首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology最新文献

英文 中文
When neighbors become family: the dear-enemy effect of swimming crab and the verification of the formation hypothesis 当邻居成为家人:游蟹的亲敌效应和形成假说的验证
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-13 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9
Boshan Zhu, Hanzun Zhang, Xin Wang, Yunliang Lu, Fang Wang, Dapeng Liu

The neighbor-stranger response difference, which serves as a fundamental social network relationship, plays a crucial role in establishing and expanding complex social networks. However, the underlying causes of this phenomenon have received limited attention in controlled indoor settings. This study focused on Portunus trituberculatus, a representative crustacean species in the Western Pacific Ocean. We analyzed the behavioral responses of crabs towards intruders of varying familiarity and threat levels. Additionally, we quantified various parameters such as territory size, behavior quantification, time allocation, and territorial behavior score. The results showed that with increased familiarity, there was a decrease in the frequency of occupiers’ aggressive dash and aggressive wave. As the threat from the intruder decreased, occupiers displayed fewer territorial behaviors and engaged in fewer bouts, while the success rate in fights improved and the territorial behavior score declined. The results indicate a clear dear-enemy effect in the territorial defense of P. trituberculatus, and demonstrates that crabs adjusted the defense strategy according to the relative threat level of intruders. We verified the formation hypothesis of dear-enemy effect in crabs. In the future, we will expand the research from individuals to populations, analyze crustacean social networks from the perspective of group decision making, and provide more support for population dynamic analysis and decision evolution research of crustaceans.

邻居-陌生人反应差异作为一种基本的社会网络关系,在建立和扩展复杂的社会网络中发挥着至关重要的作用。然而,这种现象的根本原因在受控室内环境中受到的关注有限。本研究以三疣梭子蟹(Portunus trituberculatus)为对象,它是西太平洋具有代表性的甲壳类物种。我们分析了螃蟹对不同熟悉程度和威胁程度的入侵者的行为反应。此外,我们还量化了各种参数,如领地大小、行为量化、时间分配和领地行为得分。结果表明,随着熟悉程度的增加,入侵者的攻击性冲撞和攻击性挥手的频率会降低。随着入侵者威胁的降低,占领者表现出的领地行为和参与的斗殴次数也减少了,而斗殴的成功率提高了,领地行为得分也降低了。结果表明,三疣梭子蟹的领地防御存在明显的亲敌效应,并表明梭子蟹会根据入侵者的相对威胁程度调整防御策略。我们验证了河蟹亲敌效应的形成假说。未来,我们将把研究从个体扩展到种群,从群体决策的角度分析甲壳动物的社会网络,为甲壳动物的种群动态分析和决策进化研究提供更多支持。
{"title":"When neighbors become family: the dear-enemy effect of swimming crab and the verification of the formation hypothesis","authors":"Boshan Zhu, Hanzun Zhang, Xin Wang, Yunliang Lu, Fang Wang, Dapeng Liu","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03460-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The neighbor-stranger response difference, which serves as a fundamental social network relationship, plays a crucial role in establishing and expanding complex social networks. However, the underlying causes of this phenomenon have received limited attention in controlled indoor settings. This study focused on <i>Portunus trituberculatus</i>, a representative crustacean species in the Western Pacific Ocean. We analyzed the behavioral responses of crabs towards intruders of varying familiarity and threat levels. Additionally, we quantified various parameters such as territory size, behavior quantification, time allocation, and territorial behavior score. The results showed that with increased familiarity, there was a decrease in the frequency of occupiers’ aggressive dash and aggressive wave. As the threat from the intruder decreased, occupiers displayed fewer territorial behaviors and engaged in fewer bouts, while the success rate in fights improved and the territorial behavior score declined. The results indicate a clear dear-enemy effect in the territorial defense of <i>P. trituberculatus</i>, and demonstrates that crabs adjusted the defense strategy according to the relative threat level of intruders. We verified the formation hypothesis of dear-enemy effect in crabs. In the future, we will expand the research from individuals to populations, analyze crustacean social networks from the perspective of group decision making, and provide more support for population dynamic analysis and decision evolution research of crustaceans.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140580967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Geographic variation in acoustic and visual cues and their potential to signal body condition in the Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata 巴西树蛙(Boana albomarginata)声学和视觉线索的地理差异及其身体状况信号潜力
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7
Guilherme Augusto-Alves, Gerlinde Höbel, Luís Felipe Toledo

Abstract

Anuran communication is largely based on acoustic signals, but different sensory modes are also widespread, including visual communication using body color traits as a way of signaling. The Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata, has a complex behavioral repertoire presenting several call types and performing gestures as visual signals. This species has a greenish body color with orange patches on the flanks and thighs. These patches become visible when males are in a calling posture or performing visual signals such as leg kicking and limb lifting, suggesting that they might use the patches as visual cues. We sampled seven populations, using call recordings and photographs to access males call and color traits. We demonstrate that there is variation in color and call properties across populations. Additionally, we observe variation in the relationship between color traits and call properties in different populations, revealing that only two populations exhibit a significant correlation between color and call traits. Further, while call properties and color traits were not related with individual body size, they were associated with body condition. The results indicate a universal pattern across populations for call properties, wherein males in better condition consistently displayed lower-pitched calls, longer calls, and shorter intervals between calls. Regarding color traits, males in better condition in four out of the seven evaluated populations exhibited larger orange patch sizes, lower orange hue values, and higher hue contrasts. Although we observed some level of relation among color, call, and body traits, there is not a universal pattern across all populations.

Significance statement

Animal social interactions are mediated by signals transmitted through different sensory modes (i.e., acoustic, chemical, tactile, and visual), and more than one of these modalities can compose the behavioral repertoire of one species. Using photographs and acoustic recordings of Boana albomarginata males in natural environments, we documented geographic variation in both signals, call and color, and investigated their potential to convey individual body size and condition. Our findings reveal that both signals were correlated with individual body condition. In addition, color traits were associated with call properties in some populations.

