This paper describes a rich dataset from a registered report investigating sleep’s effect on false memory in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. 534 young adults completed free recall either shortly or 12 hours after studying lists of semantic associates (e.g., hospital, nurse). Collected online, our recall data showcase high data quality, replicating classic behavioural effects (e.g., serial position curve). The dataset contains raw recall data with original spelling and recall order, accompanied by demographic information (e.g., gender, time-of-day preference). Its versatility supports reuse in modelling memory decay and search processes, understanding lexical effects and individual differences, and benchmarking online memory studies.
{"title":"Data from “A Registered Report Testing the Effect of Sleep on DRM False Memory: Greater Lure and Veridical Recall but Fewer Intrusions After Sleep”","authors":"Matthew H. C. Mak","doi":"10.5334/jopd.98","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.98","url":null,"abstract":"This paper describes a rich dataset from a registered report investigating sleep’s effect on false memory in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. 534 young adults completed free recall either shortly or 12 hours after studying lists of semantic associates (e.g., hospital, nurse). Collected online, our recall data showcase high data quality, replicating classic behavioural effects (e.g., serial position curve). The dataset contains raw recall data with original spelling and recall order, accompanied by demographic information (e.g., gender, time-of-day preference). Its versatility supports reuse in modelling memory decay and search processes, understanding lexical effects and individual differences, and benchmarking online memory studies.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":"105 20","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141666121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The data presented here comes from the Perret and Solier (2022) study. 30 participants handwrote labels for 150 black-and-white drawings. The experiment was carried out using the DmDx program. Response times and production errors were the two behavioral reported measures. DmDx scripts and data are available on the OSF platform (DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/GAZF3). These data should be useful for pre-testing to explore new hypotheses, as well as for methodological elements (e.g., sample size estimation, estimation of a priori distributions for Bayesian analyses).
{"title":"Data from the Paper Entitled “Application of a Bayesian Approach for Exploring the Impact of Syllable Frequency in Handwritten Picture Naming”","authors":"C. Perret, Clara Solier","doi":"10.5334/jopd.110","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.110","url":null,"abstract":"The data presented here comes from the Perret and Solier (2022) study. 30 participants handwrote labels for 150 black-and-white drawings. The experiment was carried out using the DmDx program. Response times and production errors were the two behavioral reported measures. DmDx scripts and data are available on the OSF platform (DOI: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/GAZF3). These data should be useful for pre-testing to explore new hypotheses, as well as for methodological elements (e.g., sample size estimation, estimation of a priori distributions for Bayesian analyses).","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141011178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We validated the use of 3-D printed Shepard and Metzler-style shapes in a simultaneous cross-modal (vision & touch) mental rotation procedure. Participants compared a visually presented 3-D shape to a 3-D shape they could only feel to determine if the shapes were the same. Participant response time and error rate demonstrated the expected linear increase as the angular disparity of the 3-D printed shapes increased. We expect the freely available data and stimuli from the procedure will be useful to researchers studying both traditional mental rotation and cross-modality mental rotation with complex, highly adaptable, and easy to create shapes.
{"title":"A New Procedure and Stimulus Set for Examining Cross-Modality Mental Rotation","authors":"Joshua E. Wolf, Melissa Larsen","doi":"10.5334/jopd.99","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.99","url":null,"abstract":"We validated the use of 3-D printed Shepard and Metzler-style shapes in a simultaneous cross-modal (vision & touch) mental rotation procedure. Participants compared a visually presented 3-D shape to a 3-D shape they could only feel to determine if the shapes were the same. Participant response time and error rate demonstrated the expected linear increase as the angular disparity of the 3-D printed shapes increased. We expect the freely available data and stimuli from the procedure will be useful to researchers studying both traditional mental rotation and cross-modality mental rotation with complex, highly adaptable, and easy to create shapes.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":"61 32","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140664021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We report data from the Small World of Singlish Words (SWOSW) Project, where word associations are obtained for a list of common Singlish words. Singapore English, colloquially known as Singlish, is a dialect of English spoken in Singapore. Free association data on these words were collected from a young adult population of university undergraduates using an online survey platform. This data enables the construction of semantic networks, allowing one to examine the semantic structure of individual words in the Singlish lexicon, as well as to compare differences in semantic structure across groups of participants.
