首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology最新文献

英文 中文
Vocal and genetic variation between a land-bridge island and mainland populations of the Black-crowned Antshrike (Thamnophilus atrinucha) 黑冠长尾雉(Thamnophilus atrinucha)陆桥岛屿种群与大陆种群之间的声音和遗传变异
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6

Abstract

Island-mainland systems serve as natural laboratories for studying communication signals. We explored potential divergence in the song of the black-crowned antshrike between Gorgona Island, a land-bridge island off the Pacific coast of Colombia, and several mainland populations. We found that the perching song of this species, the so called loudsong, was distinctly different on Gorgona Island. This differentiation encompassed an increase in frequency bandwidth, a decrease in vocal performance, and a reduction in song duration. All are consistent with documented patterns of divergence observed in avian acoustic signals on oceanic islands. The observed distinctions in loudsong acoustics, including variations in frequency and temporal features, led to the inference that the population on Gorgona Island produces the least complex loudsong dialect across the entire species range. This pattern of how an innate vocalization manifests within a land-bridge island, which was connected to the mainland not long ago, lends support to the Character Release Hypothesis. To ensure an unbiased comparison, we considered genetic clusters and analyzed them by comparing ten acoustic signals between the land-bridge island and several mainland populations. Combined with the gene genealogy, this finding supports the validity of two geographic forms: Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha and Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae (island). However, they also reveal the existence of additional unrecognized dialects of loudsong that transcend current taxonomic classifications. We provide evidence that challenges the long-standing presumption suggesting minimal intraspecific geographic variation in the vocalization among non-vocal learning species.

Significance statement

Significant evolutionary implications exist for an intriguing pattern of variation of the loudsong behavior in the Black-crowned Antshrike over a land-bridge island. Our gene tree does provide evidence of the enormous haplotypic diversity accumulated in the trans-Andean region long after the final uplift of the northern Andes. The acoustic divergence of the loudsong suggests substantial geographic variation within a non-vocal learning suboscine. Genetic and loudsong divergence on a land-bridge island add to document that evolution of acoustic traits may be relaxed in islands as compared to those observed on the species-rich mainland. The island-mainland system is one of the few definitive biogeographic contexts for understanding evolution of acoustic behavior.

摘要 岛屿-大陆系统是研究通讯信号的天然实验室。我们研究了哥伦比亚太平洋沿岸的一个陆桥岛屿戈尔戈纳岛(Gorgona Island)与几个大陆种群之间黑冠滨鹬鸣声的潜在差异。我们发现,在戈尔戈纳岛上,该物种的栖息鸣唱(即所谓的 "云歌")明显不同。这种差异包括频率带宽的增加、发声性能的下降和鸣唱持续时间的缩短。所有这些都与在海洋岛屿上观察到的鸟类声学信号的分化模式相一致。观察到的扬声声学差异,包括频率和时间特征的变化,推断出戈尔戈纳岛的种群在整个物种范围内发出的扬声方言复杂程度最低。这种先天发声在不久前与大陆相连的陆桥岛屿上的表现模式为 "特征释放假说 "提供了支持。为了确保比较的公正性,我们考虑了基因群,并通过比较陆桥岛和几个大陆种群之间的十个声学信号对其进行了分析。结合基因谱系,这一发现支持了两种地理形态的有效性:Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha 和 Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae(岛屿)。然而,这些研究还揭示了存在其他未被认可的方言,这些方言超越了目前的分类学分类。我们提供的证据挑战了长期以来的假定,即非发声学习物种的发声在种内地理上的差异极小。 意义声明 黑冠长尾雉在陆桥岛屿上的引人入胜的扬声行为变异模式具有重要的进化意义。我们的基因树确实提供了证据,证明在安第斯山脉北部最终隆起之后的很长一段时间里,跨安第斯山脉地区积累了巨大的单倍型多样性。扬声的声学分化表明,在非发声学习亚目中存在着大量的地理变异。一个陆桥岛屿上的遗传和扬声分化进一步证明,与物种丰富的大陆相比,岛屿上声学特征的进化可能较为宽松。岛屿-大陆系统是了解声学行为进化的少数明确的生物地理背景之一。
{"title":"Vocal and genetic variation between a land-bridge island and mainland populations of the Black-crowned Antshrike (Thamnophilus atrinucha)","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03455-6","url":null,"abstract":"<span> <h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Island-mainland systems serve as natural laboratories for studying communication signals. We explored potential divergence in the song of the black-crowned antshrike between Gorgona Island, a land-bridge island off the Pacific coast of Colombia, and several mainland populations. We found that the perching song of this species, the so called loudsong, was distinctly different on Gorgona Island. This differentiation encompassed an increase in frequency bandwidth, a decrease in vocal performance, and a reduction in song duration. All are consistent with documented patterns of divergence observed in avian acoustic signals on oceanic islands. The observed distinctions in loudsong acoustics, including variations in frequency and temporal features, led to the inference that the population on Gorgona Island produces the least complex loudsong dialect across the entire species range. This pattern of how an innate vocalization manifests within a land-bridge island, which was connected to the mainland not long ago, lends support to the Character Release Hypothesis. To ensure an unbiased comparison, we considered genetic clusters and analyzed them by comparing ten acoustic signals between the land-bridge island and several mainland populations. Combined with the gene genealogy, this finding supports the validity of two geographic forms: <em>Thamnophilus atrinucha atrinucha</em> and <em>Thamnophilus atrinucha gorgonae</em> (island). However, they also reveal the existence of additional unrecognized dialects of loudsong that transcend current taxonomic classifications. We provide evidence that challenges the long-standing presumption suggesting minimal intraspecific geographic variation in the vocalization among non-vocal learning species.</p> </span> <span> <h3>Significance statement</h3> <p>Significant evolutionary implications exist for an intriguing pattern of variation of the loudsong behavior in the Black-crowned Antshrike over a land-bridge island. Our gene tree does provide evidence of the enormous haplotypic diversity accumulated in the <em>trans</em>-Andean region long after the final uplift of the northern Andes. The acoustic divergence of the loudsong suggests substantial geographic variation within a non-vocal learning suboscine. Genetic and loudsong divergence on a land-bridge island add to document that evolution of acoustic traits may be relaxed in islands as compared to those observed on the species-rich mainland. The island-mainland system is one of the few definitive biogeographic contexts for understanding evolution of acoustic behavior.</p> </span>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"229 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140205580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Iberian wolf’s diet and its quality during breeding season: exploring the influence of zone, wolf groups, prey availability and individual factors 伊比利亚狼繁殖季节的饮食及其质量:探讨区域、狼群、猎物可获得性和个体因素的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4
Isabel Barja, Ana Piñeiro, Javier Talegón, Aritz Ruiz-González, Álvaro Navarro-Castilla, Amaia Caro, Toni Gago-Barja, M. Carmen Hernández

Abstract

Large predators are essential in maintaining ecosystem functioning, and comprehending how their feeding habits change across natural and human-dominated landscapes is crucial to preserve biodiversity. In this study, the diet of Iberian wolves (Canis lupus signatus) during pup rearing season (July to September) has been studied in relation to prey abundance and putting emphasis in the analysis of the differences between zones, wolf groups and individual factors (age, sex and social status). For this, non-invasive monitoring was carried out in three zones of Spain where nine different wolf breeding groups were detected (Galicia, n = 4; Zamora, n = 4 and Valladolid, n = 1). Faecal samples were collected near breeding sites for dietary and genetic analyses, registering if it was or not a scent mark to know the social status of the individuals. Prey availability was determined by camera trapping or requesting the official census of wild prey in the study areas. We found differences in wolf’s diet depending on the zone and the breeding group however, the diet did not vary depending on the age, sex and social status. In general, Iberian wolves mainly fed on wild ungulates (wild boar, roe deer and red deer), feeding on the most abundant prey, except for Baldriz group in Galicia which seems to be specialized in hunting roe deer. Domestic animals’ consumption (sheep, goat, donkey, pig) was not high, but it occurred specially in agriculture and livestock areas (Ferreras in Zamora and Valladolid) where wild prey were less available.