摘要有尾类动物的交流主要基于声音信号,但不同的感官模式也很普遍,包括利用体色特征作为信号方式的视觉交流。巴西树蛙(Boana albomarginata)具有复杂的行为曲目,可发出多种叫声,并以手势作为视觉信号。该物种身体呈绿色,侧腹和大腿上有橙色斑块。当雄性处于叫声姿态或做出踢腿和抬起四肢等视觉信号时,这些斑块就会显现出来,这表明它们可能将这些斑块用作视觉线索。我们对七个种群进行了采样,利用叫声录音和照片来了解雄鸟的叫声和颜色特征。结果表明,不同种群之间的颜色和叫声特征存在差异。此外,我们还观察到不同种群中颜色特征与叫声特征之间关系的差异,发现只有两个种群的颜色与叫声特征之间存在显著的相关性。此外,虽然鸣叫特性和颜色特征与个体体型无关,但它们与身体状况相关。研究结果表明,在不同种群中,雄性个体的叫声特征具有普遍性,即体质较好的雄性个体叫声音调较低、叫声较长、叫声间隔较短。在颜色特征方面,七个评估种群中有四个种群中状态较好的雄性表现出较大的橙色斑块面积、较低的橙色色调值和较高的色调对比度。虽然我们观察到颜色、叫声和身体特征之间存在一定程度的关系,但在所有种群中并不存在普遍的模式。意义声明动物的社会互动是通过不同的感官模式(即声学、化学、触觉和视觉)传递的信号进行中介的,而且这些模式中的一种以上可以构成一个物种的行为剧目。我们利用对自然环境中雄性白眉鲣鸟的照片和声学录音,记录了这两种信号(叫声和颜色)的地理差异,并研究了它们传递个体体型和状态的潜力。我们的研究结果表明,这两种信号都与个体的身体状况相关。此外,在某些种群中,颜色特征与叫声特性相关。
{"title":"Geographic variation in acoustic and visual cues and their potential to signal body condition in the Brazilian treefrog, Boana albomarginata","authors":"Guilherme Augusto-Alves, Gerlinde Höbel, Luís Felipe Toledo","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03462-7","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Anuran communication is largely based on acoustic signals, but different sensory modes are also widespread, including visual communication using body color traits as a way of signaling. The Brazilian treefrog, <i>Boana albomarginata</i>, has a complex behavioral repertoire presenting several call types and performing gestures as visual signals. This species has a greenish body color with orange patches on the flanks and thighs. These patches become visible when males are in a calling posture or performing visual signals such as leg kicking and limb lifting, suggesting that they might use the patches as visual cues. We sampled seven populations, using call recordings and photographs to access males call and color traits. We demonstrate that there is variation in color and call properties across populations. Additionally, we observe variation in the relationship between color traits and call properties in different populations, revealing that only two populations exhibit a significant correlation between color and call traits. Further, while call properties and color traits were not related with individual body size, they were associated with body condition. The results indicate a universal pattern across populations for call properties, wherein males in better condition consistently displayed lower-pitched calls, longer calls, and shorter intervals between calls. Regarding color traits, males in better condition in four out of the seven evaluated populations exhibited larger orange patch sizes, lower orange hue values, and higher hue contrasts. Although we observed some level of relation among color, call, and body traits, there is not a universal pattern across all populations.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Animal social interactions are mediated by signals transmitted through different sensory modes (i.e., acoustic, chemical, tactile, and visual), and more than one of these modalities can compose the behavioral repertoire of one species. Using photographs and acoustic recordings of <i>Boana albomarginata</i> males in natural environments, we documented geographic variation in both signals, call and color, and investigated their potential to convey individual body size and condition. Our findings reveal that both signals were correlated with individual body condition. In addition, color traits were associated with call properties in some populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To house or oust: Honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies can evaluate and evict drones of low quality 安置或驱逐:蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)蜂群可以评估和驱逐低质量的无人驾驶飞行器
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8
Izaak R. Gilchrist, Jonathan M. Nixon, Riley R. Shultz, Matthew D. Ginzel, Brock A. Harpur

Abstract

Across the animal kingdom, males advertise their quality to potential mates. Males of low reproductive quality, such as those that are sick, may be excluded from mating. In eusocial species, there is some evidence that reproductive females gauge the quality of their mates. However, males often spend much more time with non-reproductive females when being raised or when returning from unsuccessful mating flights. Do non-reproductive workers evaluate the quality of male reproductives? Here we address this question using male honey bees (Apis mellifera), called drones, as a model. We generated immune-challenged drones by injecting them with lipopolysaccharide and tested: 1) do workers evict immune-challenged drones from their colony, 2) do cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profiles, body size, or mass change when drones are immune-challenged, and 3) are these changes used by workers to exclude low quality males from the colony? We found that an immune challenge causes changes in CHC profiles of drones and reduces their body mass. Workers selectively evict small and immune-challenged drones who, themselves, do not self-evict. This work demonstrates that some eusocial males undergo an additional layer of scrutiny prior to mating mediated by the non-reproductive worker caste.

Significance statement

Males of some species must advertise their quality to mates but, in the case of eusocial species, must they also advertise their quality to nestmates? By manipulating honey bee male quality, we found that small and immune-challenged drones are evicted from colonies overnight. Workers may not use a drone’s cuticular hydrocarbon profile to make this assessment. This is a new example of social immunity expressed against adult males and an example of worker involvement in reproductive decisions.

