的研究支持在社会免疫背景下大群体对表现有益的观点,尽管群体大小如何增强卫生行为的机制仍有待进一步研究。 意义声明社会性昆虫在生态学上的成功部分归功于其有效的劳动分工和群体成员的行为专业化。群体表现随群体大小而提高的理论支持并不充分。卫生行为是抵御威胁蜜蜂健康的害虫和疾病的重要手段。然而,还没有人研究过卫生行为与蜂群规模的关系。在这里,我们对一个模拟模型进行了分析,从理论上证明了群体大小会增加卫生行为。然后,我们在各种规模的蜂群中为这一预测提供了实验支持,尽管有些实验结果模棱两可,而且样本数量有限,限制了我们的经验性结论。在小群体中,我们发现卫生表现会随着群体规模的扩大而提高这一理论观点得到了支持,但我们的研究也表明,在与养蜂相关的群体规模条件下,这种影响并不十分重要。在较大的群体中,我们没有发现支持个体专业化程度较高的观点。此外,我们的研究还表明,养蜂人在评估蜂群的卫生行为时,可以不考虑蜂群规模的偏差。
{"title":"The impact of honey bee (Apis mellifera) group size on hygienic behavior performance","authors":"Phoebe Snyder, Jaymie Martin, Jacob J. Herman, Shlomo Franklin, Kaira M. Wagoner, Victoria Soroker, Olav Rueppell","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03471-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03471-6","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\"&gt;Abstract&lt;/h3&gt;&lt;p&gt;The size of animal groups has profound effects on individual and collective behavior, particularly in social insect colonies. Larger colonies are predicted to be more complex with more specialization among members. However, the empirical support of this theoretical expectation is limited. Hygienic behavior of honey bees is a complex cooperative behavior of workers detecting, uncapping, and removing unhealthy brood. It is an important defense against brood diseases, including the ectoparasitic mite &lt;i&gt;Varroa destructor&lt;/i&gt;. We support the prediction that hygienic behavior increases with group size using a simulation model. To also test this prediction empirically, we performed five experiments, to compare the hygienic performance of small and large honey bee groups at four different scales, roughly representing four orders of magnitude. Hygienic performance qualitatively increased across the different scales, but different methodologies limit quantitative comparisons across experiments. Within experiments, group size was also positively related to hygienic behavior. The strongest effects of group size were measured in groups that were smaller than what honey bees adopt under natural conditions. The group-size effect on hygienic performance decreased with increasing scale and at the full colony scale, group size was unrelated to hygienic assay scores. Therefore, colony size is unlikely to confound the hygienic evaluation of colonies in apicultural practice although we demonstrate clear effects of group size on hygienic behavior. Direct observations of individual behavior that were performed in two small scale experiments did not support our prediction of increased individual specialization in larger groups. Thus, our study supports the notion of performance benefits of larger groups in the context of social immunity, although the mechanisms of how group size enhances hygienic behavior remain to be investigated further.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\"&gt;Significance Statement&lt;/h3&gt;&lt;p&gt;Social insects owe their ecological success partly to their efficient division of labor and behavioral specialization of colony members. Empirical support for the theoretical argument that group performance increases with group size is insufficient. Hygienic behavior is an important defense against brood pests and diseases that threaten honey bee health. Yet, it has not been investigated with respect to group size. Here, we analyze a simulation model, demonstrating theoretically that group size is predicted to increase hygienic behavior. We then provide experimental support for this prediction across a range of group sizes, though results of some experiments are equivocal and sample sizes are limited, constraining our empirical conclusions. In small groups, we find support for the theoretical idea that hygienic performance increases with group size, but our study also indicates that this effect is not very import","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"87 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140624436","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual vocal recognition and dear enemy effect in the black-and-gold howler monkey (Alouatta caraya) 黑金吼猴(Alouatta caraya)的个体声音识别和亲爱的敌人效应
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03469-0
Ingrid Holzmann, R. S. Córdoba

Many animal species respond less aggressively to calls emitted by neighbors in comparison with strangers, an asymmetry known as the “dear enemy” effect. The adaptive significance of having “dear enemies” would be to minimize defensive costs towards less-threatening individuals (like neighbors). The opposite situation, in which known neighbors become untrustworthy, representing an even greater menace than strangers, is call “nasty neighbor effect”. In addition to these neighbor-stranger discrimination abilities, some species are also capable of recognizing neighbors individually, allowing them to avoid risky encounters based on identity and past experiences, minimizing the probability of losing an encounter. In this study we tested if black-and-gold howler monkey (Alouatta caraya) males can recognize neighbors individually and investigated the nature of long-term relationships under the dear enemy/nasty neighbors hypotheses. We conducted 36 playback experiments on four dominant males in “El Cachapé” reserve, in Argentina. We exposed each male to three different treatments, consisting of roars from: 1- Neighbors from the area of home range overlap, 2- Misplaced neighbors from the opposite side to the area of home range overlap, and 3- Strangers, quantifying eight response variables during each experiment. Our results showed that dominant males recognize neighbors individually (by roaring longer in response to misplaced neighbors), clearly reacting more aggressively to neighbors who violate mutual agreements (like home range boundaries). Also, dominant males displayed a longer roar duration and closer approach to the sound source when hearing roars from strangers, supporting the hypothesis that neighbors are dear enemies in this species. Our results show that neighbor vocal recognition is key to understanding the configuration of areas of collective use and navigation decision in primates and that strangers exert the major threat to group stability in howler monkeys.