Significance statement

In this study, we shed light on the vital role of Iberian wolves in the ecosystem by investigating the dietary preferences during pup rearing season across different landscapes. Our research revealed differences in wolf diets based on geographic location and breeding groups, emphasizing their adaptability. We found that age, sex, and social status did not significantly influence their dietary preferences. Iberian wolves predominantly target wild ungulates, except for a specialized group in Galicia. Our study introduces a novel approach as it is the first to analyse wolf diet based on individual factors and using nitrogen analysis. Additionally, the study highlights previously overlooked dietary patterns of the Iberian wolf subspecies, despite its critical role in the ecosystem as a top predator. Furthermore, we emphasize the necessity of conserving wild ungulate populations to reduce livestock attacks by ensuring a stable supply of wild prey, promoting coexistence between humans and wolves.

摘要 大型食肉动物是维持生态系统功能的关键,了解它们的食性在自然和人类主导的景观中是如何变化的,对于保护生物多样性至关重要。在这项研究中,研究了伊比利亚狼(Canis lupus signatus)在幼狼饲养季节(7 月至 9 月)的饮食与猎物丰度的关系,并重点分析了不同区域、狼群和个体因素(年龄、性别和社会地位)之间的差异。为此,在西班牙的三个地区进行了非侵入式监测,发现了九个不同的狼繁殖群(加利西亚,n = 4;萨莫拉,n = 4;巴利亚多利德,n = 1)。在繁殖地附近采集了粪便样本,用于饮食和基因分析,并登记是否有气味标记,以了解个体的社会地位。通过相机诱捕或请求官方普查研究区域内的野生猎物来确定猎物的可用性。我们发现,狼的食谱因地区和繁殖群体而异,但食谱并不因年龄、性别和社会地位而异。一般来说,伊比利亚狼主要以野生有蹄类动物(野猪、狍子和红鹿)为食,捕食数量最多的猎物,但加利西亚的巴尔德里斯狼群除外,该狼群似乎专门捕食狍子。家畜(绵羊、山羊、驴、猪)的消费量并不高,但在野生猎物较少的农业和畜牧业地区(萨莫拉的费雷拉斯和巴利亚多利德)尤其如此。我们的研究揭示了不同地理位置和繁殖群体的狼的饮食差异,强调了它们的适应能力。我们发现,年龄、性别和社会地位对它们的饮食偏好没有显著影响。除了加利西亚的一个特殊群体外,伊比利亚狼主要以野生蹄类动物为目标。我们的研究引入了一种新的方法,因为这是首次根据个体因素和氮分析来分析狼的饮食。此外,尽管伊比利亚狼亚种作为顶级掠食者在生态系统中扮演着重要角色,但这项研究强调了以前被忽视的伊比利亚狼亚种的饮食模式。此外,我们还强调了保护野生羚羊种群的必要性,通过确保稳定的野生猎物供应来减少对牲畜的攻击,从而促进人类与狼的共存。
{"title":"Iberian wolf’s diet and its quality during breeding season: exploring the influence of zone, wolf groups, prey availability and individual factors","authors":"Isabel Barja, Ana Piñeiro, Javier Talegón, Aritz Ruiz-González, Álvaro Navarro-Castilla, Amaia Caro, Toni Gago-Barja, M. Carmen Hernández","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03457-4","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Large predators are essential in maintaining ecosystem functioning, and comprehending how their feeding habits change across natural and human-dominated landscapes is crucial to preserve biodiversity. In this study, the diet of Iberian wolves (<i>Canis lupus signatus</i>) during pup rearing season (July to September) has been studied in relation to prey abundance and putting emphasis in the analysis of the differences between zones, wolf groups and individual factors (age, sex and social status). For this, non-invasive monitoring was carried out in three zones of Spain where nine different wolf breeding groups were detected (Galicia, <i>n</i> = 4; Zamora, <i>n</i> = 4 and Valladolid, <i>n</i> = 1). Faecal samples were collected near breeding sites for dietary and genetic analyses, registering if it was or not a scent mark to know the social status of the individuals. Prey availability was determined by camera trapping or requesting the official census of wild prey in the study areas. We found differences in wolf’s diet depending on the zone and the breeding group however, the diet did not vary depending on the age, sex and social status. In general, Iberian wolves mainly fed on wild ungulates (wild boar, roe deer and red deer), feeding on the most abundant prey, except for Baldriz group in Galicia which seems to be specialized in hunting roe deer. Domestic animals’ consumption (sheep, goat, donkey, pig) was not high, but it occurred specially in agriculture and livestock areas (Ferreras in Zamora and Valladolid) where wild prey were less available.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>In this study, we shed light on the vital role of Iberian wolves in the ecosystem by investigating the dietary preferences during pup rearing season across different landscapes. Our research revealed differences in wolf diets based on geographic location and breeding groups, emphasizing their adaptability. We found that age, sex, and social status did not significantly influence their dietary preferences. Iberian wolves predominantly target wild ungulates, except for a specialized group in Galicia. Our study introduces a novel approach as it is the first to analyse wolf diet based on individual factors and using nitrogen analysis. Additionally, the study highlights previously overlooked dietary patterns of the Iberian wolf subspecies, despite its critical role in the ecosystem as a top predator. Furthermore, we emphasize the necessity of conserving wild ungulate populations to reduce livestock attacks by ensuring a stable supply of wild prey, promoting coexistence between humans and wolves.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"183 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140199702","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between preference and switching in flower foraging by bees 蜜蜂觅花过程中的偏好与转换之间的关系
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5
Daniel R. Papaj, Avery L. Russell

It seems self-evident that generalist foragers switch more between resources than specialists but despite diverse ecological and evolutionary implications, how variation in switching relates to variation in preference warrants additional study. Here we tested predictions based on a simple probability model, using flower-foraging bees as a model system. In laboratory assays, we presented bumble bee (Bombus impatiens) workers with flowers of two species, Tecoma stans and T. alata, from which they could collect nectar and/or pollen. We quantified landing preference and occurrence of switching between species in successive visits. Bees varied greatly in floral preference. Almost half showed statistically significant preferences for one or the other species, while the rest were generalists in preference. As expected, generalists using both flower species switched more in successive visits than bees that were more specialized, a pattern fit to a quadratic function. However, generalist individuals switched more than expected based on null expectation. A Modified Jacob’s Index (MJI) of switching was significantly positively correlated with degree of preference: generalist bees had more negative MJI’s than specialist bees, indicating that even after the expected statistical effect of preference on switching was accounted for, they switched more than specialists. A simulation ruled out the possibility that the pattern was due to bias in MJI. Generalist-specialist differences in which food was collected (nectar versus pollen) were also ruled out. We offer possible explanations for our observed pattern and advocate consideration of preference and switching throughout behavioral ecology.