{"title":"Preliminary Data from the Small World of Singlish Words Project: Examining Responses to Common Singlish Words","authors":"Jin Jye Wong, Cynthia S. Q. Siew","doi":"10.5334/jopd.108","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.108","url":null,"abstract":"We report data from the Small World of Singlish Words (SWOSW) Project, where word associations are obtained for a list of common Singlish words. Singapore English, colloquially known as Singlish, is a dialect of English spoken in Singapore. Free association data on these words were collected from a young adult population of university undergraduates using an online survey platform. This data enables the construction of semantic networks, allowing one to examine the semantic structure of individual words in the Singlish lexicon, as well as to compare differences in semantic structure across groups of participants.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":" 22","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140687475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Misophonia is a disorder of decreased tolerance to certain “trigger” sounds (e.g., chewing, tapping, clicking). While misophonia research is scant in general, studies presenting sounds are especially rare and methodologically variable, likely due to the labor and time required to create stimuli. Thus, we introduce FOAMS: Free Open-Access Misophonia Stimuli, a sound bank publicly available on Zenodo, accompanied by pilot discomfort ratings for 32 of these sounds (4 exemplars of 8 classes). The FOAMS database aims to decrease the burden on researchers, facilitating reproducibility and the pursuit of nuanced research questions to better understand this perplexing disorder.
{"title":"Curation of FOAMS: a Free Open-Access Misophonia Stimuli Database","authors":"Dean M. Orloff, Daniel Benesch, Heather A. Hansen","doi":"10.5334/jopd.94","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.94","url":null,"abstract":"Misophonia is a disorder of decreased tolerance to certain “trigger” sounds (e.g., chewing, tapping, clicking). While misophonia research is scant in general, studies presenting sounds are especially rare and methodologically variable, likely due to the labor and time required to create stimuli. Thus, we introduce FOAMS: Free Open-Access Misophonia Stimuli, a sound bank publicly available on Zenodo, accompanied by pilot discomfort ratings for 32 of these sounds (4 exemplars of 8 classes). The FOAMS database aims to decrease the burden on researchers, facilitating reproducibility and the pursuit of nuanced research questions to better understand this perplexing disorder.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":"14 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70678993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
We created a dataset of 205 English novel noun-noun compounds (NNCs, e.g., “doctor charity”) by combining nouns with higher and lower agentivity (i.e., the probability of being an agent in a sentence). We collected active and passive interpretations of NNCs from a group of 58 English native speakers. We then measured interpretation time differences between NNCs with active and passive interpretations (i.e., 108 NNCs), using data obtained from a group of 68 English native speakers. Data were collected online using crowdsourcing platforms (SONA and Prolific). The datasets are available at osf.io/gvc2w/ and can be used to address questions about semantic and syntactic composition.
{"title":"A Dataset of 108 Novel Noun-Noun Compound Words with Active and Passive Interpretation","authors":"Phoebe Chen, D. Poeppel, Arianna Zuanazzi","doi":"10.5334/jopd.93","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.93","url":null,"abstract":"We created a dataset of 205 English novel noun-noun compounds (NNCs, e.g., “doctor charity”) by combining nouns with higher and lower agentivity (i.e., the probability of being an agent in a sentence). We collected active and passive interpretations of NNCs from a group of 58 English native speakers. We then measured interpretation time differences between NNCs with active and passive interpretations (i.e., 108 NNCs), using data obtained from a group of 68 English native speakers. Data were collected online using crowdsourcing platforms (SONA and Prolific). The datasets are available at osf.io/gvc2w/ and can be used to address questions about semantic and syntactic composition.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45195757","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The here presented data were collected to explore the relationship between people’s attitudes toward COVID-19 measures and policy strictness. We conducted online surveys in July 2020 and May 2021 with 131 respectively 130 participants from Switzerland and Germany. Participants responded on visual analogue scales to 33 respectively 25 questions. Further data on participants’ information sources, health status, and demographics were collected. The data contribute to understanding psychological and behavioural reactions to COVID-19 policies and may help to further examine the pandemic policy management. The dataset, coding, and variables can be found online on PsychArchives (https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12899). The study was preregistered on OSF (https://osf.io/uw8mh/).
{"title":"The Relation Between the Public Attitude Towards COVID-19 and its Applied Policies – a Dataset for Binational and Temporal Comparison","authors":"N. Huber, Raphael Buchmüller, Ulf-Dietrich Reips","doi":"10.5334/jopd.84","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.84","url":null,"abstract":"The here presented data were collected to explore the relationship between people’s attitudes toward COVID-19 measures and policy strictness. We conducted online surveys in July 2020 and May 2021 with 131 respectively 130 participants from Switzerland and Germany. Participants responded on visual analogue scales to 33 respectively 25 questions. Further data on participants’ information sources, health status, and demographics were collected. The data contribute to understanding psychological and behavioural reactions to COVID-19 policies and may help to further examine the pandemic policy management. The dataset, coding, and variables can be found online on PsychArchives (https://doi.org/10.23668/psycharchives.12899). The study was preregistered on OSF (https://osf.io/uw8mh/).","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43082909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
In April 2020, only a few weeks after the COVID-19 pandemic had erupted, we conducted an online survey and collected data from 2031 individuals in four European countries (Germany, the Netherlands, Spain and the United Kingdom) using a cross-sectional design. Participants recruited on Cint completed new and pre-existing measures of socio-political and populist attitudes perceived threats, appraisals (anger at the government, anger at transgressors of hygiene measures, anxiety about coronavirus via the appraisals of health-related threats), conspiracy mentality, moral reasoning, threat estimation (coronavirus, climate, symbolic material/safety), news consumption, support for and compliance with governmental hygiene measures, subjective social status and demographics. The dataset is stored on figshare repository. It can be used to study social-psychological, emotional, socio-political and socio-economic factors of the COVID-19 pandemic.