与陌生人相比,许多动物对邻居发出的叫声的攻击性较弱,这种不对称现象被称为 "亲爱的敌人 "效应。拥有 "亲爱的敌人 "的适应意义在于最大限度地降低对威胁较小的个体(如邻居)的防御成本。与此相反的情况是,已知的邻居变得不值得信任,比陌生人的威胁更大,这就是所谓的 "讨厌的邻居效应"。除了这些对邻居和陌生人的辨别能力外,有些物种还能单独识别邻居,使它们能够根据身份和过去的经验避免危险的相遇,从而将相遇失败的概率降到最低。在这项研究中,我们测试了黑金吼猴(Alouatta caraya)雄性是否能够单独识别邻居,并研究了在亲爱的敌人/讨厌的邻居假说下长期关系的性质。我们在阿根廷的 "El Cachapé "保护区对四只占优势的雄性吼猴进行了 36 次回放实验。我们让每只雄性暴露于三种不同的处理中,包括来自以下方面的吼叫声:1- 来自家庭范围重叠区域的邻居;2- 来自家庭范围重叠区域对侧的错位邻居;3- 陌生人。我们的结果表明,优势雄性能单独识别邻居(对错位邻居吼叫的时间更长),对违反双方协议(如家域边界)的邻居显然反应更激烈。此外,优势雄性在听到陌生人的吼叫时,吼叫持续时间会更长,并且会更靠近声源,这支持了 "邻居是该物种亲爱的敌人 "这一假设。我们的研究结果表明,邻居声音识别是了解灵长类动物集体使用区域配置和导航决策的关键,陌生人是吼猴群体稳定性的主要威胁。
{"title":"Individual vocal recognition and dear enemy effect in the black-and-gold howler monkey (Alouatta caraya)","authors":"Ingrid Holzmann, R. S. Córdoba","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03469-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03469-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many animal species respond less aggressively to calls emitted by neighbors in comparison with strangers, an asymmetry known as the “dear enemy” effect. The adaptive significance of having “dear enemies” would be to minimize defensive costs towards less-threatening individuals (like neighbors). The opposite situation, in which known neighbors become untrustworthy, representing an even greater menace than strangers, is call “nasty neighbor effect”. In addition to these neighbor-stranger discrimination abilities, some species are also capable of recognizing neighbors individually, allowing them to avoid risky encounters based on identity and past experiences, minimizing the probability of losing an encounter. In this study we tested if black-and-gold howler monkey (<i>Alouatta caraya</i>) males can recognize neighbors individually and investigated the nature of long-term relationships under the dear enemy/nasty neighbors hypotheses. We conducted 36 playback experiments on four dominant males in “El Cachapé” reserve, in Argentina. We exposed each male to three different treatments, consisting of roars from: 1- Neighbors from the area of home range overlap, 2- Misplaced neighbors from the opposite side to the area of home range overlap, and 3- Strangers, quantifying eight response variables during each experiment. Our results showed that dominant males recognize neighbors individually (by roaring longer in response to misplaced neighbors), clearly reacting more aggressively to neighbors who violate mutual agreements (like home range boundaries). Also, dominant males displayed a longer roar duration and closer approach to the sound source when hearing roars from strangers, supporting the hypothesis that neighbors are dear enemies in this species. Our results show that neighbor vocal recognition is key to understanding the configuration of areas of collective use and navigation decision in primates and that strangers exert the major threat to group stability in howler monkeys.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"100 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140624243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spatiotemporal variation in cognitive phenotype, social network position, and distribution of social associations in a food-caching bird 觅食鸟类认知表型、社会网络位置和社会关联分布的时空变化
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03466-3
A. M. Pitera, V. K. Heinen, J. F. Welklin, C. L. Branch, B. R. Sonnenberg, L. M. Benedict, E. S. Bridge, V. V. Pravosudov