通才觅食者比专才觅食者在不同资源之间的转换更多,这似乎是不言而喻的,但尽管存在多种生态和进化影响,转换的变异与偏好的变异之间的关系如何还需要进一步研究。在这里,我们以觅食花卉的蜜蜂为模型系统,检验了基于简单概率模型的预测。在实验室试验中,我们向熊蜂(Bombus impatiens)工蜂展示了两种花(Tecoma stans 和 T. alata),它们可以从中采集花蜜和/或花粉。我们对蜜蜂的着陆偏好以及在连续访问中切换花种的情况进行了量化。蜜蜂对花的偏好差异很大。据统计,近一半的蜜蜂表现出对一种或另一种物种的偏好,而其余的蜜蜂则是偏好通才。不出所料,使用两种花卉的通才比专一性更强的蜜蜂在连续访问中转换的次数更多,这种模式符合二次函数。然而,基于空期望,通才个体的转换比预期的要多。切换的修正雅各布指数(MJI)与偏好程度呈显著的正相关:通才蜂比专才蜂有更多的负MJI,这表明即使考虑了偏好对切换的预期统计影响,通才蜂的切换也比专才蜂多。模拟排除了这种模式是由于 MJI 偏差造成的可能性。此外,还排除了普通蜜蜂与特种蜜蜂在采集食物(花蜜还是花粉)上的差异。我们为观察到的模式提供了可能的解释,并提倡在整个行为生态学中考虑偏好和转换。
{"title":"The relationship between preference and switching in flower foraging by bees","authors":"Daniel R. Papaj, Avery L. Russell","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03456-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It seems self-evident that generalist foragers switch more between resources than specialists but despite diverse ecological and evolutionary implications, how variation in switching relates to variation in preference warrants additional study. Here we tested predictions based on a simple probability model, using flower-foraging bees as a model system. In laboratory assays, we presented bumble bee (<i>Bombus impatiens</i>) workers with flowers of two species, <i>Tecoma stans</i> and <i>T. alata</i>, from which they could collect nectar and/or pollen. We quantified landing preference and occurrence of switching between species in successive visits. Bees varied greatly in floral preference. Almost half showed statistically significant preferences for one or the other species, while the rest were generalists in preference. As expected, generalists using both flower species switched more in successive visits than bees that were more specialized, a pattern fit to a quadratic function. However, generalist individuals switched more than expected based on null expectation. A Modified Jacob’s Index (MJI) of switching was significantly positively correlated with degree of preference: generalist bees had more negative MJI’s than specialist bees, indicating that even after the expected statistical effect of preference on switching was accounted for, they switched more than specialists. A simulation ruled out the possibility that the pattern was due to bias in MJI. Generalist-specialist differences in which food was collected (nectar versus pollen) were also ruled out. We offer possible explanations for our observed pattern and advocate consideration of preference and switching throughout behavioral ecology.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"18 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140166994","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mechanisms for polyandry evolution in a complex social bee 复杂社会性蜜蜂的多雄性进化机制
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-09 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x
Keith Delaplane, Katherine Hagan, Kevin Vogel, Lewis Bartlett

Abstract

Polyandry in social Hymenoptera is associated with reduced within-colony relatedness and increased colony-level ecologic fitness. One explanation for this sees increasing within-nest genetic diversity as a mechanism for improving group task efficiency and colony competitiveness. A queen on her mating flight captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s allele potential by her tenth effective mating (me ~ 10 males). Under this population allele capture (PAC) model, colony fitness gains track mating number in an asymptotic manner, leveling out after about the tenth mating. A supporting theory we call the genotype composition (GC) model sees genetic novelty at mating levels higher than the me ~ 10 asymptote, the hyperpolyandry zone, resulting from unique genotype compositions whose number are potentially infinite. Colony fitness gains under the GC model will track mating number in a linear manner. We set up field colonies with Apis mellifera queens each instrumentally mated with 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or 32 males, creating a polyandry gradient bracketing the qualitative divide of me ~ 10, measured tokens of colony level fitness, and collected observation hive data. Our results lead us to conclude that (1) ancestral colony traits fundamental to eusociality (cooperative brood care) respond to mating level changes at or below me ~ 10 in a manner consistent with the PAC model, whereas (2) more derived specialized colony phenotypes (resistance to the non-native parasite Varroa destructor) continue improving with increasing me in a manner consistent with the GC model. By either model, (3) the mechanism for increasing colony fitness is an increase in worker task specialisms and task efficiency.

Significance statement

Polyandry is a female’s practice of mating with many males, storing their sperm, and using it to produce genetically diverse offspring. In complex social bees, a queen captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s diversity potential by her tenth mating; however, queens in nature routinely mate with many more than ten males. We tested two models that, together, explain how social bee colonies ecologically benefit from queen mating numbers ranging from 2 to potential infinity. A population allele capture (PAC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at mating numbers at or below 10, and we provide evidence that it was at these polyandry levels that significant gains were made in an ancestral eusocial trait, cooperative brood care. A genotype composition (GC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at higher mating numbers, and we believe these gains are centered around more recently evolved ecologic specialisms such as parasite resistance.