摘要在整个动物界,雄性动物都会向潜在配偶宣传自己的质量。生殖质量低的雄性,如生病的雄性,可能会被排除在交配之外。在雌雄同体的物种中,有证据表明生殖能力强的雌性会衡量配偶的质量。然而,雄性在被抚养长大或交配失败后返回时,往往会花更多的时间与非生产性雌性在一起。非生殖工蜂是否会评估雄性生殖工蜂的质量?在这里,我们以雄性蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)(称为无人蜂)为模型来探讨这个问题。我们通过给无人蜂注射脂多糖产生了免疫挑战无人蜂,并测试了:1)工蜂是否会将免疫挑战无人蜂驱逐出蜂群;2)当无人蜂受到免疫挑战时,其角质烃(CHC)特征、体型或质量是否会发生变化;3)工蜂是否会利用这些变化将低质量雄蜂排除出蜂群?我们发现,免疫挑战会导致无人机的碳氢化合物谱图发生变化,并降低其体重。工蜂会选择性地驱逐体型较小且受到免疫挑战的无人机,而这些无人机本身不会自我驱逐。这项工作表明,在非生殖工蜂种姓的介导下,一些雌性雄蜂在交配前还需要接受额外的审查。意义声明一些物种的雄蜂必须向配偶宣传自己的品质,但对于雌性物种来说,它们是否也必须向巢友宣传自己的品质呢?通过操纵蜜蜂雄蜂的质量,我们发现,体型小、免疫力差的雄蜂会在一夜之间被逐出蜂群。工蜂可能不会利用无人蜂的角质层碳氢化合物特征来进行这种评估。这是针对成年雄蜂的社会免疫力的一个新例子,也是工蜂参与生殖决策的一个例子。
{"title":"To house or oust: Honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies can evaluate and evict drones of low quality","authors":"Izaak R. Gilchrist, Jonathan M. Nixon, Riley R. Shultz, Matthew D. Ginzel, Brock A. Harpur","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03461-8","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Across the animal kingdom, males advertise their quality to potential mates. Males of low reproductive quality, such as those that are sick, may be excluded from mating. In eusocial species, there is some evidence that reproductive females gauge the quality of their mates. However, males often spend much more time with non-reproductive females when being raised or when returning from unsuccessful mating flights. Do non-reproductive workers evaluate the quality of male reproductives? Here we address this question using male honey bees (<i>Apis mellifera</i>), called drones, as a model. We generated immune-challenged drones by injecting them with lipopolysaccharide and tested: 1) do workers evict immune-challenged drones from their colony, 2) do cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profiles, body size, or mass change when drones are immune-challenged, and 3) are these changes used by workers to exclude low quality males from the colony? We found that an immune challenge causes changes in CHC profiles of drones and reduces their body mass. Workers selectively evict small and immune-challenged drones who, themselves, do not self-evict. This work demonstrates that some eusocial males undergo an additional layer of scrutiny prior to mating mediated by the non-reproductive worker caste.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Males of some species must advertise their quality to mates but, in the case of eusocial species, must they also advertise their quality to nestmates? By manipulating honey bee male quality, we found that small and immune-challenged drones are evicted from colonies overnight. Workers may not use a drone’s cuticular hydrocarbon profile to make this assessment. This is a new example of social immunity expressed against adult males and an example of worker involvement in reproductive decisions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581098","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Boldness and exploratory behaviors differ between sunfish (Lepomis spp.) congeners in a standardized assay 太阳鱼(Lepomis spp.)
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5
Kevin A. Adeli, Sam J. Woods, Steven J. Cooke, Chris K. Elvidge

The shy-bold behavioral continuum is an element of animal behavior which is often studied for its ecological relevance, particularly in the context of predation risk. How individuals respond to various predation cues is well studied at the individual level, but relatively little is known about how these responses can differ among closely related species. We exposed individual wild-caught juvenile bluegill (Lepomis macrochirus) and pumpkinseed (L. gibbosus) to kairomones of a common predator (Northern pike, Esox lucius), conspecific alarm cues, or a lake water control in a Z-maze trial commonly used to assess relative levels of bold and exploratory behaviors. Neither species exhibited any significant behavioral responses to either predation cue, but bluegill consistently displayed more bold-type behaviors than pumpkinseed. Although the lack of a behavioral response to predation cues in this study is equivocal, we identify clear differences in boldness between these two congeners and discuss the possibility of ecological niche differentiation driving these behavioral differences.

害羞-大胆行为的连续性是动物行为的一个要素,它经常被用来研究其生态相关性,尤其是在捕食风险的背景下。个体如何对各种捕食线索做出反应在个体水平上已经有了很好的研究,但对于这些反应在近亲物种之间如何不同,人们的了解相对较少。我们将野生捕获的幼年蓝鳃鱼(Lepomis macrochirus)和南瓜籽鱼(L. gibbosus)个体暴露于共同捕食者(北方梭子鱼,Esox lucius)的凯尔蒙、同种警报线索或湖水对照中,并进行了常用于评估大胆和探索行为相对水平的 Z 型迷宫试验。两种鱼对捕食线索都没有表现出明显的行为反应,但蓝鳃鱼始终比南瓜籽鱼表现出更多的大胆型行为。尽管这项研究中缺乏对捕食线索的行为反应,但我们发现这两种同源鱼类在大胆程度上存在明显差异,并讨论了生态位差异驱动这些行为差异的可能性。
{"title":"Boldness and exploratory behaviors differ between sunfish (Lepomis spp.) congeners in a standardized assay","authors":"Kevin A. Adeli, Sam J. Woods, Steven J. Cooke, Chris K. Elvidge","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03464-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The shy-bold behavioral continuum is an element of animal behavior which is often studied for its ecological relevance, particularly in the context of predation risk. How individuals respond to various predation cues is well studied at the individual level, but relatively little is known about how these responses can differ among closely related species. We exposed individual wild-caught juvenile bluegill (<i>Lepomis macrochirus</i>) and pumpkinseed (<i>L. gibbosus</i>) to kairomones of a common predator (Northern pike, <i>Esox lucius</i>), conspecific alarm cues, or a lake water control in a Z-maze trial commonly used to assess relative levels of bold and exploratory behaviors. Neither species exhibited any significant behavioral responses to either predation cue, but bluegill consistently displayed more bold-type behaviors than pumpkinseed. Although the lack of a behavioral response to predation cues in this study is equivocal, we identify clear differences in boldness between these two congeners and discuss the possibility of ecological niche differentiation driving these behavioral differences.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140580963","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Daily ranging and den usage patterns structure the spatiotemporal properties of social encounters in spotted hyenas 斑鬣狗的日常活动和巢穴使用模式决定了其社会交往的时空特性
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-03 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3