Abstract

Phenotypic variation may influence social structure if animals associate nonrandomly based on phenotypic traits. For animals that rely on cognition for survival, variation in cognitive ability may also affect social structure. Individuals with worse cognitive abilities could benefit from preferentially associating with conspecifics with better cognition, from being more gregarious, or both, allowing them access to resources. Climatic conditions influence resource availability, which may also affect the relative benefits of assortment or gregariousness, resulting in spatiotemporal variation of social patterns. Using 5 years of data, we investigated assortment by spatial cognitive ability and associations between sociality and spatial cognition in food-caching mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli, inhabiting harsher environments at higher elevations and milder environments at lower elevations. Elevation environments differ in overwinter severity and in the harshest winter, high elevation chickadees with better spatial learning and memory abilities were less gregarious and showed lower social differentiation (or choosiness) than those with worse spatial learning and memory abilities but did not assort based on these traits. In two other seasons, including the second harshest winter, high elevation chickadees that were less cognitively flexible showed more social differentiation than those that were more cognitively flexible. In these two seasons alone, chickadees negatively assorted by spatial learning and memory performance. Chickadees from low elevations only demonstrated negative assortment in the second harshest winter. All other results were nonsignificant, suggesting that overall, individual spatial cognitive abilities or those of their associates have little influence on social preferences.

Significance statement

We showed that under certain environmental conditions, social behavior may be related to spatial cognitive abilities in mountain chickadees that rely heavily on spatial cognition for overwinter survival, but that overall, cognition does not appear to influence social behavior. In the few years that patterns were present, they fit three trends: 1) chickadees with worse spatial cognitive abilities were more gregarious; 2) chickadees with worse spatial cognitive abilities were choosier; and/or 3) chickadees had stronger associations with social partners that had spatial cognitive abilities that differed from their own and weaker associations with those of similar cognitive abilities. While it is unclear under which specific circumstances these patterns emerge, they coincided with extreme snowfall, suggesting that individuals with worse spatial cognition alter their social behavior to buffer against their relatively greater risk of starvation under extenuating circumstances.

摘要 如果动物根据表型特征非随机地结伴而行,那么表型变异可能会影响社会结构。对于依赖认知生存的动物来说,认知能力的差异也可能影响社会结构。认知能力较差的个体可能会优先与认知能力较强的同种动物交往,或更加合群,或两者兼而有之,从而获得资源。气候条件会影响资源的可获得性,这也可能会影响分类或聚群的相对收益,从而导致社会模式的时空变化。我们利用5年的数据,研究了在海拔较高的严酷环境和海拔较低的温和环境中栖息的觅食山鸡(Poecile gambeli)的空间认知能力分类以及社会性与空间认知之间的关联。高海拔环境的越冬严酷程度不同,在最严酷的冬季,与空间学习和记忆能力较差的雏鸟相比,空间学习和记忆能力较强的高海拔雏鸟的群居性较差,社会分化(或选择性)较低,但没有根据这些特征进行分类。在另外两个季节,包括第二个最严酷的冬季,认知灵活性较低的高海拔秧鸡比认知灵活性较高的秧鸡表现出更大的社会分化。仅在这两个季节,秧鸡的空间学习和记忆能力就出现了负分化。来自低海拔地区的秧鸡只有在第二个最严酷的冬季才表现出负分化。我们的研究表明,在特定的环境条件下,山地雏鸟的社会行为可能与空间认知能力有关,因为山地雏鸟主要依靠空间认知能力越冬生存,但总体而言,认知能力似乎并不影响社会行为。在出现模式的几年中,它们符合三种趋势:1)空间认知能力较差的秧鸡更合群;2)空间认知能力较差的秧鸡更挑剔;和/或3)秧鸡与空间认知能力不同的社会伙伴的联系更强,而与认知能力相似的社会伙伴的联系较弱。虽然尚不清楚这些模式是在什么具体情况下出现的,但它们与极端降雪同时出现,表明空间认知能力较差的个体会改变其社会行为,以缓冲在极端情况下相对较大的饥饿风险。
{"title":"Spatiotemporal variation in cognitive phenotype, social network position, and distribution of social associations in a food-caching bird","authors":"A. M. Pitera, V. K. Heinen, J. F. Welklin, C. L. Branch, B. R. Sonnenberg, L. M. Benedict, E. S. Bridge, V. V. Pravosudov","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03466-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03466-3","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Phenotypic variation may influence social structure if animals associate nonrandomly based on phenotypic traits. For animals that rely on cognition for survival, variation in cognitive ability may also affect social structure. Individuals with worse cognitive abilities could benefit from preferentially associating with conspecifics with better cognition, from being more gregarious, or both, allowing them access to resources. Climatic conditions influence resource availability, which may also affect the relative benefits of assortment or gregariousness, resulting in spatiotemporal variation of social patterns. Using 5 years of data, we investigated assortment by spatial cognitive ability and associations between sociality and spatial cognition in food-caching mountain chickadees, <i>Poecile gambeli</i>, inhabiting harsher environments at higher elevations and milder environments at lower elevations. Elevation environments differ in overwinter severity and in the harshest winter, high elevation chickadees with better spatial learning and memory abilities were less gregarious and showed lower social differentiation (or choosiness) than those with worse spatial learning and memory abilities but did not assort based on these traits. In two other seasons, including the second harshest winter, high elevation chickadees that were less cognitively flexible showed more social differentiation than those that were more cognitively flexible. In these two seasons alone, chickadees negatively assorted by spatial learning and memory performance. Chickadees from low elevations only demonstrated negative assortment in the second harshest winter. All other results were nonsignificant, suggesting that overall, individual spatial cognitive abilities or those of their associates have little influence on social preferences.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>We showed that under certain environmental conditions, social behavior may be related to spatial cognitive abilities in mountain chickadees that rely heavily on spatial cognition for overwinter survival, but that overall, cognition does not appear to influence social behavior. In the few years that patterns were present, they fit three trends: 1) chickadees with worse spatial cognitive abilities were more gregarious; 2) chickadees with worse spatial cognitive abilities were choosier; and/or 3) chickadees had stronger associations with social partners that had spatial cognitive abilities that differed from their own and weaker associations with those of similar cognitive abilities. While it is unclear under which specific circumstances these patterns emerge, they coincided with extreme snowfall, suggesting that individuals with worse spatial cognition alter their social behavior to buffer against their relatively greater risk of starvation under extenuating circumstances.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"24 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140613401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When neighbors become family: the dear-enemy effect of swimming crab and the verification of the formation hypothesis 当邻居成为家人:游蟹的亲敌效应和形成假说的验证
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9
Boshan Zhu, Hanzun Zhang, Xin Wang, Yunliang Lu, Fang Wang, Dapeng Liu