摘要社会性膜翅目昆虫的多雄性与群内亲缘关系的降低和群级生态适应性的提高有关。对此的一种解释是,增加巢内遗传多样性是提高群体任务效率和群落竞争力的一种机制。一只蜂后在其第十次有效交配(我 ~ 10 只雄蜂)之前,会捕获其繁殖种群近 90% 的等位基因潜力。在这一群体等位基因捕获(PAC)模型下,蜂群适应性的提高以渐进的方式跟踪交配次数,大约在第十次交配后趋于平稳。我们称之为基因型组成(GC)模型的支持理论认为,在交配水平高于 me ~ 10 的渐近线(即超多雄区)时,遗传新颖性来自于独特的基因型组成,其数量可能是无限的。在 GC 模型下,蜂群的适应性收益将以线性方式跟踪交配数量。我们在野外建立了蜂群,每只蜂王分别与 1、2、4、8、16 或 32 只雄蜂进行工具交配,形成了 "我"~"10 "定性分界线范围内的多雄性梯度,测量了蜂群水平的适应性标记,并收集了观察蜂巢的数据。我们的研究结果使我们得出以下结论:(1)蜂群的祖先性状(合作育雏)对 me ~ 10 或更低的交配水平变化的反应与 PAC 模式一致,而(2)更多衍生的专门化蜂群表型(对非本地寄生虫破坏者 Varroa 的抵抗力)随着 me 的增加而继续改善,其方式与 GC 模式一致。无论采用哪种模式,(3) 提高蜂群适合度的机制都是提高工蜂的任务专业性和任务效率。重要意义声明多配偶制是雌蜂与许多雄蜂交配,储存雄蜂的精子,并利用这些精子培育出基因多样化的后代。在复杂的社会性蜜蜂中,蜂王在第十次交配时可获得其繁殖种群近 90% 的多样性潜力;然而,自然界中的蜂王通常会与十多只雄蜂交配。我们对两个模型进行了测试,这两个模型共同解释了社会蜂群如何从蜂王交配数量(从 2 到潜在的无穷大)中获得生态效益。种群等位基因捕获(PAC)模型侧重于交配数为 10 或低于 10 时蜂群的适应性收益,我们提供的证据表明,正是在这些多育水平上,祖先的社会性特征--合作育雏--获得了显著收益。基因型组成(GC)模型的重点是交配数较高时的蜂群适应性收益,我们认为这些收益主要集中在最近进化出的生态特异性上,如寄生虫抵抗力。
{"title":"Mechanisms for polyandry evolution in a complex social bee","authors":"Keith Delaplane, Katherine Hagan, Kevin Vogel, Lewis Bartlett","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03450-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Polyandry in social Hymenoptera is associated with reduced within-colony relatedness and increased colony-level ecologic fitness. One explanation for this sees increasing within-nest genetic diversity as a mechanism for improving group task efficiency and colony competitiveness. A queen on her mating flight captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s allele potential by her tenth effective mating (<i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 males). Under this population allele capture (PAC) model, colony fitness gains track mating number in an asymptotic manner, leveling out after about the tenth mating. A supporting theory we call the genotype composition (GC) model sees genetic novelty at mating levels higher than the <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 asymptote, the hyperpolyandry zone, resulting from unique genotype compositions whose number are potentially infinite. Colony fitness gains under the GC model will track mating number in a linear manner. We set up field colonies with <i>Apis mellifera</i> queens each instrumentally mated with 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, or 32 males, creating a polyandry gradient bracketing the qualitative divide of <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10, measured tokens of colony level fitness, and collected observation hive data. Our results lead us to conclude that (1) ancestral colony traits fundamental to eusociality (cooperative brood care) respond to mating level changes at or below <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> ~ 10 in a manner consistent with the PAC model, whereas (2) more derived specialized colony phenotypes (resistance to the non-native parasite <i>Varroa destructor</i>) continue improving with increasing <i>m</i><sub><i>e</i></sub> in a manner consistent with the GC model. By either model, (3) the mechanism for increasing colony fitness is an increase in worker task specialisms and task efficiency.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Polyandry is a female’s practice of mating with many males, storing their sperm, and using it to produce genetically diverse offspring. In complex social bees, a queen captures nearly 90% of her breeding population’s diversity potential by her tenth mating; however, queens in nature routinely mate with many more than ten males. We tested two models that, together, explain how social bee colonies ecologically benefit from queen mating numbers ranging from 2 to potential infinity. A population allele capture (PAC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at mating numbers at or below 10, and we provide evidence that it was at these polyandry levels that significant gains were made in an ancestral eusocial trait, cooperative brood care. A genotype composition (GC) model focuses on colony fitness gains at higher mating numbers, and we believe these gains are centered around more recently evolved ecologic specialisms such as parasite resistance.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"40 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140075460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance and efficiency in leaf transport: unveiling the task allocation puzzle in Acromyrmex subterraneus 叶片运输的性能和效率:揭开亚特兰毛蟹任务分配之谜
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7
Antônio Marcos Oliveira Toledo, Arthur Zimerer, Juliane Floriano Santos Lopes

The performance of ant workers in a given task can be highly variable, generating a non-homogenous workload in the colony. However, there is no information on whether high performance workers are more efficient, or have any morphometric and physiological variation that causes them to both start earlier and continue executing the task longer. Here, we demonstrate that non-homogeneous task distribution is common in small colonies and that different performance levels occur even among Acromyrmex subterraneus workers of the same size class. High Activity workers did more work and were faster, so they were more efficient than Low Activity workers. However, their efficiency was not related to their leg length, as it was for Low Activity workers. Also, the delay of the first loaded trip of High Activity workers was shorter than that of Low Activity workers, indicating an earlier response of the former to the task. Delay variation was not affected by mass-specific metabolic rate. Considering the first five trips, we found that the first trip had a longer duration than the others, and High Activity workers were faster than Low Activity workers, suggesting that the higher efficiency of High Activity workers may be related to the reinforcement mechanism, which in turn lessens their response threshold to the task. Finally, workers had similar mandible morphometry (length of the first and second tooth, number of teeth), and body mass components (water content, lean dry weight and lipid content) despite their activity category, indicating that these variables did not explain differences in performance or efficiency among workers. The hypometric mass scaling metabolic rate showed that Non-transporters had proportionally lower energy expenditure than other categories. High Activity workers showed remarkable performance, efficiency, and faster responses to foraging stimulus. We suggest that this heightened level of individual proficiency is in line with the threshold model, explaining the operation of the task allocation mechanism within the same worker size class.

蚂蚁工蚁在某项任务中的表现可能千差万别,从而在蚁群中产生不均匀的工作量。然而,目前还没有任何信息表明高绩效的工蚁是否更有效率,或者它们的形态和生理变化是否会导致它们更早地开始执行任务并持续更长时间。在这里,我们证明了在小型蚁群中非均质的任务分配是很常见的,而且即使是同一体型等级的亚特兰蛙工蚁也会出现不同的表现水平。高活跃度工蚁做的工作更多,速度更快,因此它们比低活跃度工蚁更有效率。不过,它们的效率与腿长无关,而低活动量工蚁的效率则与腿长有关。此外,高活动量工人第一次装载的延迟时间比低活动量工人短,这表明前者对任务的反应更早。延迟变化不受特定质量代谢率的影响。考虑到前五个行程,我们发现第一个行程的持续时间比其他行程长,而且高活动量工蚁比低活动量工蚁更快,这表明高活动量工蚁更高的效率可能与强化机制有关,强化机制进而降低了它们对任务的反应阈值。最后,尽管有不同的活动类别,但工蚁的下颚形态测量(第一和第二颗牙齿的长度、牙齿数量)和体质量成分(含水量、瘦干重和脂质含量)相似,这表明这些变量并不能解释工蚁之间的表现或效率差异。低计量质量缩放代谢率表明,非运输者的能量消耗比其他类别低。高活动量工蚁表现出色,效率高,对觅食刺激的反应更快。我们认为,这种个体熟练程度的提高符合阈值模型,可以解释同一工人体型类别中任务分配机制的运作。
{"title":"Performance and efficiency in leaf transport: unveiling the task allocation puzzle in Acromyrmex subterraneus","authors":"Antônio Marcos Oliveira Toledo, Arthur Zimerer, Juliane Floriano Santos Lopes","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03454-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The performance of ant workers in a given task can be highly variable, generating a non-homogenous workload in the colony. However, there is no information on whether high performance workers are more efficient, or have any morphometric and physiological variation that causes them to both start earlier and continue executing the task longer. Here, we demonstrate that non-homogeneous task distribution is common in small colonies and that different performance levels occur even among <i>Acromyrmex subterraneus</i> workers of the same size class. High Activity workers did more work and were faster, so they were more efficient than Low Activity workers. However, their efficiency was not related to their leg length, as it was for Low Activity workers. Also, the delay of the first loaded trip of High Activity workers was shorter than that of Low Activity workers, indicating an earlier response of the former to the task. Delay variation was not affected by mass-specific metabolic rate. Considering the first five trips, we found that the first trip had a longer duration than the others, and High Activity workers were faster than Low Activity workers, suggesting that the higher efficiency of High Activity workers may be related to the reinforcement mechanism, which in turn lessens their response threshold to the task. Finally, workers had similar mandible morphometry (length of the first and second tooth, number of teeth), and body mass components (water content, lean dry weight and lipid content) despite their activity category, indicating that these variables did not explain differences in performance or efficiency among workers. The hypometric mass scaling metabolic rate showed that Non-transporters had proportionally lower energy expenditure than other categories. High Activity workers showed remarkable performance, efficiency, and faster responses to foraging stimulus. We suggest that this heightened level of individual proficiency is in line with the threshold model, explaining the operation of the task allocation mechanism within the same worker size class.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140076270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
‘Beware, I am large and dangerous’ – human listeners can be deceived by dynamic manipulation of the indexical content of agonistic dog growls 当心,我又大又危险"--人类听众可以通过动态操纵狗的咆哮声中的索引内容而上当受骗
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9
Péter Pongrácz, Petra Dobos, Borbála Zsilák, Tamás Faragó, Bence Ferdinandy