{"title":"A Dataset of Social-Psychological and Emotional Reactions During the COVID-19 Pandemic Across Four European Countries","authors":"David Abadi, Irene Arnaldo, A. Fischer","doi":"10.5334/jopd.86","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.86","url":null,"abstract":"In April 2020, only a few weeks after the COVID-19 pandemic had erupted, we conducted an online survey and collected data from 2031 individuals in four European countries (Germany, the Netherlands, Spain and the United Kingdom) using a cross-sectional design. Participants recruited on Cint completed new and pre-existing measures of socio-political and populist attitudes perceived threats, appraisals (anger at the government, anger at transgressors of hygiene measures, anxiety about coronavirus via the appraisals of health-related threats), conspiracy mentality, moral reasoning, threat estimation (coronavirus, climate, symbolic material/safety), news consumption, support for and compliance with governmental hygiene measures, subjective social status and demographics. The dataset is stored on figshare repository. It can be used to study social-psychological, emotional, socio-political and socio-economic factors of the COVID-19 pandemic.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42660813","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Klaus Buddeberg, Kristin Skowranek, Gregor Dutz, Anke Grotlüschen
The study “LEO 2018 – Living with Low Literacy” examines the reading and writing skills of adults aged between 18 and 64 years in Germany. It includes a literacy assessment and an extensive background questionnaire containing sociodemographic variables as well as information on literacy-related everyday practices and domain-specific basic skills (digital, financial, health-related, policy-related). The data was collected in 2018 as part of a household survey (N = 7,192). The dataset is available for secondary use at the repository of GESIS Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences as a Public Use File and as a Scientific Use File. The dataset offers a reuse potential for different research fields like financial literacy, health literacy, political literacy, digital literacy and with three variables about vulnerability even for psychological research questions.
{"title":"Data from LEO 2018 – Living with Low Literacy","authors":"Klaus Buddeberg, Kristin Skowranek, Gregor Dutz, Anke Grotlüschen","doi":"10.5334/jopd.91","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.91","url":null,"abstract":"The study “LEO 2018 – Living with Low Literacy” examines the reading and writing skills of adults aged between 18 and 64 years in Germany. It includes a literacy assessment and an extensive background questionnaire containing sociodemographic variables as well as information on literacy-related everyday practices and domain-specific basic skills (digital, financial, health-related, policy-related). The data was collected in 2018 as part of a household survey (N = 7,192). The dataset is available for secondary use at the repository of GESIS Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences as a Public Use File and as a Scientific Use File. The dataset offers a reuse potential for different research fields like financial literacy, health literacy, political literacy, digital literacy and with three variables about vulnerability even for psychological research questions.","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43298872","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
The Program for International Student Assessment Young Adult Follow-up Study (PISA YAFS) was conducted in the United States in 2016 with young adults (age 19) who participated in PISA 2012 when they were in high school (age 15). PISA YAFS was designed to measure the relationship between performance on PISA 2012 and subsequent outcomes (education, employment, etc.) as well as skills. These skills were assessed in an online assessment of literacy, numeracy, and problem-solving skills called Education and Skills Online (ESO). ESO was developed to provide individual-level results linked to the Program for International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC).
{"title":"Data from the Program for International Student Assessment Young Adult Follow-up Study (PISA YAFS): 2012–2016","authors":"David Kastberg, S. Mamedova, Samantha S. Burg","doi":"10.5334/jopd.82","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/jopd.82","url":null,"abstract":"The Program for International Student Assessment Young Adult Follow-up Study (PISA YAFS) was conducted in the United States in 2016 with young adults (age 19) who participated in PISA 2012 when they were in high school (age 15). PISA YAFS was designed to measure the relationship between performance on PISA 2012 and subsequent outcomes (education, employment, etc.) as well as skills. These skills were assessed in an online assessment of literacy, numeracy, and problem-solving skills called Education and Skills Online (ESO). ESO was developed to provide individual-level results linked to the Program for International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC).","PeriodicalId":91028,"journal":{"name":"Journal of open psychology data","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-06-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44724456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}