The neighbor-stranger response difference, which serves as a fundamental social network relationship, plays a crucial role in establishing and expanding complex social networks. However, the underlying causes of this phenomenon have received limited attention in controlled indoor settings. This study focused on Portunus trituberculatus, a representative crustacean species in the Western Pacific Ocean. We analyzed the behavioral responses of crabs towards intruders of varying familiarity and threat levels. Additionally, we quantified various parameters such as territory size, behavior quantification, time allocation, and territorial behavior score. The results showed that with increased familiarity, there was a decrease in the frequency of occupiers’ aggressive dash and aggressive wave. As the threat from the intruder decreased, occupiers displayed fewer territorial behaviors and engaged in fewer bouts, while the success rate in fights improved and the territorial behavior score declined. The results indicate a clear dear-enemy effect in the territorial defense of P. trituberculatus, and demonstrates that crabs adjusted the defense strategy according to the relative threat level of intruders. We verified the formation hypothesis of dear-enemy effect in crabs. In the future, we will expand the research from individuals to populations, analyze crustacean social networks from the perspective of group decision making, and provide more support for population dynamic analysis and decision evolution research of crustaceans.

邻居-陌生人反应差异作为一种基本的社会网络关系,在建立和扩展复杂的社会网络中发挥着至关重要的作用。然而,这种现象的根本原因在受控室内环境中受到的关注有限。本研究以三疣梭子蟹(Portunus trituberculatus)为对象,它是西太平洋具有代表性的甲壳类物种。我们分析了螃蟹对不同熟悉程度和威胁程度的入侵者的行为反应。此外,我们还量化了各种参数,如领地大小、行为量化、时间分配和领地行为得分。结果表明,随着熟悉程度的增加,入侵者的攻击性冲撞和攻击性挥手的频率会降低。随着入侵者威胁的降低,占领者表现出的领地行为和参与的斗殴次数也减少了,而斗殴的成功率提高了,领地行为得分也降低了。结果表明,三疣梭子蟹的领地防御存在明显的亲敌效应,并表明梭子蟹会根据入侵者的相对威胁程度调整防御策略。我们验证了河蟹亲敌效应的形成假说。未来,我们将把研究从个体扩展到种群,从群体决策的角度分析甲壳动物的社会网络,为甲壳动物的种群动态分析和决策进化研究提供更多支持。
{"title":"When neighbors become family: the dear-enemy effect of swimming crab and the verification of the formation hypothesis","authors":"Boshan Zhu, Hanzun Zhang, Xin Wang, Yunliang Lu, Fang Wang, Dapeng Liu","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The neighbor-stranger response difference, which serves as a fundamental social network relationship, plays a crucial role in establishing and expanding complex social networks. However, the underlying causes of this phenomenon have received limited attention in controlled indoor settings. This study focused on <i>Portunus trituberculatus</i>, a representative crustacean species in the Western Pacific Ocean. We analyzed the behavioral responses of crabs towards intruders of varying familiarity and threat levels. Additionally, we quantified various parameters such as territory size, behavior quantification, time allocation, and territorial behavior score. The results showed that with increased familiarity, there was a decrease in the frequency of occupiers’ aggressive dash and aggressive wave. As the threat from the intruder decreased, occupiers displayed fewer territorial behaviors and engaged in fewer bouts, while the success rate in fights improved and the territorial behavior score declined. The results indicate a clear dear-enemy effect in the territorial defense of <i>P. trituberculatus</i>, and demonstrates that crabs adjusted the defense strategy according to the relative threat level of intruders. We verified the formation hypothesis of dear-enemy effect in crabs. In the future, we will expand the research from individuals to populations, analyze crustacean social networks from the perspective of group decision making, and provide more support for population dynamic analysis and decision evolution research of crustaceans.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"46 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140580967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Geographic variation in acoustic and visual cues and their potential to signal body condition in the Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata 巴西树蛙(Boana albomarginata)声学和视觉线索的地理差异及其身体状况信号潜力
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7
Guilherme Augusto-Alves, Gerlinde Höbel, Luís Felipe Toledo