Dishonest vocal signals about body size are rarely encountered, however, dogs are capable of modifying indexical information in their growls. This apparent acoustic body-size manipulation could be affected by the level of threat experienced by the dog. We tested whether this natural size manipulation actually affects how listeners assess the size of the dog, thus whether it could be considered as a successful indexical information manipulation. We requested human participants to assess dog growls, originally recorded when dogs encountered various ‘threatening strangers’ (of different sex, stature). The participants heard several sets of growl pairs, where they had to guess, which growl belonged to the ‘larger dog’. In the Control condition, dog growls originated from two different dogs in a pair; in the Test condition, growls of the same dog were presented pair by pair, always recorded in the presence of different threatening humans. Human listeners reliably picked the larger dog from two differently sized animals based on their growls alone. In the Test condition, participants thought that the dog was ‘larger’ when it was threatened by a female experimenter, and when the dog was growling at a larger sized human. We found that while growl length modulation was the main factor behind size-choice decisions in the case of female strangers, formant dispersion difference contributed the most when listeners chose which dog was the larger in the case of male opponents. Our results provide firsthand evidence of dogs’ functionally deceptive vocalizations towards humans, a phenomenon which has not been shown before in any interspecific scenario.

关于身体大小的不诚实声音信号很少遇到,但是,狗能够在咆哮声中修改索引信息。这种明显的体型声学操纵可能会受到狗所经历的威胁程度的影响。我们测试了这种自然的体型操纵是否会影响听者如何评估狗的体型,从而是否可以将其视为一种成功的索引信息操纵。我们要求人类参与者对狗的咆哮声进行评估,这些咆哮声最初是在狗遇到各种 "威胁性陌生人"(性别、身材各不相同)时录制的。参与者会听到几组成对的咆哮声,他们必须猜测哪种咆哮声属于 "较大的狗"。在 "对照组 "条件下,狗的咆哮声来自一对不同的狗;而在 "测试组 "条件下,同一只狗的咆哮声则是一对对地出现,而且总是在不同的威胁人类的情况下录制的。人类听者仅凭咆哮声就能从两只不同体型的动物中选出体型较大的狗。在 "测试 "条件下,当狗受到女性实验者的威胁时,以及当狗向体型较大的人类咆哮时,参与者会认为狗 "体型较大"。我们发现,在雌性陌生人的情况下,咆哮声长度调制是决定狗大小的主要因素,而在雄性对手的情况下,当听者选择哪只狗更大时,声调分散差异的作用最大。我们的研究结果提供了狗对人类进行功能性欺骗发声的第一手证据,这种现象以前从未在任何种间情景中出现过。
{"title":"‘Beware, I am large and dangerous’ – human listeners can be deceived by dynamic manipulation of the indexical content of agonistic dog growls","authors":"Péter Pongrácz, Petra Dobos, Borbála Zsilák, Tamás Faragó, Bence Ferdinandy","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03452-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dishonest vocal signals about body size are rarely encountered, however, dogs are capable of modifying indexical information in their growls. This apparent acoustic body-size manipulation could be affected by the level of threat experienced by the dog. We tested whether this natural size manipulation actually affects how listeners assess the size of the dog, thus whether it could be considered as a successful indexical information manipulation. We requested human participants to assess dog growls, originally recorded when dogs encountered various ‘threatening strangers’ (of different sex, stature). The participants heard several sets of growl pairs, where they had to guess, which growl belonged to the ‘larger dog’. In the Control condition, dog growls originated from two different dogs in a pair; in the Test condition, growls of the same dog were presented pair by pair, always recorded in the presence of different threatening humans. Human listeners reliably picked the larger dog from two differently sized animals based on their growls alone. In the Test condition, participants thought that the dog was ‘larger’ when it was threatened by a female experimenter, and when the dog was growling at a larger sized human. We found that while growl length modulation was the main factor behind size-choice decisions in the case of female strangers, formant dispersion difference contributed the most when listeners chose which dog was the larger in the case of male opponents. Our results provide firsthand evidence of dogs’ functionally deceptive vocalizations towards humans, a phenomenon which has not been shown before in any interspecific scenario.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"79 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Competitive asymmetries, birthing asynchrony and sibling rivalry in a social lizard 社会蜥蜴的竞争不对称、分娩不同步和同胞竞争
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x
Alix Bouffet-Halle, Erik Wapstra, Geoffrey M. While

Abstract

Family life often involves interactions between individuals who have different fitness goals, leading to conflict. Resolution of this conflict is key for the stabilisation of family life. Here, we used a lizard, Liopholis whitii, that lives in facultative family groups to characterise the nature and extent of sibling conflict and test the role that individual and litter characteristics play in shaping conflict between family members. We found significant variation in conflict between family groups, specifically in relation to siblings. In approximately half of the litters, siblings were aggressive towards one another, while in the other half of litters, there was no aggression observed between siblings. There were no differences between aggressive and non-aggressive litters in the key factors predicted to mediate conflict, including sex, offspring size, or litter size. However, in aggressive litters, the maximum amount of within-litter conflict decreased with an increase in the spread between births of siblings. First-born offspring were significantly more likely to be aggressive towards their siblings compared to second and third born offspring. While one offspring was usually the target of that aggression, we found no evidence that any individual-level factor predicted who received aggression. In aggressive litters, aggressive offspring spent a greater amount of time with their mother compared to non-aggressive offspring. Similar asymmetries in the amount of time offspring spent with their mother between siblings were also observed in non-aggressive litters. Combined, our results suggest that birth order is the main driver of sibling conflict in aggressive litters in this facultatively social lizard species, suggesting that birthing asynchrony may provide females with a mechanism to manage conflict.

Significance statement

Conflict is a ubiquitous aspect of family life; it occurs between adults, between parents and offspring as well as between siblings. We show that the extent of conflict between siblings varies considerably within and between families in a lizard that exhibits prolonged associations between parents and offspring. We found no effects of offspring sex or size on within or between litter conflict. Instead, the number of days that passed between the birth of offspring appears to be the main factor that influences how much conflict there is between siblings. Furthermore, birth order was the main factor predicting which offspring was dominant. Combined this suggests that birthing asynchrony, the ability of females to spread out births across multiple days, may play a crucial role in the management of conflict in this system. As the moderation of conflict is crucial for the stabilisation of family life, these results provide important insights into the early evolution of social life.