Abstract

Fission–fusion dynamics describe the tendency for members of some animal societies to associate in subgroups that change size and structure fluidly over time. These dynamics shape social complexity and social structure, but are difficult to study because they unfold simultaneously over large spatial scales. Here we use simultaneous, fine-scale GPS data from spotted hyenas to examine fission–fusion dynamics through a dyadic analysis of merge-split events between pairs of individuals. We introduce a species-agnostic framework for identifying merge-split events and discretizing them into three phases (merging, together, and splitting), enabling analysis of each phase as well as the connections among phases. Applying this framework to the hyena data, we examine the temporal and spatial properties of merges and splits between dyads and test the extent to which social encounters are driven by key locations. Specifically, we focus on communal dens—shelters for juvenile hyenas where classical observational studies often report large aggregations of adults. We find that overall, 62% of merges occurred at communal dens, supporting the idea that dens facilitate meet-ups and subsequent social behavior. Social encounters most commonly involved close approaches within a few meters between hyenas, while co-travel together occurred in only 11% of events. Comparison to permutation-based reference models suggests that independent movement decisions structure broad-scale patterns of social encounters but do not explain the fine-scale dynamics of interactions that unfold during these encounters. We reflect on how physical features such as dens can become social hotspots, causing social and spatial processes to become fundamentally intertwined.

摘要 裂变-融合动力学描述了一些动物社会的成员结成亚群的趋势,这些亚群的规模和结构随着时间的推移而不断变化。这些动态变化决定了社会复杂性和社会结构,但由于它们在大空间尺度上同时展开,因此很难研究。在这里,我们利用斑鬣狗的同步、精细尺度 GPS 数据,通过对一对个体之间的合并-分裂事件进行二元分析,来研究裂变-融合动态。我们引入了一个与物种无关的框架来识别合并-分裂事件,并将其离散化为三个阶段(合并、在一起和分裂),从而能够分析每个阶段以及各阶段之间的联系。我们将这一框架应用于鬣狗数据中,研究了二元组之间合并和分裂的时间和空间特性,并检验了社交相遇在多大程度上受关键地点的驱动。具体来说,我们将重点放在幼年鬣狗的公共巢穴--庇护所上,经典的观察研究通常会报告成年鬣狗在这里大量聚集。我们发现,总体而言,62%的会合发生在公共巢穴,这支持了巢穴有助于会合和随后的社会行为的观点。社交相遇最常见的情况是鬣狗之间几米内的近距离接触,而共同旅行的情况仅占 11%。与基于排列组合的参考模型的比较表明,独立的运动决定构建了社会相遇的大尺度模式,但并不能解释相遇期间展开的细尺度互动动态。我们思考了巢穴等物理特征如何成为社会热点,导致社会和空间过程从根本上交织在一起。
{"title":"Daily ranging and den usage patterns structure the spatiotemporal properties of social encounters in spotted hyenas","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03458-3","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Fission–fusion dynamics describe the tendency for members of some animal societies to associate in subgroups that change size and structure fluidly over time. These dynamics shape social complexity and social structure, but are difficult to study because they unfold simultaneously over large spatial scales. Here we use simultaneous, fine-scale GPS data from spotted hyenas to examine fission–fusion dynamics through a dyadic analysis of <em>merge-split events</em> between pairs of individuals. We introduce a species-agnostic framework for identifying merge-split events and discretizing them into three phases (merging, together, and splitting), enabling analysis of each phase as well as the connections among phases. Applying this framework to the hyena data, we examine the temporal and spatial properties of merges and splits between dyads and test the extent to which social encounters are driven by key locations. Specifically, we focus on communal dens—shelters for juvenile hyenas where classical observational studies often report large aggregations of adults. We find that overall, 62% of merges occurred at communal dens, supporting the idea that dens facilitate meet-ups and subsequent social behavior. Social encounters most commonly involved close approaches within a few meters between hyenas, while co-travel together occurred in only 11% of events. Comparison to permutation-based reference models suggests that independent movement decisions structure broad-scale patterns of social encounters but do not explain the fine-scale dynamics of interactions that unfold during these encounters. We reflect on how physical features such as dens can become social hotspots, causing social and spatial processes to become fundamentally intertwined.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140581094","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Geographic variation and discrimination of social calls in male great himalayan leaf-nosed bats: do functionally similar calls have similar patterns? 雄性大喜马拉雅叶鼻蝠社交性叫声的地理变异和辨别:功能相似的叫声是否具有相似的模式?
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03459-2
Chunmian Zhang, Ziqi Zheng, Jeffrey R. Lucas, Jiang Feng, Congnan Sun, Tinglei Jiang

Geographical variation in animals’ acoustic signals has received much attention. However, few studies have compared the patterns and underlying selective forces driving geographical divergence of vocalizations with similar and different functions within the same species. Also, the social consequences of geographical divergence in acoustic signals are still rather poorly understood. Here we recorded three types of social calls of male great Himalayan leaf-nosed bats (Hipposideros armiger) across eight colonies in China. Two calls share similar functions and the third has a function distinct from the other two. We examined the patterns and causes of geographical variation of each of these calls. We found that all three calls had significant geographic variation with similar patterns of spatial variation. Only one of the two social calls with similar functions was found to be affected by genetic drift, while the other two calls were not affected by selection, drift, or morphological constraints. Furthermore, we found that bats could discriminate between vocalizations of their own colony and those of an allopatric colony. Overall, these results suggest that acoustic signals with similar functions may be shaped by different driving forces and acoustic signals with different functions may exhibit similar geographical patterns. This study expands our limited knowledge of the patterns of geographical variation of vocalizations emitted at different emotional states and highlights the importance of comparing simultaneously patterns and causes of geographical divergence of vocalizations with similar and different functions.