Abstract

Anuran communication is largely based on acoustic signals, but different sensory modes are also widespread, including visual communication using body color traits as a way of signaling. The Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata, has a complex behavioral repertoire presenting several call types and performing gestures as visual signals. This species has a greenish body color with orange patches on the flanks and thighs. These patches become visible when males are in a calling posture or performing visual signals such as leg kicking and limb lifting, suggesting that they might use the patches as visual cues. We sampled seven populations, using call recordings and photographs to access males call and color traits. We demonstrate that there is variation in color and call properties across populations. Additionally, we observe variation in the relationship between color traits and call properties in different populations, revealing that only two populations exhibit a significant correlation between color and call traits. Further, while call properties and color traits were not related with individual body size, they were associated with body condition. The results indicate a universal pattern across populations for call properties, wherein males in better condition consistently displayed lower-pitched calls, longer calls, and shorter intervals between calls. Regarding color traits, males in better condition in four out of the seven evaluated populations exhibited larger orange patch sizes, lower orange hue values, and higher hue contrasts. Although we observed some level of relation among color, call, and body traits, there is not a universal pattern across all populations.

Significance statement

Animal social interactions are mediated by signals transmitted through different sensory modes (i.e., acoustic, chemical, tactile, and visual), and more than one of these modalities can compose the behavioral repertoire of one species. Using photographs and acoustic recordings of Boana albomarginata males in natural environments, we documented geographic variation in both signals, call and color, and investigated their potential to convey individual body size and condition. Our findings reveal that both signals were correlated with individual body condition. In addition, color traits were associated with call properties in some populations.

摘要有尾类动物的交流主要基于声音信号,但不同的感官模式也很普遍,包括利用体色特征作为信号方式的视觉交流。巴西树蛙(Boana albomarginata)具有复杂的行为曲目,可发出多种叫声,并以手势作为视觉信号。该物种身体呈绿色,侧腹和大腿上有橙色斑块。当雄性处于叫声姿态或做出踢腿和抬起四肢等视觉信号时,这些斑块就会显现出来,这表明它们可能将这些斑块用作视觉线索。我们对七个种群进行了采样,利用叫声录音和照片来了解雄鸟的叫声和颜色特征。结果表明,不同种群之间的颜色和叫声特征存在差异。此外,我们还观察到不同种群中颜色特征与叫声特征之间关系的差异,发现只有两个种群的颜色与叫声特征之间存在显著的相关性。此外,虽然鸣叫特性和颜色特征与个体体型无关,但它们与身体状况相关。研究结果表明,在不同种群中,雄性个体的叫声特征具有普遍性,即体质较好的雄性个体叫声音调较低、叫声较长、叫声间隔较短。在颜色特征方面,七个评估种群中有四个种群中状态较好的雄性表现出较大的橙色斑块面积、较低的橙色色调值和较高的色调对比度。虽然我们观察到颜色、叫声和身体特征之间存在一定程度的关系,但在所有种群中并不存在普遍的模式。意义声明动物的社会互动是通过不同的感官模式(即声学、化学、触觉和视觉)传递的信号进行中介的,而且这些模式中的一种以上可以构成一个物种的行为剧目。我们利用对自然环境中雄性白眉鲣鸟的照片和声学录音,记录了这两种信号(叫声和颜色)的地理差异,并研究了它们传递个体体型和状态的潜力。我们的研究结果表明,这两种信号都与个体的身体状况相关。此外,在某些种群中,颜色特征与叫声特性相关。
{"title":"Geographic variation in acoustic and visual cues and their potential to signal body condition in the Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata","authors":"Guilherme Augusto-Alves, Gerlinde Höbel, Luís Felipe Toledo","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Anuran communication is largely based on acoustic signals, but different sensory modes are also widespread, including visual communication using body color traits as a way of signaling. The Brazilian treefrog, <i>Boana albomarginata</i>, has a complex behavioral repertoire presenting several call types and performing gestures as visual signals. This species has a greenish body color with orange patches on the flanks and thighs. These patches become visible when males are in a calling posture or performing visual signals such as leg kicking and limb lifting, suggesting that they might use the patches as visual cues. We sampled seven populations, using call recordings and photographs to access males call and color traits. We demonstrate that there is variation in color and call properties across populations. Additionally, we observe variation in the relationship between color traits and call properties in different populations, revealing that only two populations exhibit a significant correlation between color and call traits. Further, while call properties and color traits were not related with individual body size, they were associated with body condition. The results indicate a universal pattern across populations for call properties, wherein males in better condition consistently displayed lower-pitched calls, longer calls, and shorter intervals between calls. Regarding color traits, males in better condition in four out of the seven evaluated populations exhibited larger orange patch sizes, lower orange hue values, and higher hue contrasts. Although we observed some level of relation among color, call, and body traits, there is not a universal pattern across all populations.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Animal social interactions are mediated by signals transmitted through different sensory modes (i.e., acoustic, chemical, tactile, and visual), and more than one of these modalities can compose the behavioral repertoire of one species. Using photographs and acoustic recordings of <i>Boana albomarginata</i> males in natural environments, we documented geographic variation in both signals, call and color, and investigated their potential to convey individual body size and condition. Our findings reveal that both signals were correlated with individual body condition. In addition, color traits were associated with call properties in some populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"25 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To house or oust: Honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies can evaluate and evict drones of low quality 安置或驱逐:蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)蜂群可以评估和驱逐低质量的无人驾驶飞行器
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8
Izaak R. Gilchrist, Jonathan M. Nixon, Riley R. Shultz, Matthew D. Ginzel, Brock A. Harpur