摘要家庭生活往往涉及具有不同健康目标的个人之间的互动,从而导致冲突。解决这种冲突是稳定家庭生活的关键。在这里,我们利用一种生活在面生家庭群体中的蜥蜴--Liopholis whitii--来描述兄弟姐妹冲突的性质和程度,并检验个体和窝的特征在形成家庭成员间冲突中所起的作用。我们发现,家庭群体之间的冲突差异很大,尤其是兄弟姐妹之间的冲突。在大约一半的幼仔中,兄弟姐妹之间具有攻击性,而在另一半幼仔中,兄弟姐妹之间没有攻击性。攻击性幼仔和非攻击性幼仔之间在性别、后代大小或幼仔大小等预计会调解冲突的关键因素上没有差异。然而,在攻击性窝中,窝内冲突的最大值随着兄弟姐妹出生间隔的增加而减少。与第二胎和第三胎的后代相比,第一胎的后代更有可能对其兄弟姐妹采取攻击性行为。虽然通常有一个后代是攻击的目标,但我们没有发现任何个体水平的因素能预测谁会受到攻击。在具有攻击性的幼仔中,与不具有攻击性的幼仔相比,具有攻击性的幼仔与母亲在一起的时间更长。在非攻击性幼仔中,也观察到同胞之间后代与母亲相处时间的类似不对称性。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,出生顺序是这一表面社会性蜥蜴物种攻击性幼仔中兄弟姐妹冲突的主要驱动因素,这表明分娩不同步可能为雌性提供了一种管理冲突的机制。我们的研究表明,在一种父母与后代之间有长期联系的蜥蜴中,兄弟姐妹之间的冲突程度在家庭内部和家庭之间有很大的不同。我们发现,后代的性别或大小对同窝或同窝之间的冲突没有影响。相反,后代出生间隔天数似乎是影响兄弟姐妹间冲突程度的主要因素。此外,出生顺序也是预测哪个后代占优势的主要因素。综合这些因素,我们可以看出,在这个系统中,分娩不同步,即雌性在多天内分散分娩的能力,可能在冲突管理中起着至关重要的作用。由于冲突的缓和对家庭生活的稳定至关重要,这些结果为社会生活的早期演化提供了重要的启示。
{"title":"Competitive asymmetries, birthing asynchrony and sibling rivalry in a social lizard","authors":"Alix Bouffet-Halle, Erik Wapstra, Geoffrey M. While","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03442-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Family life often involves interactions between individuals who have different fitness goals, leading to conflict. Resolution of this conflict is key for the stabilisation of family life. Here, we used a lizard, <i>Liopholis whitii</i>, that lives in facultative family groups to characterise the nature and extent of sibling conflict and test the role that individual and litter characteristics play in shaping conflict between family members. We found significant variation in conflict between family groups, specifically in relation to siblings. In approximately half of the litters, siblings were aggressive towards one another, while in the other half of litters, there was no aggression observed between siblings. There were no differences between aggressive and non-aggressive litters in the key factors predicted to mediate conflict, including sex, offspring size, or litter size. However, in aggressive litters, the maximum amount of within-litter conflict decreased with an increase in the spread between births of siblings. First-born offspring were significantly more likely to be aggressive towards their siblings compared to second and third born offspring. While one offspring was usually the target of that aggression, we found no evidence that any individual-level factor predicted who received aggression. In aggressive litters, aggressive offspring spent a greater amount of time with their mother compared to non-aggressive offspring. Similar asymmetries in the amount of time offspring spent with their mother between siblings were also observed in non-aggressive litters. Combined, our results suggest that birth order is the main driver of sibling conflict in aggressive litters in this facultatively social lizard species, suggesting that birthing asynchrony may provide females with a mechanism to manage conflict.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Conflict is a ubiquitous aspect of family life; it occurs between adults, between parents and offspring as well as between siblings. We show that the extent of conflict between siblings varies considerably within and between families in a lizard that exhibits prolonged associations between parents and offspring. We found no effects of offspring sex or size on within or between litter conflict. Instead, the number of days that passed between the birth of offspring appears to be the main factor that influences how much conflict there is between siblings. Furthermore, birth order was the main factor predicting which offspring was dominant. Combined this suggests that birthing asynchrony, the ability of females to spread out births across multiple days, may play a crucial role in the management of conflict in this system. As the moderation of conflict is crucial for the stabilisation of family life, these results provide important insights into the early evolution of social life.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing a bigger opponent: the path to victory and greater gains for the small contestant 选择更大的对手:小选手的胜利之路和更大收益
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6
Luis M. Burciaga, Guillermina Alcaraz

Abstract

Animals often engage in contests over limited resources. The probability of winning a contest is primarily determined by the individual's fighting ability relative to its opponent and the perceived value of the disputed resource. Individuals of the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis frequently fight over gastropod shells. We conducted a free-choice experiment to assess the factors that determine individuals’ choice of opponent, contest initiation, and contest resolution. We placed hermit crabs occupying two shell species into a large container and monitored agonistic interactions between crabs. We assessed the asymmetries in fighting ability based on the differences in body mass between the opponents. The shell species and fit (i.e., shell size relative to crab body size) were used as measures of the objective and subjective resource value, respectively. Motivation influenced contest initiation; the crabs occupying too-tight shells were more likely to initiate a contest than the ones in looser-fitting shells. In most cases, the attackers fought for a shell with a looser fit, even if that meant losing a tighter-fitting shell of the preferred shell species. The fighting success for attackers was positively associated with the number of bouts of rapping and the shell size improvement. However, success was negatively correlated with body size asymmetry; attackers that chose opponents larger than themselves were more likely to evict their opponent than the attackers that chose opponents that were smaller. Experimental designs that allow animals to select their own opponents, rather than assigning specific opponents, can strongly contribute to knowledge of agonistic interactions.

Significance statement

Fighting dynamics are commonly assessed by estimating the influence of the resource-holding potential and resource value in pair-matched opponents. Here, however, we examined the influence of asymmetries in resource-holding potential and the objective and subjective value of gastropod shells on the choice of a contender and contest resolution through a free-choice opponent experiment in hermit crabs. The contest initiation was driven by the motivation to obtain a better gastropod shell species or one with a better size fit—factors which are known to improve individual fitness. Furthermore, fighting success was associated with an individual's persistence in displaying aggressive behaviors and its motivation to obtain a better resource. However, contrary to the predictions of game theoretical models, fighting success was higher when attackers chose opponents larger than themselves. Our results highlight the relevance of assessing fighting under more natural conditions by allowing animals to select their opponents.