动物声音信号的地理差异已受到广泛关注。然而,很少有研究对同一物种中功能相似和不同的发声的地理差异模式和潜在的选择性力量进行比较。此外,人们对声学信号的地理差异所产生的社会后果还知之甚少。在这里,我们记录了雄性喜马拉雅叶鼻蝠(Hipposideros armiger)在中国八个群落中的三种社会性叫声。两种叫声具有相似的功能,而第三种叫声的功能则与其他两种叫声不同。我们研究了每种叫声的地理变异模式和原因。我们发现,这三种叫声都有显著的地理差异,而且空间差异模式相似。在功能相似的两种社会性鸣叫中,只有一种受到遗传漂移的影响,而另外两种鸣叫则没有受到选择、漂移或形态限制的影响。此外,我们还发现,蝙蝠可以区分自己群体的叫声和同域群体的叫声。总之,这些结果表明,具有相似功能的声音信号可能受到不同驱动力的影响,而具有不同功能的声音信号可能表现出相似的地理模式。这项研究拓展了我们对不同情绪状态下发出的声音的地理变异模式的有限认识,并强调了同时比较功能相似和功能不同的声音的地理变异模式和原因的重要性。
{"title":"Geographic variation and discrimination of social calls in male great himalayan leaf-nosed bats: do functionally similar calls have similar patterns?","authors":"Chunmian Zhang, Ziqi Zheng, Jeffrey R. Lucas, Jiang Feng, Congnan Sun, Tinglei Jiang","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03459-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03459-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Geographical variation in animals’ acoustic signals has received much attention. However, few studies have compared the patterns and underlying selective forces driving geographical divergence of vocalizations with similar and different functions within the same species. Also, the social consequences of geographical divergence in acoustic signals are still rather poorly understood. Here we recorded three types of social calls of male great Himalayan leaf-nosed bats (<i>Hipposideros armiger</i>) across eight colonies in China. Two calls share similar functions and the third has a function distinct from the other two. We examined the patterns and causes of geographical variation of each of these calls. We found that all three calls had significant geographic variation with similar patterns of spatial variation. Only one of the two social calls with similar functions was found to be affected by genetic drift, while the other two calls were not affected by selection, drift, or morphological constraints. Furthermore, we found that bats could discriminate between vocalizations of their own colony and those of an allopatric colony. Overall, these results suggest that acoustic signals with similar functions may be shaped by different driving forces and acoustic signals with different functions may exhibit similar geographical patterns. This study expands our limited knowledge of the patterns of geographical variation of vocalizations emitted at different emotional states and highlights the importance of comparing simultaneously patterns and causes of geographical divergence of vocalizations with similar and different functions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140313850","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Background contrast affects detection and recognition of courting wolf spiders by intended and unintended receivers 背景对比度影响有意和无意接收者对求偶狼蛛的检测和识别
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03453-8
George W. Uetz, Rachel Gilbert, Rebecca Wilson, Jacqueline Mann, Joseph Macedonia, David L. Clark

We used video playback of courting male Schizocosa ocreata wolf spiders to examine responses of intended receivers (conspecific females) and eavesdroppers (competitor males, predatory spiders, toads) to manipulations of spider color (natural color, monochromatic gray, monochromatic RBG average) displayed against complex leaf litter backgrounds (color, grayscale). Models of chromatic and achromatic contrast between spider stimuli and backgrounds were used to predict receiver responses. The results support the hypothesis that interactions between spider and background coloration affect detection and recognition, although responses varied with receiver type. Detection responses of intended receivers (female S. ocreata) did not fit predictions of the chromatic contrast model in some cases, but showed a fair fit to the achromatic model. Detection responses of social eavesdroppers (male S. ocreata) fit the chromatic and achromatic contrast models slightly better than did female responses (poor fit and very good fit, respectively). Eavesdropping wolf spider predators (Rabidosa) exhibited detection responses that significantly matched predictions of the chromatic (very good fit) and achromatic (excellent fit) models. Whereas jumping spiders (Phidippus) showed a good fit to the chromatic and achromatic contrast models, toad predators had a good fit only to the chromatic model. Recognition responses revealed a different pattern of fit to the chromatic and achromatic models across receiver types, although Rabidosa again indicated a significant fit to both models. Taken together, the results of this study identify both chromatic and achromatic features of spider appearance as likely explanations for differences in behavioral responses of intended and unintended receivers. This outcome suggests the possibility that both sexual and natural selection likely target different features of male appearance during courtship.

我们利用狼蛛求偶雄蛛的视频回放,研究了目标接收者(同种雌蛛)和窃听者(竞争雄蛛、捕食蛛、蟾蜍)对复杂叶丛背景(彩色、灰度)下蜘蛛颜色(自然色、单色灰色、单色 RBG 平均值)的反应。蜘蛛刺激物和背景之间的色度和消色差模型被用来预测接收者的反应。结果支持蜘蛛与背景颜色之间的相互作用会影响检测和识别的假设,尽管反应随接收者类型的不同而不同。意向接收者(雌性 S. ocreata)的检测反应在某些情况下不符合色度对比模型的预测,但与消色差模型相当吻合。社会窃听者(雄性狼蛛)的探测反应与色度和消色差对比度模型的拟合程度略好于雌性狼蛛(分别为拟合程度较差和拟合程度很好)。偷听的狼蛛捕食者(Rabidosa)的探测反应与色度模型(拟合非常好)和消色差模型(拟合非常好)的预测非常吻合。跳蛛(Phidippus)与色度和消色差对比度模型的拟合度良好,而蟾蜍捕食者仅与色度模型拟合度良好。不同类型的接收者对色度和消色差模型的识别反应显示出不同的拟合模式,尽管拉比多萨(Rabidosa)再次显示出对两种模型的显著拟合。综上所述,本研究的结果表明,蜘蛛外观的色度和消色差特征都有可能解释有意接收者和无意接收者行为反应的差异。这一结果表明,在求偶过程中,性选择和自然选择可能会针对雄性外观的不同特征。
{"title":"Background contrast affects detection and recognition of courting wolf spiders by intended and unintended receivers","authors":"George W. Uetz, Rachel Gilbert, Rebecca Wilson, Jacqueline Mann, Joseph Macedonia, David L. Clark","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03453-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03453-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We used video playback of courting male <i>Schizocosa ocreata</i> wolf spiders to examine responses of intended receivers (conspecific females) and eavesdroppers (competitor males, predatory spiders, toads) to manipulations of spider color (natural color, monochromatic gray, monochromatic RBG average) displayed against complex leaf litter backgrounds (color, grayscale). Models of chromatic and achromatic contrast between spider stimuli and backgrounds were used to predict receiver responses. The results support the hypothesis that interactions between spider and background coloration affect detection and recognition, although responses varied with receiver type. Detection responses of intended receivers (female <i>S. ocreata</i>) did not fit predictions of the chromatic contrast model in some cases, but showed a fair fit to the achromatic model. Detection responses of social eavesdroppers (male <i>S. ocreata</i>) fit the chromatic and achromatic contrast models slightly better than did female responses (poor fit and very good fit, respectively). Eavesdropping wolf spider predators (<i>Rabidosa</i>) exhibited detection responses that significantly matched predictions of the chromatic (very good fit) and achromatic (excellent fit) models. Whereas jumping spiders (<i>Phidippus</i>) showed a good fit to the chromatic and achromatic contrast models, toad predators had a good fit only to the chromatic model. Recognition responses revealed a different pattern of fit to the chromatic and achromatic models across receiver types, although <i>Rabidosa</i> again indicated a significant fit to both models. Taken together, the results of this study identify both chromatic and achromatic features of spider appearance as likely explanations for differences in behavioral responses of intended and unintended receivers. This outcome suggests the possibility that both sexual and natural selection likely target different features of male appearance during courtship.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140199737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Vocal and genetic variation between a land-bridge island and mainland populations of the Black-crowned Antshrike (Thamnophilus atrinucha) 黑冠长尾雉(Thamnophilus atrinucha)陆桥岛屿种群与大陆种群之间的声音和遗传变异
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6