Abstract

Across the animal kingdom, males advertise their quality to potential mates. Males of low reproductive quality, such as those that are sick, may be excluded from mating. In eusocial species, there is some evidence that reproductive females gauge the quality of their mates. However, males often spend much more time with non-reproductive females when being raised or when returning from unsuccessful mating flights. Do non-reproductive workers evaluate the quality of male reproductives? Here we address this question using male honey bees (Apis mellifera), called drones, as a model. We generated immune-challenged drones by injecting them with lipopolysaccharide and tested: 1) do workers evict immune-challenged drones from their colony, 2) do cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profiles, body size, or mass change when drones are immune-challenged, and 3) are these changes used by workers to exclude low quality males from the colony? We found that an immune challenge causes changes in CHC profiles of drones and reduces their body mass. Workers selectively evict small and immune-challenged drones who, themselves, do not self-evict. This work demonstrates that some eusocial males undergo an additional layer of scrutiny prior to mating mediated by the non-reproductive worker caste.

Significance statement

Males of some species must advertise their quality to mates but, in the case of eusocial species, must they also advertise their quality to nestmates? By manipulating honey bee male quality, we found that small and immune-challenged drones are evicted from colonies overnight. Workers may not use a drone’s cuticular hydrocarbon profile to make this assessment. This is a new example of social immunity expressed against adult males and an example of worker involvement in reproductive decisions.