摘要 动物经常会为了争夺有限的资源而展开较量。争夺获胜的概率主要取决于个体相对于对手的战斗力以及对争议资源的认知价值。加州寄居蟹(Calcinus californiensis)的个体经常为争夺腹足类动物的壳而打斗。我们进行了一次自由选择实验,以评估决定个体选择对手、开始争夺和解决争夺的因素。我们将占据两种贝壳的寄居蟹放入一个大容器中,并监测寄居蟹之间的争斗互动。我们根据对手身体质量的差异评估了战斗能力的不对称性。壳的种类和适合度(即壳的大小相对于蟹的体型)分别被用来衡量客观和主观资源价值。螃蟹的动机会影响竞争的开始;壳太紧的螃蟹比壳太松的螃蟹更有可能开始竞争。在大多数情况下,攻击者会争夺一个更宽松的壳,即使这意味着要失去一个更紧身的壳。攻击者的战斗成功率与拍击次数和贝壳大小的改善呈正相关。然而,成功与否与体型不对称呈负相关;选择比自己大的对手的攻击者比选择比自己小的对手的攻击者更有可能驱逐对手。让动物自己选择对手,而不是指定特定对手的实验设计,可以极大地促进对激动式相互作用的了解。意义声明通常通过估计成对匹配对手的资源占有潜力和资源价值的影响来评估战斗动态。然而,在这里,我们通过寄居蟹自由选择对手实验,考察了资源占有潜力和腹足类贝壳的主客观价值的不对称性对竞争者选择和竞争解决的影响。开始竞争的动机是为了获得更好的腹足类贝壳种类或更好的大小适合度--众所周知,这些因素可以提高个体的适应性。此外,搏斗的成功与个体持续表现出的攻击行为及其获得更好资源的动机有关。然而,与博弈论模型的预测相反,当攻击者选择比自己大的对手时,战斗成功率更高。我们的研究结果凸显了在更自然的条件下通过让动物选择对手来评估打斗的意义。
{"title":"Choosing a bigger opponent: the path to victory and greater gains for the small contestant","authors":"Luis M. Burciaga, Guillermina Alcaraz","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03447-6","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>Animals often engage in contests over limited resources. The probability of winning a contest is primarily determined by the individual's fighting ability relative to its opponent and the perceived value of the disputed resource. Individuals of the hermit crab <i>Calcinus californiensis</i> frequently fight over gastropod shells. We conducted a free-choice experiment to assess the factors that determine individuals’ choice of opponent, contest initiation, and contest resolution. We placed hermit crabs occupying two shell species into a large container and monitored agonistic interactions between crabs. We assessed the asymmetries in fighting ability based on the differences in body mass between the opponents. The shell species and fit (i.e., shell size relative to crab body size) were used as measures of the objective and subjective resource value, respectively. Motivation influenced contest initiation; the crabs occupying too-tight shells were more likely to initiate a contest than the ones in looser-fitting shells. In most cases, the attackers fought for a shell with a looser fit, even if that meant losing a tighter-fitting shell of the preferred shell species. The fighting success for attackers was positively associated with the number of bouts of rapping and the shell size improvement. However, success was negatively correlated with body size asymmetry; attackers that chose opponents larger than themselves were more likely to evict their opponent than the attackers that chose opponents that were smaller. Experimental designs that allow animals to select their own opponents, rather than assigning specific opponents, can strongly contribute to knowledge of agonistic interactions.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Fighting dynamics are commonly assessed by estimating the influence of the resource-holding potential and resource value in pair-matched opponents. Here, however, we examined the influence of asymmetries in resource-holding potential and the objective and subjective value of gastropod shells on the choice of a contender and contest resolution through a free-choice opponent experiment in hermit crabs. The contest initiation was driven by the motivation to obtain a better gastropod shell species or one with a better size fit—factors which are known to improve individual fitness. Furthermore, fighting success was associated with an individual's persistence in displaying aggressive behaviors and its motivation to obtain a better resource. However, contrary to the predictions of game theoretical models, fighting success was higher when attackers chose opponents larger than themselves. Our results highlight the relevance of assessing fighting under more natural conditions by allowing animals to select their opponents.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140005796","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Experimental quantification of genetic and ontogenetic effects on fighting behavior in the broad-horned flour beetle 通过实验量化宽角面粉甲虫的遗传和个体发育对打斗行为的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-24 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w
Toshiki Nishitani, Kentarou Matsumura, Erik Postma, Manmohan Dev Sharma, David J Hosken, Takahisa Miyatake

Most animal behaviors show large within- and among-individual variation, and this includes competitive male behaviors. With male fighting for example, aggressiveness often correlates with dominance, and contest duration varies with age. However, few studies have directly quantified how mean aggressiveness and contest duration, the variation among individuals in both traits, and the relationship among them, vary with age. Here we address these gaps and examine the effect of male age and genotype on two key aspects of male fighting behavior - aggressiveness (here measured as latency to fight) and contest duration - and the relationship between them. We do this using isogenic lines of the broad-horned flour beetle Gnatocerus cornutus. We observed fighting behavior of paired males of similar body size and age. Using uni- and multivariate mixed models, we show that although there was a significant difference between younger and older males in contest duration, mean aggressiveness was not affected by male age. However, the variation in aggression and fight duration varied with age, being greater in younger and older males respectively. Additionally, although there was a positive correlation between aggressiveness and contest duration in younger males, this relationship was not found in older males. Finally, the only significant genetic effect was for aggression in younger males. Our study shows that age differentially shapes key components of male fighting behavior as well as the relationship among them, highlighting the dynamic nature and context-dependence of fighting.

大多数动物行为在个体内部和个体之间都有很大差异,雄性动物的竞争行为也不例外。以雄性争斗为例,攻击性通常与优势相关,争斗持续时间随年龄而变化。然而,很少有研究直接量化平均攻击性和争斗持续时间、个体间这两个特征的变化以及它们之间的关系是如何随年龄变化的。在这里,我们针对这些空白,研究了雄性年龄和基因型对雄性格斗行为的两个关键方面--攻击性(这里用格斗潜伏期来衡量)和较量持续时间--的影响,以及它们之间的关系。我们利用宽角面粉甲虫(Gnatocerus cornutus)的同源系来进行研究。我们观察了体型和年龄相似的配对雄性的打斗行为。通过使用单变量和多变量混合模型,我们发现虽然年轻雄性和年长雄性在较量持续时间上存在显著差异,但平均攻击性并不受雄性年龄的影响。然而,攻击性和搏斗持续时间随年龄的变化而变化,年轻雄性和年长雄性的攻击性和搏斗持续时间分别更大。此外,虽然年轻雄性的攻击性与比赛持续时间呈正相关,但在年长雄性中却没有发现这种关系。最后,唯一显著的遗传效应是年轻雄性的攻击性。我们的研究表明,年龄对雄性格斗行为的关键因素以及它们之间的关系有着不同的影响,这突出了格斗的动态性质和环境依赖性。
{"title":"Experimental quantification of genetic and ontogenetic effects on fighting behavior in the broad-horned flour beetle","authors":"Toshiki Nishitani, Kentarou Matsumura, Erik Postma, Manmohan Dev Sharma, David J Hosken, Takahisa Miyatake","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03451-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Most animal behaviors show large within- and among-individual variation, and this includes competitive male behaviors. With male fighting for example, aggressiveness often correlates with dominance, and contest duration varies with age. However, few studies have directly quantified how mean aggressiveness and contest duration, the variation among individuals in both traits, and the relationship among them, vary with age. Here we address these gaps and examine the effect of male age and genotype on two key aspects of male fighting behavior - aggressiveness (here measured as latency to fight) and contest duration - and the relationship between them. We do this using isogenic lines of the broad-horned flour beetle <i>Gnatocerus cornutus</i>. We observed fighting behavior of paired males of similar body size and age. Using uni- and multivariate mixed models, we show that although there was a significant difference between younger and older males in contest duration, mean aggressiveness was not affected by male age. However, the variation in aggression and fight duration varied with age, being greater in younger and older males respectively. Additionally, although there was a positive correlation between aggressiveness and contest duration in younger males, this relationship was not found in older males. Finally, the only significant genetic effect was for aggression in younger males. Our study shows that age differentially shapes key components of male fighting behavior as well as the relationship among them, highlighting the dynamic nature and context-dependence of fighting.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"61 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No sign of reproductive cessation in the old workers of a queenless ponerine ant 无王椿树蚁的老工蚁没有生殖停止的迹象
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8
Kodai Kishino, Keiko Sakiyama, Haruna Fujioka, Yasukazu Okada