Abstract

Island-mainland systems serve as natural laboratories for studying communication signals. We explored potential divergence in the song of the black-crowned antshrike between Gorgona Island, a land-bridge island off the Pacific coast of Colombia, and several mainland populations. We found that the perching song of this species, the so called loudsong, was distinctly different on Gorgona Island. This differentiation encompassed an increase in frequency bandwidth, a decrease in vocal performance, and a reduction in song duration. All are consistent with documented patterns of divergence observed in avian acoustic signals on oceanic islands. The observed distinctions in loudsong acoustics, including variations in frequency and temporal features, led to the inference that the population on Gorgona Island produces the least complex loudsong dialect across the entire species range. This pattern of how an innate vocalization manifests within a land-bridge island, which was connected to the mainland not long ago, lends support to the Character Release Hypothesis. To ensure an unbiased comparison, we considered genetic clusters and analyzed them by comparing ten acoustic signals between the land-bridge island and several mainland populations. Combined with the gene genealogy, this finding supports the validity of two geographic forms: Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha and Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae (island). However, they also reveal the existence of additional unrecognized dialects of loudsong that transcend current taxonomic classifications. We provide evidence that challenges the long-standing presumption suggesting minimal intraspecific geographic variation in the vocalization among non-vocal learning species.

Significance statement

Significant evolutionary implications exist for an intriguing pattern of variation of the loudsong behavior in the Black-crowned Antshrike over a land-bridge island. Our gene tree does provide evidence of the enormous haplotypic diversity accumulated in the trans-Andean region long after the final uplift of the northern Andes. The acoustic divergence of the loudsong suggests substantial geographic variation within a non-vocal learning suboscine. Genetic and loudsong divergence on a land-bridge island add to document that evolution of acoustic traits may be relaxed in islands as compared to those observed on the species-rich mainland. The island-mainland system is one of the few definitive biogeographic contexts for understanding evolution of acoustic behavior.

摘要 岛屿-大陆系统是研究通讯信号的天然实验室。我们研究了哥伦比亚太平洋沿岸的一个陆桥岛屿戈尔戈纳岛(Gorgona Island)与几个大陆种群之间黑冠滨鹬鸣声的潜在差异。我们发现,在戈尔戈纳岛上,该物种的栖息鸣唱(即所谓的 "云歌")明显不同。这种差异包括频率带宽的增加、发声性能的下降和鸣唱持续时间的缩短。所有这些都与在海洋岛屿上观察到的鸟类声学信号的分化模式相一致。观察到的扬声声学差异,包括频率和时间特征的变化,推断出戈尔戈纳岛的种群在整个物种范围内发出的扬声方言复杂程度最低。这种先天发声在不久前与大陆相连的陆桥岛屿上的表现模式为 "特征释放假说 "提供了支持。为了确保比较的公正性,我们考虑了基因群,并通过比较陆桥岛和几个大陆种群之间的十个声学信号对其进行了分析。结合基因谱系,这一发现支持了两种地理形态的有效性:Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha 和 Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae(岛屿)。然而,这些研究还揭示了存在其他未被认可的方言,这些方言超越了目前的分类学分类。我们提供的证据挑战了长期以来的假定,即非发声学习物种的发声在种内地理上的差异极小。 意义声明 黑冠长尾雉在陆桥岛屿上的引人入胜的扬声行为变异模式具有重要的进化意义。我们的基因树确实提供了证据,证明在安第斯山脉北部最终隆起之后的很长一段时间里,跨安第斯山脉地区积累了巨大的单倍型多样性。扬声的声学分化表明,在非发声学习亚目中存在着大量的地理变异。一个陆桥岛屿上的遗传和扬声分化进一步证明,与物种丰富的大陆相比,岛屿上声学特征的进化可能较为宽松。岛屿-大陆系统是了解声学行为进化的少数明确的生物地理背景之一。
{"title":"Vocal and genetic variation between a land-bridge island and mainland populations of the Black-crowned Antshrike (Thamnophilus atrinucha)","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6","url":null,"abstract":"<span> <h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Island-mainland systems serve as natural laboratories for studying communication signals. We explored potential divergence in the song of the black-crowned antshrike between Gorgona Island, a land-bridge island off the Pacific coast of Colombia, and several mainland populations. We found that the perching song of this species, the so called loudsong, was distinctly different on Gorgona Island. This differentiation encompassed an increase in frequency bandwidth, a decrease in vocal performance, and a reduction in song duration. All are consistent with documented patterns of divergence observed in avian acoustic signals on oceanic islands. The observed distinctions in loudsong acoustics, including variations in frequency and temporal features, led to the inference that the population on Gorgona Island produces the least complex loudsong dialect across the entire species range. This pattern of how an innate vocalization manifests within a land-bridge island, which was connected to the mainland not long ago, lends support to the Character Release Hypothesis. To ensure an unbiased comparison, we considered genetic clusters and analyzed them by comparing ten acoustic signals between the land-bridge island and several mainland populations. Combined with the gene genealogy, this finding supports the validity of two geographic forms: <em>Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha</em> and <em>Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae</em> (island). However, they also reveal the existence of additional unrecognized dialects of loudsong that transcend current taxonomic classifications. We provide evidence that challenges the long-standing presumption suggesting minimal intraspecific geographic variation in the vocalization among non-vocal learning species.</p> </span> <span> <h3>Significance statement</h3> <p>Significant evolutionary implications exist for an intriguing pattern of variation of the loudsong behavior in the Black-crowned Antshrike over a land-bridge island. Our gene tree does provide evidence of the enormous haplotypic diversity accumulated in the <em>trans</em>-Andean region long after the final uplift of the northern Andes. The acoustic divergence of the loudsong suggests substantial geographic variation within a non-vocal learning suboscine. Genetic and loudsong divergence on a land-bridge island add to document that evolution of acoustic traits may be relaxed in islands as compared to those observed on the species-rich mainland. The island-mainland system is one of the few definitive biogeographic contexts for understanding evolution of acoustic behavior.</p> </span>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140205580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Iberian wolf’s diet and its quality during breeding season: exploring the influence of zone, wolf groups, prey availability and individual factors 伊比利亚狼繁殖季节的饮食及其质量:探讨区域、狼群、猎物可获得性和个体因素的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4
Isabel Barja, Ana Piñeiro, Javier Talegón, Aritz Ruiz-González, Álvaro Navarro-Castilla, Amaia Caro, Toni Gago-Barja, M. Carmen Hernández