摘要在整个动物界,雄性动物都会向潜在配偶宣传自己的质量。生殖质量低的雄性,如生病的雄性,可能会被排除在交配之外。在雌雄同体的物种中,有证据表明生殖能力强的雌性会衡量配偶的质量。然而,雄性在被抚养长大或交配失败后返回时,往往会花更多的时间与非生产性雌性在一起。非生殖工蜂是否会评估雄性生殖工蜂的质量?在这里,我们以雄性蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)(称为无人蜂)为模型来探讨这个问题。我们通过给无人蜂注射脂多糖产生了免疫挑战无人蜂,并测试了:1)工蜂是否会将免疫挑战无人蜂驱逐出蜂群;2)当无人蜂受到免疫挑战时,其角质烃(CHC)特征、体型或质量是否会发生变化;3)工蜂是否会利用这些变化将低质量雄蜂排除出蜂群?我们发现,免疫挑战会导致无人机的碳氢化合物谱图发生变化,并降低其体重。工蜂会选择性地驱逐体型较小且受到免疫挑战的无人机,而这些无人机本身不会自我驱逐。这项工作表明,在非生殖工蜂种姓的介导下,一些雌性雄蜂在交配前还需要接受额外的审查。意义声明一些物种的雄蜂必须向配偶宣传自己的品质,但对于雌性物种来说,它们是否也必须向巢友宣传自己的品质呢?通过操纵蜜蜂雄蜂的质量,我们发现,体型小、免疫力差的雄蜂会在一夜之间被逐出蜂群。工蜂可能不会利用无人蜂的角质层碳氢化合物特征来进行这种评估。这是针对成年雄蜂的社会免疫力的一个新例子,也是工蜂参与生殖决策的一个例子。
{"title":"To house or oust: Honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies can evaluate and evict drones of low quality","authors":"Izaak R. Gilchrist, Jonathan M. Nixon, Riley R. Shultz, Matthew D. Ginzel, Brock A. Harpur","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Across the animal kingdom, males advertise their quality to potential mates. Males of low reproductive quality, such as those that are sick, may be excluded from mating. In eusocial species, there is some evidence that reproductive females gauge the quality of their mates. However, males often spend much more time with non-reproductive females when being raised or when returning from unsuccessful mating flights. Do non-reproductive workers evaluate the quality of male reproductives? Here we address this question using male honey bees (<i>Apis mellifera</i>), called drones, as a model. We generated immune-challenged drones by injecting them with lipopolysaccharide and tested: 1) do workers evict immune-challenged drones from their colony, 2) do cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profiles, body size, or mass change when drones are immune-challenged, and 3) are these changes used by workers to exclude low quality males from the colony? We found that an immune challenge causes changes in CHC profiles of drones and reduces their body mass. Workers selectively evict small and immune-challenged drones who, themselves, do not self-evict. This work demonstrates that some eusocial males undergo an additional layer of scrutiny prior to mating mediated by the non-reproductive worker caste.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Males of some species must advertise their quality to mates but, in the case of eusocial species, must they also advertise their quality to nestmates? By manipulating honey bee male quality, we found that small and immune-challenged drones are evicted from colonies overnight. Workers may not use a drone’s cuticular hydrocarbon profile to make this assessment. This is a new example of social immunity expressed against adult males and an example of worker involvement in reproductive decisions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"280 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581098","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Boldness and exploratory behaviors differ between sunfish (Lepomis spp.) congeners in a standardized assay 太阳鱼(Lepomis spp.)
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5
Kevin A. Adeli, Sam J. Woods, Steven J. Cooke, Chris K. Elvidge

The shy-bold behavioral continuum is an element of animal behavior which is often studied for its ecological relevance, particularly in the context of predation risk. How individuals respond to various predation cues is well studied at the individual level, but relatively little is known about how these responses can differ among closely related species. We exposed individual wild-caught juvenile bluegill (Lepomis macrochirus) and pumpkinseed (L. gibbosus) to kairomones of a common predator (Northern pike, Esox lucius), conspecific alarm cues, or a lake water control in a Z-maze trial commonly used to assess relative levels of bold and exploratory behaviors. Neither species exhibited any significant behavioral responses to either predation cue, but bluegill consistently displayed more bold-type behaviors than pumpkinseed. Although the lack of a behavioral response to predation cues in this study is equivocal, we identify clear differences in boldness between these two congeners and discuss the possibility of ecological niche differentiation driving these behavioral differences.

害羞-大胆行为的连续性是动物行为的一个要素,它经常被用来研究其生态相关性,尤其是在捕食风险的背景下。个体如何对各种捕食线索做出反应在个体水平上已经有了很好的研究,但对于这些反应在近亲物种之间如何不同,人们的了解相对较少。我们将野生捕获的幼年蓝鳃鱼(Lepomis macrochirus)和南瓜籽鱼(L. gibbosus)个体暴露于共同捕食者(北方梭子鱼,Esox lucius)的凯尔蒙、同种警报线索或湖水对照中,并进行了常用于评估大胆和探索行为相对水平的 Z 型迷宫试验。两种鱼对捕食线索都没有表现出明显的行为反应,但蓝鳃鱼始终比南瓜籽鱼表现出更多的大胆型行为。尽管这项研究中缺乏对捕食线索的行为反应,但我们发现这两种同源鱼类在大胆程度上存在明显差异,并讨论了生态位差异驱动这些行为差异的可能性。
{"title":"Boldness and exploratory behaviors differ between sunfish (Lepomis spp.) congeners in a standardized assay","authors":"Kevin A. Adeli, Sam J. Woods, Steven J. Cooke, Chris K. Elvidge","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The shy-bold behavioral continuum is an element of animal behavior which is often studied for its ecological relevance, particularly in the context of predation risk. How individuals respond to various predation cues is well studied at the individual level, but relatively little is known about how these responses can differ among closely related species. We exposed individual wild-caught juvenile bluegill (<i>Lepomis macrochirus</i>) and pumpkinseed (<i>L. gibbosus</i>) to kairomones of a common predator (Northern pike, <i>Esox lucius</i>), conspecific alarm cues, or a lake water control in a Z-maze trial commonly used to assess relative levels of bold and exploratory behaviors. Neither species exhibited any significant behavioral responses to either predation cue, but bluegill consistently displayed more bold-type behaviors than pumpkinseed. Although the lack of a behavioral response to predation cues in this study is equivocal, we identify clear differences in boldness between these two congeners and discuss the possibility of ecological niche differentiation driving these behavioral differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"280 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140580963","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Daily ranging and den usage patterns structure the spatiotemporal properties of social encounters in spotted hyenas 斑鬣狗的日常活动和巢穴使用模式决定了其社会交往的时空特性
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-03 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3