Abstract

In many social hymenopterans, workers of different ages engage in different tasks; younger workers remain inside the nest as intranidal workers, while older workers go outside the nest as extranidal workers (i.e., age polyethism). Previous studies have shown that ovarian activity is diminished in old, extranidal workers, but it remains unclear whether workers’ reproductive ability persists for life or whether they exhibit post-reproductive lifespans. In this study, we investigated the age-dependence of worker reproductive ability in a monomorphic ponerine ant Diacamma cf. indicum. In Diacamma ants, all females in a colony have reproductive ability, but effective reproduction is limited to one or a few dominant females, and the remaining females act as sterile helpers. Using long-term laboratory rearing, we investigated whether worker reproductive ability lasts throughout a worker’s lifetime. The ages of workers were accurately tracked, and the reproductive ability of young and old workers was examined by creating several gamergate-less sub-colonies. Results showed that at least one individual in each sub-colony developed ovaries, even in the sub-colonies that solely consisted of very old workers (> 252 days old). Interestingly, in the presence of younger workers, old workers rarely showed ovarian development. Besides age, we found a positive correlation between the amount of fat (i.e., nutritional condition) and ovarian development in old workers. Our data suggest that reproductive activity of old workers is low but maintained throughout their life in Diacamma.

Significance statement

Females of social animals, such as cetaceans, are known to stop reproducing before the end of their lifespan. It is suggested that the reproductive ability of ant queens does not decline and is maintained throughout their lives; however, it is unclear whether this is also true in ant workers. We maintained ant colonies for more than 500 days and then tested whether the reproductive ability of ant workers is maintained throughout their lifespan. Even in small groups composed of only very old workers (> 252 days old), at least one individual in each group always reproduced actively. Interestingly, the presence of young workers seemed to suppress the reproduction of old workers. In addition, fat content was positively associated with the individual’s reproductive potential.

摘要 在许多社会性膜翅目昆虫中,不同年龄的工蜂从事不同的任务;年轻的工蜂留在巢内,成为巢内工蜂,而年长的工蜂则走出巢外,成为巢外工蜂(即年龄多态性)。以前的研究表明,年老的潮间带外工蚁的卵巢活动会减弱,但工蚁的生殖能力是否终生存在,或者它们是否表现出生殖后寿命,目前仍不清楚。在这项研究中,我们调查了单形绒毛蚁 Diacamma cf. indicum 工蚁生殖能力的年龄依赖性。在Diacamma蚂蚁中,蚁群中的所有雌性都具有繁殖能力,但有效繁殖仅限于一只或几只优势雌性,其余雌性则充当不育帮手。通过长期实验室饲养,我们研究了工蚁的生殖能力是否贯穿工蚁的一生。我们对工蜂的年龄进行了精确的追踪,并通过建立几个无配子门的子群来考察年轻和年老工蜂的繁殖能力。结果表明,每个子群中至少有一个个体发育出卵巢,即使是在仅由非常老的工蚁(252天)组成的子群中也是如此。有趣的是,在有年轻工蜂存在的情况下,老工蜂很少出现卵巢发育。除年龄外,我们还发现老龄工蚁的脂肪量(即营养状况)与卵巢发育呈正相关。我们的数据表明,在 Diacamma 中,老龄工蚁的生殖活动较低,但终其一生都能保持生殖活动。意义声明众所周知,鲸目动物等社会性动物的雌性会在寿命结束前停止生殖。有研究表明,蚁后的繁殖能力不会衰退,而且会终生保持;但目前还不清楚蚂蚁工蚁是否也是如此。我们将蚂蚁群落维持了 500 多天,然后测试了蚂蚁工蚁的生殖能力是否在其整个生命周期中都能保持。即使是由非常年老的工蚁(252 天)组成的小群体,每个群体中至少有一个个体总是在积极繁殖。有趣的是,年轻工蚁的存在似乎抑制了老工蚁的繁殖。此外,脂肪含量与个体的繁殖潜力呈正相关。
{"title":"No sign of reproductive cessation in the old workers of a queenless ponerine ant","authors":"Kodai Kishino, Keiko Sakiyama, Haruna Fujioka, Yasukazu Okada","doi":"10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-024-03445-8","url":null,"abstract":"<h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Abstract</h3><p>In many social hymenopterans, workers of different ages engage in different tasks; younger workers remain inside the nest as intranidal workers, while older workers go outside the nest as extranidal workers (i.e., age polyethism). Previous studies have shown that ovarian activity is diminished in old, extranidal workers, but it remains unclear whether workers’ reproductive ability persists for life or whether they exhibit post-reproductive lifespans. In this study, we investigated the age-dependence of worker reproductive ability in a monomorphic ponerine ant <i>Diacamma</i> cf. <i>indicum</i>. In <i>Diacamma</i> ants, all females in a colony have reproductive ability, but effective reproduction is limited to one or a few dominant females, and the remaining females act as sterile helpers. Using long-term laboratory rearing, we investigated whether worker reproductive ability lasts throughout a worker’s lifetime. The ages of workers were accurately tracked, and the reproductive ability of young and old workers was examined by creating several gamergate-less sub-colonies. Results showed that at least one individual in each sub-colony developed ovaries, even in the sub-colonies that solely consisted of very old workers (&gt; 252 days old). Interestingly, in the presence of younger workers, old workers rarely showed ovarian development. Besides age, we found a positive correlation between the amount of fat (i.e., nutritional condition) and ovarian development in old workers. Our data suggest that reproductive activity of old workers is low but maintained throughout their life in <i>Diacamma</i>.</p><h3 data-test=\"abstract-sub-heading\">Significance statement</h3><p>Females of social animals, such as cetaceans, are known to stop reproducing before the end of their lifespan. It is suggested that the reproductive ability of ant queens does not decline and is maintained throughout their lives; however, it is unclear whether this is also true in ant workers. We maintained ant colonies for more than 500 days and then tested whether the reproductive ability of ant workers is maintained throughout their lifespan. Even in small groups composed of only very old workers (&gt; 252 days old), at least one individual in each group always reproduced actively. Interestingly, the presence of young workers seemed to suppress the reproduction of old workers. In addition, fat content was positively associated with the individual’s reproductive potential.</p>","PeriodicalId":8881,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology","volume":"37 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139954581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1