Abstract

Large predators are essential in maintaining ecosystem functioning, and comprehending how their feeding habits change across natural and human-dominated landscapes is crucial to preserve biodiversity. In this study, the diet of Iberian wolves (Canis lupus signatus) during pup rearing season (July to September) has been studied in relation to prey abundance and putting emphasis in the analysis of the differences between zones, wolf groups and individual factors (age, sex and social status). For this, non-invasive monitoring was carried out in three zones of Spain where nine different wolf breeding groups were detected (Galicia, n = 4; Zamora, n = 4 and Valladolid, n = 1). Faecal samples were collected near breeding sites for dietary and genetic analyses, registering if it was or not a scent mark to know the social status of the individuals. Prey availability was determined by camera trapping or requesting the official census of wild prey in the study areas. We found differences in wolf’s diet depending on the zone and the breeding group however, the diet did not vary depending on the age, sex and social status. In general, Iberian wolves mainly fed on wild ungulates (wild boar, roe deer and red deer), feeding on the most abundant prey, except for Baldriz group in Galicia which seems to be specialized in hunting roe deer. Domestic animals’ consumption (sheep, goat, donkey, pig) was not high, but it occurred specially in agriculture and livestock areas (Ferreras in Zamora and Valladolid) where wild prey were less available.

Significance statement

In this study, we shed light on the vital role of Iberian wolves in the ecosystem by investigating the dietary preferences during pup rearing season across different landscapes. Our research revealed differences in wolf diets based on geographic location and breeding groups, emphasizing their adaptability. We found that age, sex, and social status did not significantly influence their dietary preferences. Iberian wolves predominantly target wild ungulates, except for a specialized group in Galicia. Our study introduces a novel approach as it is the first to analyse wolf diet based on individual factors and using nitrogen analysis. Additionally, the study highlights previously overlooked dietary patterns of the Iberian wolf subspecies, despite its critical role in the ecosystem as a top predator. Furthermore, we emphasize the necessity of conserving wild ungulate populations to reduce livestock attacks by ensuring a stable supply of wild prey, promoting coexistence between humans and wolves.

摘要 大型食肉动物是维持生态系统功能的关键,了解它们的食性在自然和人类主导的景观中是如何变化的,对于保护生物多样性至关重要。在这项研究中,研究了伊比利亚狼(Canis lupus signatus)在幼狼饲养季节(7 月至 9 月)的饮食与猎物丰度的关系,并重点分析了不同区域、狼群和个体因素(年龄、性别和社会地位)之间的差异。为此,在西班牙的三个地区进行了非侵入式监测,发现了九个不同的狼繁殖群(加利西亚,n = 4;萨莫拉,n = 4;巴利亚多利德,n = 1)。在繁殖地附近采集了粪便样本,用于饮食和基因分析,并登记是否有气味标记,以了解个体的社会地位。通过相机诱捕或请求官方普查研究区域内的野生猎物来确定猎物的可用性。我们发现,狼的食谱因地区和繁殖群体而异,但食谱并不因年龄、性别和社会地位而异。一般来说,伊比利亚狼主要以野生有蹄类动物(野猪、狍子和红鹿)为食,捕食数量最多的猎物,但加利西亚的巴尔德里斯狼群除外,该狼群似乎专门捕食狍子。家畜(绵羊、山羊、驴、猪)的消费量并不高,但在野生猎物较少的农业和畜牧业地区(萨莫拉的费雷拉斯和巴利亚多利德)尤其如此。我们的研究揭示了不同地理位置和繁殖群体的狼的饮食差异,强调了它们的适应能力。我们发现,年龄、性别和社会地位对它们的饮食偏好没有显著影响。除了加利西亚的一个特殊群体外,伊比利亚狼主要以野生蹄类动物为目标。我们的研究引入了一种新的方法,因为这是首次根据个体因素和氮分析来分析狼的饮食。此外,尽管伊比利亚狼亚种作为顶级掠食者在生态系统中扮演着重要角色,但这项研究强调了以前被忽视的伊比利亚狼亚种的饮食模式。此外,我们还强调了保护野生羚羊种群的必要性,通过确保稳定的野生猎物供应来减少对牲畜的攻击,从而促进人类与狼的共存。
{"title":"Iberian wolf’s diet and its quality during breeding season: exploring the influence of zone, wolf groups, prey availability and individual factors","authors":"Isabel Barja, Ana Piñeiro, Javier Talegón, Aritz Ruiz-González, Álvaro Navarro-Castilla, Amaia Caro, Toni Gago-Barja, M. Carmen Hernández","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Large predators are essential in maintaining ecosystem functioning, and comprehending how their feeding habits change across natural and human-dominated landscapes is crucial to preserve biodiversity. In this study, the diet of Iberian wolves (<i>Canis lupus signatus</i>) during pup rearing season (July to September) has been studied in relation to prey abundance and putting emphasis in the analysis of the differences between zones, wolf groups and individual factors (age, sex and social status). For this, non-invasive monitoring was carried out in three zones of Spain where nine different wolf breeding groups were detected (Galicia, <i>n</i> = 4; Zamora, <i>n</i> = 4 and Valladolid, <i>n</i> = 1). Faecal samples were collected near breeding sites for dietary and genetic analyses, registering if it was or not a scent mark to know the social status of the individuals. Prey availability was determined by camera trapping or requesting the official census of wild prey in the study areas. We found differences in wolf’s diet depending on the zone and the breeding group however, the diet did not vary depending on the age, sex and social status. In general, Iberian wolves mainly fed on wild ungulates (wild boar, roe deer and red deer), feeding on the most abundant prey, except for Baldriz group in Galicia which seems to be specialized in hunting roe deer. Domestic animals’ consumption (sheep, goat, donkey, pig) was not high, but it occurred specially in agriculture and livestock areas (Ferreras in Zamora and Valladolid) where wild prey were less available.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>In this study, we shed light on the vital role of Iberian wolves in the ecosystem by investigating the dietary preferences during pup rearing season across different landscapes. Our research revealed differences in wolf diets based on geographic location and breeding groups, emphasizing their adaptability. We found that age, sex, and social status did not significantly influence their dietary preferences. Iberian wolves predominantly target wild ungulates, except for a specialized group in Galicia. Our study introduces a novel approach as it is the first to analyse wolf diet based on individual factors and using nitrogen analysis. Additionally, the study highlights previously overlooked dietary patterns of the Iberian wolf subspecies, despite its critical role in the ecosystem as a top predator. Furthermore, we emphasize the necessity of conserving wild ungulate populations to reduce livestock attacks by ensuring a stable supply of wild prey, promoting coexistence between humans and wolves.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140199702","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between preference and switching in flower foraging by bees 蜜蜂觅花过程中的偏好与转换之间的关系
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5
Daniel R. Papaj, Avery L. Russell