Abstract

Fission–fusion dynamics describe the tendency for members of some animal societies to associate in subgroups that change size and structure fluidly over time. These dynamics shape social complexity and social structure, but are difficult to study because they unfold simultaneously over large spatial scales. Here we use simultaneous, fine-scale GPS data from spotted hyenas to examine fission–fusion dynamics through a dyadic analysis of merge-split events between pairs of individuals. We introduce a species-agnostic framework for identifying merge-split events and discretizing them into three phases (merging, together, and splitting), enabling analysis of each phase as well as the connections among phases. Applying this framework to the hyena data, we examine the temporal and spatial properties of merges and splits between dyads and test the extent to which social encounters are driven by key locations. Specifically, we focus on communal dens—shelters for juvenile hyenas where classical observational studies often report large aggregations of adults. We find that overall, 62% of merges occurred at communal dens, supporting the idea that dens facilitate meet-ups and subsequent social behavior. Social encounters most commonly involved close approaches within a few meters between hyenas, while co-travel together occurred in only 11% of events. Comparison to permutation-based reference models suggests that independent movement decisions structure broad-scale patterns of social encounters but do not explain the fine-scale dynamics of interactions that unfold during these encounters. We reflect on how physical features such as dens can become social hotspots, causing social and spatial processes to become fundamentally intertwined.

摘要 裂变-融合动力学描述了一些动物社会的成员结成亚群的趋势,这些亚群的规模和结构随着时间的推移而不断变化。这些动态变化决定了社会复杂性和社会结构,但由于它们在大空间尺度上同时展开,因此很难研究。在这里,我们利用斑鬣狗的同步、精细尺度 GPS 数据,通过对一对个体之间的合并-分裂事件进行二元分析,来研究裂变-融合动态。我们引入了一个与物种无关的框架来识别合并-分裂事件,并将其离散化为三个阶段(合并、在一起和分裂),从而能够分析每个阶段以及各阶段之间的联系。我们将这一框架应用于鬣狗数据中,研究了二元组之间合并和分裂的时间和空间特性,并检验了社交相遇在多大程度上受关键地点的驱动。具体来说,我们将重点放在幼年鬣狗的公共巢穴--庇护所上,经典的观察研究通常会报告成年鬣狗在这里大量聚集。我们发现,总体而言,62%的会合发生在公共巢穴,这支持了巢穴有助于会合和随后的社会行为的观点。社交相遇最常见的情况是鬣狗之间几米内的近距离接触,而共同旅行的情况仅占 11%。与基于排列组合的参考模型的比较表明,独立的运动决定构建了社会相遇的大尺度模式,但并不能解释相遇期间展开的细尺度互动动态。我们思考了巢穴等物理特征如何成为社会热点,导致社会和空间过程从根本上交织在一起。
{"title":"Daily ranging and den usage patterns structure the spatiotemporal properties of social encounters in spotted hyenas","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Fission–fusion dynamics describe the tendency for members of some animal societies to associate in subgroups that change size and structure fluidly over time. These dynamics shape social complexity and social structure, but are difficult to study because they unfold simultaneously over large spatial scales. Here we use simultaneous, fine-scale GPS data from spotted hyenas to examine fission–fusion dynamics through a dyadic analysis of <em>merge-split events</em> between pairs of individuals. We introduce a species-agnostic framework for identifying merge-split events and discretizing them into three phases (merging, together, and splitting), enabling analysis of each phase as well as the connections among phases. Applying this framework to the hyena data, we examine the temporal and spatial properties of merges and splits between dyads and test the extent to which social encounters are driven by key locations. Specifically, we focus on communal dens—shelters for juvenile hyenas where classical observational studies often report large aggregations of adults. We find that overall, 62% of merges occurred at communal dens, supporting the idea that dens facilitate meet-ups and subsequent social behavior. Social encounters most commonly involved close approaches within a few meters between hyenas, while co-travel together occurred in only 11% of events. Comparison to permutation-based reference models suggests that independent movement decisions structure broad-scale patterns of social encounters but do not explain the fine-scale dynamics of interactions that unfold during these encounters. We reflect on how physical features such as dens can become social hotspots, causing social and spatial processes to become fundamentally intertwined.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"44 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581094","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1