It seems self-evident that generalist foragers switch more between resources than specialists but despite diverse ecological and evolutionary implications, how variation in switching relates to variation in preference warrants additional study. Here we tested predictions based on a simple probability model, using flower-foraging bees as a model system. In laboratory assays, we presented bumble bee (Bombus impatiens) workers with flowers of two species, Tecoma stans and T. alata, from which they could collect nectar and/or pollen. We quantified landing preference and occurrence of switching between species in successive visits. Bees varied greatly in floral preference. Almost half showed statistically significant preferences for one or the other species, while the rest were generalists in preference. As expected, generalists using both flower species switched more in successive visits than bees that were more specialized, a pattern fit to a quadratic function. However, generalist individuals switched more than expected based on null expectation. A Modified Jacob’s Index (MJI) of switching was significantly positively correlated with degree of preference: generalist bees had more negative MJI’s than specialist bees, indicating that even after the expected statistical effect of preference on switching was accounted for, they switched more than specialists. A simulation ruled out the possibility that the pattern was due to bias in MJI. Generalist-specialist differences in which food was collected (nectar versus pollen) were also ruled out. We offer possible explanations for our observed pattern and advocate consideration of preference and switching throughout behavioral ecology.

通才觅食者比专才觅食者在不同资源之间的转换更多,这似乎是不言而喻的,但尽管存在多种生态和进化影响,转换的变异与偏好的变异之间的关系如何还需要进一步研究。在这里,我们以觅食花卉的蜜蜂为模型系统,检验了基于简单概率模型的预测。在实验室试验中,我们向熊蜂(Bombus impatiens)工蜂展示了两种花(Tecoma stans 和 T. alata),它们可以从中采集花蜜和/或花粉。我们对蜜蜂的着陆偏好以及在连续访问中切换花种的情况进行了量化。蜜蜂对花的偏好差异很大。据统计,近一半的蜜蜂表现出对一种或另一种物种的偏好,而其余的蜜蜂则是偏好通才。不出所料,使用两种花卉的通才比专一性更强的蜜蜂在连续访问中转换的次数更多,这种模式符合二次函数。然而,基于空期望,通才个体的转换比预期的要多。切换的修正雅各布指数(MJI)与偏好程度呈显著的正相关:通才蜂比专才蜂有更多的负MJI,这表明即使考虑了偏好对切换的预期统计影响,通才蜂的切换也比专才蜂多。模拟排除了这种模式是由于 MJI 偏差造成的可能性。此外,还排除了普通蜜蜂与特种蜜蜂在采集食物(花蜜还是花粉)上的差异。我们为观察到的模式提供了可能的解释,并提倡在整个行为生态学中考虑偏好和转换。
{"title":"The relationship between preference and switching in flower foraging by bees","authors":"Daniel R. Papaj, Avery L. Russell","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It seems self-evident that generalist foragers switch more between resources than specialists but despite diverse ecological and evolutionary implications, how variation in switching relates to variation in preference warrants additional study. Here we tested predictions based on a simple probability model, using flower-foraging bees as a model system. In laboratory assays, we presented bumble bee (<i>Bombus impatiens</i>) workers with flowers of two species, <i>Tecoma stans</i> and <i>T. alata</i>, from which they could collect nectar and/or pollen. We quantified landing preference and occurrence of switching between species in successive visits. Bees varied greatly in floral preference. Almost half showed statistically significant preferences for one or the other species, while the rest were generalists in preference. As expected, generalists using both flower species switched more in successive visits than bees that were more specialized, a pattern fit to a quadratic function. However, generalist individuals switched more than expected based on null expectation. A Modified Jacob’s Index (MJI) of switching was significantly positively correlated with degree of preference: generalist bees had more negative MJI’s than specialist bees, indicating that even after the expected statistical effect of preference on switching was accounted for, they switched more than specialists. A simulation ruled out the possibility that the pattern was due to bias in MJI. Generalist-specialist differences in which food was collected (nectar versus pollen) were also ruled out. We offer possible explanations for our observed pattern and advocate consideration of preference and switching throughout behavioral ecology.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140166994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1